Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T18:05:03.557Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring the effects of workplace deviance on perpetrators’ own work outcomes: the role of benevolent leadership in regulating fear activation and implication

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 June 2021

Shao-Long Li
Affiliation:
Economics and Management School, Wuhan University, 299 Bayi Road, Wuhan, CN 430072, China
Zhen Xiong (George) Chen
Affiliation:
College of Business and Economics, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia
Guanglei Zhang*
Affiliation:
School of Management, Wuhan University of Technology, 122 Luoshi Road, Wuhan, CN 430070, China
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The prevalence of workplace deviance has become an urgent issue for managers. Although increasing research has investigated the detriments of workplace deviance on other employees and organizations, limited research has studied the harm of workplace deviance on perpetrators themselves. This research drew from appraisal theories of emotion and sought to understand perpetrators' affective and behavioral consequences of engaging in deviance. Using a diary method, a survey consisting of 92 employees with 918 observations was conducted. The results reveal that employees' deviance is positively related to their feeling of fear and that fear overrides feelings of guilt, ultimately decreasing work engagement and organizational citizenship behavior (OCB). Importantly, perceived benevolent leadership weakens the effects of deviance on perpetrators themselves by relieving fear associated with past deviance and mitigating the negative influences of fear on OCB.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press and Australian and New Zealand Academy of Management 2021

Introduction

The prevalence of workplace deviance – intentional acts that violate the legitimate interests of, or cause actual harm to, organizations or their stakeholders (Bennett & Robinson, Reference Bennett, Robinson and Greenberg2003) – has become an increasingly urgent ethical issue for managers (Harvey, Martinko, & Borkowski, Reference Harvey, Martinko and Borkowski2017). Ethics and Compliance Initiative (2018) revealed that ‘50% of employees reported observing misconduct in the workplace.’ And the employees' engagement in workplace deviance costs billions of dollars per year for organizations (Bennett, Marasi, & Locklear, Reference Bennett, Marasi and Locklear2019). Overall, extant empirical research and meta-analysis have revealed that workplace deviance is related to decreased business unit performance, productivity, and profit (Carpenter, Whitman, & Amrhein, Reference Carpenter, Whitman and Amrhein2021; Dunlop & Lee, Reference Dunlop and Lee2004), as well as negative consequences for other employees in the organization, such as decreased job satisfaction, organizational commitment and well-being, and increased stress, turnover intentions, and abusive supervision (Bowling & Beehr, Reference Bowling and Beehr2006).

Despite the various consequences of employees' deviant behaviors in the workplace, scholars have underestimated deviance's total cost. Indeed, past research has almost exclusively examined how an employee's deviance negatively affects their team or organizational performance. In other words, extant research has primarily employed a victim-centric perspective to understand how an employee's deviance affects his or her coworkers, teams, and organizations. The present research adopts a perpetrator-centric perspective to investigate how employees react to their deviant behaviors. Because deviance in the workplace is almost unavoidable (Jetten & Hornsey, Reference Jetten and Hornsey2014), it is imperative for practitioners and researchers to know more about the full range of consequences associated with workplace deviance.

Several scholars have taken a perpetrator-centric perspective of deviance; however, most discussions have been theoretical in nature and have linked deviance to perpetrators' own outcomes such as reduced job performance, health, and well-being (Spector & Fox, Reference Spector and Fox2002). However, among the handful of empirical studies, some have surprisingly revealed several positive consequences of deviance for the perpetrator. For example, Ilies, Peng, Savani, and Dimotakis (Reference Ilies, Peng, Savani and Dimotakis2013) proposed an emotion-based model to demonstrate that perpetrators engaged in more organizational citizenship behaviors (OCBs) to compensate for prior deviant behaviors due to heightened feelings of guilt.

The current research drew from appraisal theories of emotion (Smith & Ellsworth, Reference Smith and Ellsworth1985; Smith & Lazarus, Reference Smith, Lazarus and Pervin1990) and sought to understand the perpetrators' affective and behavioral consequences of engaging in deviance. Specifically, this research extends the literature and suggests that perpetrators experience intense feelings of fear after participating in deviant behaviors – a negatively-valenced, high-arousal emotion arising from low certainty and control (Ellsworth & Scherer, Reference Ellsworth, Scherer, Davidson, Goldsmith and Scherer2003). Unlike the feeling of guilt, which is often associated with individual reparatory behaviors (Haidt, Reference Haidt, Davidson, Scherer and Goldsmith2003), fear is generally related to behavioral withdrawal (Frijda, Kuipers, & Ter Schure, Reference Frijda, Kuipers and Ter Schure1989). Specifically, drawing from and extending past research that has revealed the effects of deviance on perpetrators' feelings of guilt (Bonner, Greenbaum, & Quade, Reference Bonner, Greenbaum and Quade2017; Ilies et al., Reference Ilies, Peng, Savani and Dimotakis2013), this study theorizes that perpetrators would also feel fearful after engaging in deviant behaviors. As a result, they would devote less to their work (Clark & Loxton, Reference Clark and Loxton2012; Schaufeli, Salanova, González-Romá, & Bakker, Reference Schaufeli, Salanova, González-Romá and Bakker2002) and perform fewer OCBs. While guilt is a reasonable emotional response to deviance (Ilies et al., Reference Ilies, Peng, Savani and Dimotakis2013), fear is a basic and universal human emotion associated with survival (Ekman, Reference Ekman1992). Therefore, its effect on the perpetrator will be much stronger than, and override, the effects of guilt.

Although the indirect relationships between deviance and perpetrators' work engagement and OCB via fear are expected to be negative in general, several boundary conditions of these relationships are likely. Notably, appraisal theories of emotion suggest that the social context is vital in shaping employees' emotional reactions and the behavioral consequences of their emotional expressions (Gross, Reference Gross2002). In organizations, leadership creates a social context that can affect the strength of fearful feelings associated with one's prior deviance by shaping the perpetrator's cognitions of environmental certainty and controllability (Smith & Lazarus, Reference Smith and Lazarus1993). Specifically, leader support can weaken the negative social influences of fear (Lebel, Reference Lebel2017). As such, this research aims to explore whether benevolent leadership, defined as ‘leader behaviors that demonstrate individualized, holistic concern for subordinates’ personal and family well-being’ (Pellegrini & Scandura, Reference Pellegrini and Scandura2008), can attenuate the effect of deviance on fear.

Benevolent leadership emphasizes that leaders care about followers' own benefit in an individualized and holistic way (Farh & Cheng, Reference Farh, Cheng, Li, Tsui and Weldon2000). Accordingly, benevolent leadership is expected to mitigate the influences of deviance on fear as benevolent leaders' high tolerance for subordinates' mistakes and wrong-doings can alleviate the uncertain and uncontrollable appraisals associated with deviance. This research also expects benevolent leadership to weaken the effects of fear on work engagement and OCB because benevolent leaders provide employees with individual care and support to manage their fear. Figure 1 illustrates our overall conceptual model.

Figure 1. Conceptual model.

The present research contributes to the literature on workplace deviance in several ways. First, this research moves beyond the victim-centric perspective of deviance and examines the deviance's consequences to the perpetrators themselves. In doing so, this research contributes to a greater theoretical understanding of workplace deviance, its full range of outcomes, and the emerging stream of perpetrator-centric studies on deviance (Bonner, Greenbaum, & Quade, Reference Bonner, Greenbaum and Quade2017; Meier & Spector, Reference Meier and Spector2013). Second, by identifying fear as a salient, and perhaps dominating, emotional reaction to deviance, the present work extends recent research by revealing that perpetrators do not always engage in compensatory behaviors to amend for deviant behaviors. When fear is considered, this research suggests that employees are likely to suffer from their deviance by disengaging from work and behaving less pro-socially. Finally, benevolent leadership is a type of positive leadership, and existing research has connected it directly to several positive outcomes at work for employees (Lin, Ma, Zhang, Li, & Jiang, Reference Lin, Ma, Zhang, Li and Jiang2018; Pellegrini & Scandura, Reference Pellegrini and Scandura2008). This research advances the field's knowledge on how benevolent leadership works in the context of workplace deviance. Specifically, this research suggests that benevolent leadership has beneficial moderating effects on employees who have engaged in deviant behaviors by mitigating the detrimental effects of that deviance on the perpetrator's emotional and behavioral outcomes. These moderation findings provide practical implications for managers who must minimize the damaging consequences of deviance, which is usually unavoidable in the workplace.

Theory and hypotheses

Appraisal theories of emotion

Appraisal theorists of emotion (Arnold, Reference Arnold1960; Frijda, Reference Frijda1986) seek to understand the factors that determine people's emotional experience from a cognitive perspective. The central tenet of appraisal theories of emotion is that an event's appraisal, rather than the event itself, determines whether an emotion (and why a certain emotion) is felt in a specific situation. Appraisals detect and evaluate the significance of the environment for individual well-being, that is, whether individual interests, concerns, or goals are satisfied or obstructed (Frijda, Reference Frijda2007). According to Smith and Lazarus (Reference Smith, Lazarus and Pervin1990), ‘each emotion expresses a person's appraisal of a person–environment relationship involving a particular kind of harm or benefit’ (p. 611).

There are two well-established dimensions of appraisal (Moors, Ellsworth, Scherer, & Frijda, Reference Moors, Ellsworth, Scherer and Frijda2013). The first is motivational congruence, which assesses the consistency between the events and a person's motives or goals (Lazarus & Smith, Reference Lazarus and Smith1988). And the second is certainty, which assesses the extent to which the situation is unpredictable (Arnold, Reference Arnold1960). In the next section, this research suggests that fear will be activated after employees engage in deviance based on these appraisal processes.

Workplace deviance and perpetrators' feelings of fear

Workplace deviance is intentional behavior that seeks to harm the organization or its members (Mackey, McAllister, Ellen, & Carson, Reference Mackey, McAllister, Ellen and Carson2021). Prototypical deviance includes verbal aggression, theft, and various forms of interpersonal mistreatment (Bennett & Robinson, Reference Bennett, Robinson and Greenberg2003). Because deviance induces actual harm to others or the organization, perpetrators of deviance will be, or will at least entail, a high risk of being retaliated against by others (Spector & Fox, Reference Spector, Fox, Fox and Spector2005). Aquino, Tripp, and Bies (Reference Aquino, Tripp and Bies2001), for example, found that victims often cope with interpersonal deviance by retaliating against the perpetrators. According to appraisal theories of emotion, when an event is appraised as inconsistent with one's motives and goals and will likely result in uncertain outcomes, the event typically elicits the emotion of fear (Smith & Ellsworth, Reference Smith and Ellsworth1985; Smith & Lazarus, Reference Smith, Lazarus and Pervin1990). Accordingly, we suggest that perpetrators of deviance are likely to experience feelings of fear because their appraisals of prior deviance are associated with the cognitions of motivational incongruence and uncertainty.

First, deviance should arouse perpetrators' cognitions of motivational incongruence, which assesses the consistency between the events and a person's motives or goals (Lazarus & Smith, Reference Lazarus and Smith1988). People typically strive to be moral persons or at least strive to be perceived as such (Aquino & Reed, Reference Aquino and Reed2002). Engaging in deviance jeopardizes such goals because deviance is often regarded as a form of unethical behavior (Moore, Detert, Treviño, Baker, & Mayer, Reference Moore, Detert, Treviño, Baker and Mayer2012; Moore, Mayer, Chiang, Crossley, Karlesky, & Birtch, Reference Moore, Mayer, Chiang, Crossley, Karlesky and Birtch2019), which leads to motivational incongruence. Besides, people fundamentally need to be accepted by social groups (Baumeister & Leary, Reference Baumeister and Leary1995). In the work context, employees often desire to maintain good interpersonal relationships with others to maintain connectedness or climb the organizational ladder more effectively (Ferris, Lian, Brown, & Morrison, Reference Ferris, Lian, Brown and Morrison2015). Once discovered, perpetrators may expect their deviant behaviors to lead others to dislike or ostracize them (Lian, Ferris, Morrison, & Brown, Reference Lian, Ferris, Morrison and Brown2014). As such, this research suggests that engaging in deviance or appraising past behavior as deviant will lead to motivational incongruence.

Second, appraisals of past deviance induce high levels of uncertainty, which is the core appraisal dimension for the emotion of fear (Smith & Lazarus, Reference Smith, Lazarus and Pervin1990). Perpetrators' uncertainty comes from ruminating on whether, when, and by whom they will be caught or retaliated against at work (Aquino, Tripp, & Bies, Reference Aquino, Tripp and Bies2001). Empirical findings demonstrate that those who observe workplace deviance are motivated to punish the perpetrator, and the perpetrators themselves will experience stress and constraints due to such uncertainties (Meier & Spector, Reference Meier and Spector2013).

Taken together, engaging in or appraising prior deviance leads to motivational incongruence and high uncertainty levels, which are the core appraisal contents for the negative emotion of fear (Smith & Ellsworth, Reference Smith and Ellsworth1985). Hence, the following hypothesis is proposed:

Hypothesis 1: Workplace deviance is positively related to the perpetrator's negative emotion of fear.

Implications for the perpetrator's work engagement and OCB

Once the appraisal dimensions of deviance trigger feelings of fear, we suggest that such feelings would lead to behavioral withdrawal (Fox & Spector, Reference Fox and Spector1999). The experience of fear entails high coping potential levels; that is, individuals are motivated to expend effort to deal with unpleasant emotions (Smith & Ellsworth, Reference Smith and Ellsworth1985). The coping strategy of fear is generally associated with ‘flight,’ withdrawing, or avoidant behavioral tendencies as an attempt to seek protection (Frijda, Kuipers, & Ter Schure, Reference Frijda, Kuipers and Ter Schure1989). More specifically, fear is a typical form of avoidance emotion that activates avoidance motivation (Lerner & Keltner, Reference Lerner and Keltner2001). Driven by avoidance motivation, individuals experiencing fear usually withdraw from their current activities and seek to preserve their efforts (Öhman, Reference Öhman, Lewis, Haviland-Jones and Barrett2008). In the organizational context, an employee suffering from fear is likely to disengage from their work (Clark & Loxton, Reference Clark and Loxton2012). Organizational behavior research has suggested that employee work engagement can vary significantly across workdays, and emotions play a key role in affecting daily work engagement (Bledow, Schmitt, Frese, & Kühnel, Reference Bledow, Schmitt, Frese and Kühnel2011; Park, Kim, Jung, Yun, & Hai, Reference Park, Kim, Jung, Yun and Hai2021). For example, using a diary study involving 1,203 day-level data from 117 workers, Miralles, Navarro, and Unger (Reference Miralles, Navarro and Unger2015) empirically demonstrated that general negative emotions negatively influenced state work engagement. Hence, the following hypothesis is proposed:

Hypothesis 2a: Fear is negatively related to the perpetrator's work engagement.

Social psychology research also suggests that, in addition to withdrawing from current activities, the avoidance motivation activated by fear will motivate individuals to avoid social interactions with others (Plant & Devine, Reference Plant, Devine and Elliot2008). In the organizational context, recent research has revealed that avoidance emotions (i.e., anxiety) are associated with employees' avoidance-oriented interpersonal behaviors that ‘include any action that seeks to remove an individual from, or otherwise minimize, interaction with situations or individuals’ (Ferris, Yan, Lim, Chen, & Fatimah, Reference Ferris, Yan, Lim, Chen and Fatimah2016). Drawing on these arguments and empirical findings, this research postulates that experiencing fear will also decrease employee OCB.

Broadly defined, OCB is a form of discretionary behavior that goes beyond formal job requirements but promotes organizational effectiveness (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, & Organ, Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie and Organ2006). Engaging in OCBs is an effortful social interaction process (Fehr, Yam, He, Chiang, & Wei, Reference Fehr, Yam, He, Chiang and Wei2017); it requires strong psychological resources (e.g., self-control; for a review, see Lian, Yam, Ferris, and Brown, Reference Lian, Yam, Ferris and Brown2017) to enact altruistic behaviors. Thus, employees are less likely to do OCB when they experience fear, the action tendencies of which are avoidance-oriented (Öhman, Reference Öhman, Lewis, Haviland-Jones and Barrett2008). Indirect support for this argument comes from Milliken, Morrison, and Hewlin (Reference Milliken, Morrison and Hewlin2003), who revealed that employees experiencing fear were more likely to remain silent at work, which is negatively related to OCBs such as suggesting new ideas to improve organizational effectiveness. Following these theoretical arguments and findings, the hypothesis is as follows:

Hypothesis 2b: Fear is negatively related to the perpetrator's OCB.

Integrating Hypothesis 1 with Hypotheses 2a and 2b, this research suggests that deviance will have indirect and negative relationships with work engagement and OCB via heightened feelings of fear. At first glance, our hypotheses seem to directly contrast past research, which has revealed that deviance has an indirect, positive relationship with OCB via the negative moral emotion of guilt (Ilies et al., Reference Ilies, Peng, Savani and Dimotakis2013). Although individuals can experience both guilt and fear due to prior deviance, we argue that the detrimental effects of fear on perpetrator OCB and work engagement will override the positive effects of guilt for several reasons.

First, as aforementioned, fear is widely recognized as a basic human emotion (Izard, Reference Izard1992). Specifically, Frijda (Reference Frijda1986) stated that the significant difference between basic emotions and nonbasic emotions was action readiness. He regarded basic emotions as behavior-oriented. Also, he considered that basic emotions are not composed of different other emotions. Fear is considered a basic emotion because the corresponding action readiness (to avoid a threat) cannot be reduced to any other action tendency. Guilt, however, is not basic because it is defined by its object (i.e., the negative evaluation of a specific behavior), and its correlated action tendencies (e.g., reparative behaviors) are composed of sorrow and disgust, which belong to other basic emotions (Ekman, Friesen, & Ellsworth, Reference Ekman, Friesen, Ellsworth and Ekman1982). Therefore, fear tends to have more direct and stronger influences on individual actions than guilt because basic emotions are biologically primitive; that is, they are essential in evolution and adaptation (Izard, Reference Izard1992). Indeed, Öhman and Mineka (Reference Öhman and Mineka2001) demonstrated that fear activation and fear responses are automatic and impenetrable to cognitive control. In other words, after fearful feelings are activated, the resulting responses and actions are biologically natural, fast, and strong; indeed, they are likely stronger than the responses and actions are driven by nonbasic emotions such as guilt.

Second, whereas one's avoidance motivation generally drives the negative effect of fear on OCB, the positive effect of guilt on OCB is generally driven by one's approach motivation (Elliot & Covington, Reference Elliot and Covington2001). The classic research on social psychology has suggested that when both motivational tendencies are activated, the self is more likely to be dominated by avoidance motivation than approach motivation (Tversky & Kahneman, Reference Tversky and Kahneman1991). In directly comparing the appraisal differences of various emotions, Smith and Ellsworth (Reference Smith and Ellsworth1985) revealed that the avoidance tendencies associated with fear were significantly stronger than the approach tendencies associated with guilt and shame. The hypothesis is as follows:

Hypothesis 3: Controlling for guilt, workplace deviance has indirect and negative relationships with the perpetrator's (a) work engagement and (b) OCB via the negative emotion of fear.

The moderating role of benevolent leadership

This research highlights the role of fear in the link between workplace deviance and work engagement and OCB. Besides, it is important to understand that this mediated effect is unlikely to be consistent across all employees. To identify factors most likely to moderate the mediated effect, this research draws from Gross (Reference Gross1998) theory of emotion regulation. Specifically, benevolent leadership, referring to leaders demonstrating individualized, holistic concern for subordinates' personal welfare within or beyond the work domain (Farh & Cheng, Reference Farh, Cheng, Li, Tsui and Weldon2000; Li, Rubenstein, Lin, Wang, & Chen, Reference Li, Rubenstein, Lin, Wang and Chen2018; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ma, Zhang, Li and Jiang2018), will shape the influences of deviance on the perpetrator's fearful feelings and work outcomes by affecting fear's effects on employees' behaviors.

Our theorizing is consistent with Gross' theory of emotion regulation (Gross & Barrett, Reference Gross and Barrett2011), which explains to what intensity an emotion is felt and whether the felt emotion would lead to behavioral outcomes. Briefly stated, Gross (Reference Gross1998) suggested that emotion regulation is a two-stage process. The first stage focuses on interpreting the emotion experienced, whereas the second stage focuses on behavioral responses, if any, to the emotion experienced. Next, this research explicates the role benevolent leadership plays at both stages (i.e., fear activation and fear implication).

Antecedent-focused emotion regulation

In the antecedent-focused stage of emotion regulation, employees could respond to their deviant behaviors differently based on their specific leader. Although the experience of fear is common across all employees who committed deviance, the intensity of fear differs. Specifically, Gross and Barrett (Reference Gross and Barrett2011) suggested that individuals can alter their interpretations of an emotion-eliciting cue based on social contexts. For example, an employee who perceives strong organizational sanctions against unethical behavior would likely experience more fear after committing deviance than an employee who perceives weak organizational sanctions against unethical behavior.

The positive relationship between a perpetrator's deviance and his/her own emotion of fear will be weakened by high benevolent leadership, which reduces the perpetrator's uncertainty appraisals after deviance. Specifically, benevolent leaders have a high tolerance for subordinates' mistakes and wrongdoings, are likely to forgive employees' deviant behaviors, and allow opportunities to correct mistakes (Farh & Cheng, Reference Farh, Cheng, Li, Tsui and Weldon2000; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ma, Zhang, Li and Jiang2018; Wang & Cheng, Reference Wang and Cheng2010). On the one hand, benevolent leaders will neither adopt nor allow others to adopt a tit-for-tat policy to retaliate against employees who engaged in deviance. On the other hand, those employees would be certain that retaliation against their deviant behaviors is unlikely, and thus feelings of fear are likely less intense. Chen, Eberly, Chiang, Farh, and Cheng (Reference Chen, Eberly, Chiang, Farh and Cheng2014), for example, discovered that benevolent leadership was beneficial in building a forgiving climate in an organization, thus reducing the likelihood of perpetrators receiving retaliation (Fehr & Gelfand, Reference Fehr and Gelfand2012). Taken together, the hypothesis is as follows:

Hypothesis 4: Benevolent leadership moderates the intra-individual level relationship between workplace deviance and the perpetrator's negative emotion of fear such that the relationship is less positive when benevolent leadership is high rather than it is low.

Response-focused emotion regulation

After experiencing an emotion, in our case fear, individuals turn to the response-focused stage of emotion regulation (Gross & Barrett, Reference Gross and Barrett2011). In this stage, emotion-driven behavioral consequences can be amplified or suppressed based on social contexts. For example, whereas experiencing gratitude might lead to increased prosocial behavior (Spence, Brown, Keeping, & Lian, Reference Spence, Brown, Keeping and Lian2014), a cut-throat organizational culture might suppress emotion-driven prosocial behavior. Although experiencing fear might lead to reduced job engagement and OCB in a similar vein, a supportive social context might suppress such avoidance tendencies (Lebel, Reference Lebel2017).

In the context of individual deviance related to fear and the two work outcomes this research theorized, we suggest that benevolent leaders can mitigate the negative behavioral effects of fear by providing employees with individualized care (Li et al., Reference Li, Rubenstein, Lin, Wang and Chen2018; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ma, Zhang, Li and Jiang2018; Pellegrini & Scandura, Reference Pellegrini and Scandura2008). Typical benevolent leadership behaviors include treating employees as family members and showing holistic concern about employees' emotional and attitudinal states (Wang & Cheng, Reference Wang and Cheng2010). By providing tangible and intangible resources and social support, benevolent leaders give employees the confidence to deal with negative events or situations and manage their fearful emotions (Lebel, Reference Lebel2017). In short, we expect that the relationships between fear and work engagement and OCB will be less negative when individuals perceive high rather than low benevolent leadership. Thus, the hypothesis is as follows:

Hypothesis 5: Benevolent leadership moderates the intra-individual level relationships between the perpetrator's negative emotion of fear and (a) work engagement, and (b) OCB, such that the relationships are less negative when benevolent leadership is high rather than it is low.

Integrating Hypothesis 4 with Hypotheses 5a and 5b, the following hypothesis is proposed:

Hypothesis 6: Benevolent leadership moderates the indirect relationships between workplace deviance and the perpetrator's (a) work engagement and (b) OCB via the negative emotion of fear, such that the indirect relationships will be less negative when benevolent leadership is high rather than low.

Methods

Participants and procedures

This research collected data from employees working at a Chinese firm located in Central China. This firm is a large corporation with diversified business areas such as manufacturing, construction, and real estate. The chief executive officer (CEO) of this firm is an EMBA alumnus who graduated from the business school with which the first author is affiliated. The CEO acted as the ‘gatekeeper’ in the data collection process. With his permission and support, the research team asked the HR manager of this firm to post an advertisement to all full-time employees. In this advertisement, the research team provided information about our university affiliation and invited employees to participate in a work attitudes and behavior-related survey. Also, the research team stated the survey's time requirement (10 consecutive working days' participation) and indicated each participant would receive 50 Chinese Yuan (about $7.70) for participating.

A total of 102 employees from different departments and work roles voluntarily participated in this diary survey. One week before starting the diary survey, the research team gathered participants in a large meeting room and repeatedly emphasized that the daily questionnaires had to be filled out after work. Besides, the research team emphasized voluntary participation and full confidentiality again. Then, the research team asked the participants to complete an initial survey in which they reported their demographic information and rated their leaders' benevolent leadership. A week after the initial survey, participants started the diary survey assessing their daily performance of deviance, negative emotions of fear and guilt, and work engagement and OCB.

Ten employees did not return any of their daily surveys; therefore, the final sample consisted of 92 employees with 918 observations, indicating the response rate was 90.20% in this study. Of the 92 participants, the average age was 30.17 (SD = 7.11), and 46.55% were male. Most of them were well-educated (81.52% had a college education or higher). In terms of their occupation, 11.96% of participants were from production departments, 51.09% were from administrative departments, 19.57% were from research and development departments, and 17.39% were from marketing departments.

Measures

The scales this research used in this study were originally in English. Hence, to guarantee our questionnaires' accuracy, which were finally in Chinese, this study followed Brislin (Reference Brislin, Triandis and Berry1980) translation and back-translation procedure. Except for perceived benevolent leadership, all of the variables were measured using 7-point Likert scales (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree).

Deviance

This study measured employees' daily deviance using a 5-item short scale of workplace deviance adapted from Bennett and Robinson (Reference Bennett and Robinson2000). The original version of Bennett and Robinson (Reference Bennett and Robinson2000) scale has 19 items and makes it difficult to use in a diary survey. This study selected five items, complying with the following standard (1) the behaviors described in items could vary daily, and (2) the behaviors described in items composed of not only individual-targeted deviance but also organization-targeted deviance. These five items are ‘Worked on a personal matter instead of work for my employer,’ ‘Spent too much time fantasizing or daydreaming instead of working,’ ‘Made fun of someone at work,’ ‘Took an additional or a longer break than is acceptable at my workplace,’ and ‘Lost my temper while at work.’ This 5-item short scale was validated in a prior diary study conducted in the Chinese context (Fehr et al., Reference Fehr, Yam, He, Chiang and Wei2017). Cronbach's α was .80 in this study.

Fear

This study measured employees' daily negative emotion of fear using four items, including ‘afraid,’ ‘scared,’ ‘nervous,’ and ‘jittery.’ This scale was adapted from the PANAS-X scale (Watson & Clark, Reference Watson and Clark1994). Compared with the original 6-item scale of fear, the item ‘frightened’ was not used because its Chinese translation is virtually identical to the Chinese translation of ‘scared.’ Besides, the item ‘shaky’ was not used because its Chinese translation has vague meanings. Cronbach's α was .87 in this study.

Work engagement

This study measured employees' daily work engagement using Schaufeli, Bakker, and Salanova (Reference Schaufeli, Bakker and Salanova2006) 9-item scale. Sample items included, ‘I felt happy when I was doing this task intensely,’ and ‘I was enthusiastic about my task.’ Cronbach's α of these nine items was .95.

Organizational citizenship behavior

Consistent with Fehr et al. (Reference Fehr, Yam, He, Chiang and Wei2017), this study measured employees' daily OCB using a 5-item short scale adapted from Lee and Allen (Reference Lee and Allen2002) 16-item scale. Like our logic in shortening the deviance scale, these five items were selected because they were most likely to vary each day and the behaviors described in items composed of individual-targeted OCB and organization-targeted OCB. Specifically, these five items are ‘Expressed loyalty toward the organization,’ ‘Took action to protect the organization from potential problems,’ ‘Demonstrated concern about the image of the organization,’ ‘Willingly gave my time to help others who had work-related problems,’ and ‘Showed genuine concern and courtesy toward my coworkers.’ In this study, Cronbach's α was .87.

Perceived benevolent leadership

In the entry survey, participants evaluated their supervisors' benevolent behaviors using an 11-item subscale of the Paternalistic Leadership scale (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Eberly, Chiang, Farh and Cheng2014). Sample items included, ‘My supervisor tries to understand the cause when I do not perform well,’ and ‘My supervisor will help me when I am in an emergency’ (1 = strongly disagree, 6 = strongly agree). Cronbach's α was .94.

Control variables

This study controlled for the possible influences of employees' negative emotion of guilt on their state work engagement and OCB; indeed, Ilies et al. (Reference Ilies, Peng, Savani and Dimotakis2013) have demonstrated the mediating role of guilt in linking individual deviance with increased OCB. Guilt was measured using the first two items adapted from the PANAS-X scale (Watson & Clark, Reference Watson and Clark1994). Specifically, the research team asked the participants to evaluate their feeling of ‘guilty’ and ‘ashamed’ at work. Cronbach's α was .80.

Analytic approach

Following Bolger, Davis, and Rafaeli (Reference Bolger, Davis and Rafaeli2003) recommendations for how to analyze diary data, and in line with prior diary studies (Wang, Liu, Liao, Gong, Kammeyer-Mueller, & Shi, Reference Wang, Liu, Liao, Gong, Kammeyer-Mueller and Shi2013), this study controlled for previous days' measures of fear (i.e., the mediator), and state work engagement and OCB (i.e., the dependent variables) at the within-individual level in all our analyses.

This study tested our research model using multi-level path-analysis because our data were nested. The Mplus 7.0 software (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2010) was used in the analyses of this study. To test the main effects and mediation relationships (i.e., Hypotheses 1–3), this study initially estimated a multilevel mediation model that specified the Level 1 random slope effect of deviance on fear, the Level 1 random slope effects of fear on state work engagement, and OCB.

This study also calculated the direct influence of deviance on the outcome variables. Moreover, the cross-level effects of benevolent leadership on the mediator of fear and the two dependent variables were estimated as controls. Besides, as noted, guilt was added as a control variable, mediating the relationships of deviance with state work engagement and OCB. All previous days' measures of fear, state work engagement, and OCB were included in this model as control variables. Before conducting these analyses, this study first group-mean centered Level 1 predicting and control variables (Hofmann, Griffin, & Gavin, Reference Hofmann, Griffin, Gavin, Klein and Kozlowski2000).

Based on this multilevel mediation model, this study tested our multilevel moderated mediation hypotheses by additionally estimating the cross-level moderating effects of benevolent leadership on the random slope for deviance predicting fear and on the random slopes for fear predicting state work engagement and OCB. This multilevel moderated mediation model was used to demonstrate the moderation and moderated mediation relationships (i.e., Hypotheses 4–6). To alleviate potential problems relating to multicollinearity, the moderator in Level 2 (i.e., benevolent leadership) was grand-mean centered before conducting these analyses (Hofmann, Griffin, & Gavin, Reference Hofmann, Griffin, Gavin, Klein and Kozlowski2000).

In further testing the indirect effect of deviance on state work engagement and OCB through fear (i.e., Hypotheses 3a and 3b), Monte Carlo simulations were conducted with 20,000 replications to compute 95% confidence intervals (Preacher & Selig, Reference Preacher and Selig2012). Besides, to examine the moderated mediation effect further (i.e., Hypotheses 6a and 6b), this study tested the indirect effects of deviance on state work engagement and OCB through fear under two conditions (high vs. low level of benevolent leadership) using a method Bauer, Preacher, and Gil (Reference Bauer, Preacher and Gil2006) introduced.

Results

Preliminary analysis

Means, standard deviations, reliabilities, and correlations are presented in Table 1. This study conducted a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis (MCFA) to confirm the hypothesized 6-factor structure of employee perceived benevolent leadership, daily deviance, fear, state work engagement, OCB, and guilt, while also accounting for the nested structure of the data. Before conducting MCFA, state work engagement was packed into three indices representing the three distinct sub-dimensions: vigor, dedication, and absorption.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among variables

Note: Correlations above the diagonal represent between-individual correlations (N = 92).

Correlations below the diagonal represent within-individual correlations (N = 918).

SDw and SDb are standard deviations computed within and between individuals separately.

Coefficient α estimates of reliability are in parentheses on the diagonal. (The same below).

The hypothesized 6-factor model demonstrated a good fit to the data: χ2 = 563.28, df = 186, p < .001, CFI = .90, RMSEA = .05; SRMR (within) = .05, SRMR (between) = .10. This 6-factor model fit the data better than a 5-factor model grouping the two dependent variables of state work engagement and OCB: χ2 = 1,085.84, df = 190, p < .001, CFI = .76, RMSEA = .07; SRMR (within) = .07, SRMR (between) = .10; Δχ2 = 680.72, Δdf = 4, p < .001. Another 5-factor model grouped the independent variable (deviance) and the mediator (fear): χ2 = 1,197.86, df = 190, p < .001, CFI = .74, RMSEA = .08; SRMR (within) = .11, SRMR (between) = .10; Δχ2 = 189.13, Δdf = 4, p < .001. Overall, the results of the MCFA support the discriminant validity among our daily focal constructs.

Hypotheses testing

Hypothesis 1 proposed the direct effect of daily deviance on fear, and Hypotheses 2a and 2b proposed the direct effects of fear on state work engagement and OCB, respectively. This study tested these three hypotheses simultaneously in the same multilevel mediation path-analytical model. Here, fear was included as the mediator, and state work engagement and OCB were included as the dependent variables while controlling for guilt, previous day's fear, work engagement, and OCB. Results as shown in Table 2 revealed that daily deviance was negatively related to fear (β = .12, SE = .05, p < .01), supporting Hypothesis 1. Consistent with prior research, this study also found a direct relationship between deviance and guilt (β = .12, SE = .05, p < .05). In addition, when the effects of guilt on state work engagement (β = –.07, SE = .05, n.s.) and OCB (β = –.00, SE = .05, n.s.) were controlled, fear was still negatively related to state work engagement (β = –.15, SE = .06, p < .01) and OCB (β = –.12, SE = .05, p < .01). Hence, the results supported Hypotheses 2a and 2b.

Table 2. Multilevel path analysis results for testing Hypotheses 1, 2a, and 2b

Note: ‘t’ reflect the evaluation ‘today’; ‘t−1’ reflects the evaluation on the previous day compared to ‘today.’

The above estimates represent unstandardized path coefficients.

All hypothesized effects were calculated in the same path-analytical model and specified as random slopes.

**p < .01; ***p < .001. (The same below).

This research proposed in Hypotheses 3a and 3b that fear mediated the effects of daily deviance on perpetrators' state work engagement and OCB after controlling for the influences of guilt feelings. Following Bauer, Preacher, and Gil (Reference Bauer, Preacher and Gil2006), this study computed the indirect effects as the product of path a (daily deviance to fear) and path b (fear to state work engagement and OCB), plus the covariance between them (i.e., indirect effect = path a × path b + cov [a, b]). Bootstrapping results revealed that after controlling for the mediating effects of guilt, the indirect relationship between daily deviance and state work engagement via fear was significant (indirect effect = –.02, 95% CI = [–.004, –.001]). Besides, the indirect relationship between daily deviance and OCB via fear was also significant (indirect effect = –.01, 95% CI = [–.035, –.001]). Thus, Hypotheses 3a and 3b are supported.

Hypothesis 4 stated that employees' perceptions of benevolent leadership would moderate the relationship between daily deviance and fear. This relationship would be stronger for employees whose direct leaders are less (vs. more) benevolent. This study tested this cross-level moderation effect in a model that included the Level 2 moderator (i.e., perceived benevolent leadership) as a predictor of the random slope of the effect of daily deviance on fear. As the data in Table 3 and Figure 2 show, the relationship between daily deviance and fear was moderated by employee's perceived benevolent leadership (β = –.12, SE = .06, p < .05). Also, the moderating effect was plotted in Figure 3 (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, Reference Cohen, Cohen, West and Aiken2013). Simple slope tests indicated that daily deviance was more positively related to fear when employee perceived low benevolent leadership (simple slope = .64, p < .01) compared to high benevolent leadership (simple slope = .49, p < .01). Thus, Hypothesis 4 is supported.

Figure 2. Unstandardized estimates of the path coefficients (H4–H6).

Figure 3. The moderating effect of benevolent leadership on the relationship between deviance and fear.

Table 3. Results of the cross-level moderating effects of perceived benevolent leadership

Hypotheses 5a and 5b proposed that the effects of fear on state work engagement and OCB were moderated by benevolent leadership. This study tested these two hypotheses following the same procedures we used to test Hypothesis 4. Results revealed that employee perceived benevolent leadership moderated the relationship between fear and OCB (β = .13, SE = .05, p < .01), but could not moderate the relationship between fear and state work engagement (β = .10, SE = .08, p > .05). The interactive effects of fear with perceived benevolent leadership on OCB are plotted in Figure 4. Simple slope tests showed the association between fear and OCB was more negative when employees perceived low benevolent leadership (simple slope = –.67, p < .001), compared to high benevolent leadership (simple slope = –.50, p < .001). In sum, Hypothesis 5a is not supported, and Hypothesis 5b is supported.

Figure 4. The moderating effect of benevolent leadership on the relationship between fear and OCB.

Finally, Hypotheses 6a and 6b proposed moderated mediation effects that the indirect effects of daily deviance on state work engagement and OCB via fear would be stronger when employees perceived lower (vs. higher) levels of benevolent leadership. Following Bauer, Preacher, and Gil (Reference Bauer, Preacher and Gil2006), this study calculated the indirect influences of daily deviance on the two outcomes through fear at low perceived benevolent leadership (one SD below the mean) versus high (one SD above the mean) levels of perceived benevolent leadership. Specifically, the indirect effect was calculated as the product of path a (daily deviance to daily fear) and path b (fear to state work engagement/OCB) plus the covariance between them (i.e., indirect effect = path a × path b + cov [a, b]). The effect sizes of path a and path b will change at high versus low levels of the moderator (perceived benevolent leadership).

Bootstrapping results revealed that for state work engagement, the indirect effect was –.35 (95% CI = [–.82, .11]) when perceived benevolent leadership was low, and the indirect effect was –.21 (95% CI = [–.47, .05]) when perceived benevolent leadership was high. These results suggested that benevolent leadership does not moderate the mediated relationship between deviance and engagement via fear. Hence, Hypothesis 6a is not supported. For OCB, bootstrapping results showed that the indirect effect was –.43 (95% CI = [–.81, –.05]) when perceived benevolent leadership was low, and this indirect effect was –.25 (95% CI = [–.45, –.04]) when perceived benevolent leadership was high. The difference between the two indirect effects was significant (difference = .18; 95% CI = [.01, .36]). These results indicated that the indirect effects of daily deviance on OCB through fear were stronger when perceived benevolent leadership was low than high, thereby supporting Hypothesis 6b.

Discussion

Through a diary study, this research demonstrated the conceptual model regarding the intra-individual relationships among employee deviance, the negative emotion of fear, and the individual's work engagement and OCB, as contingent on benevolent leadership at the between-individual level. Specifically, this research found that employee deviance decreases work engagement and OCB through the increased negative emotion of fear. Moreover, benevolent leadership weakens these negative effects of deviance by mitigating both the first-stage relationship between deviance and fear (i.e., fear activation) and the second-stage relationships between fear and work outcomes (i.e., fear implication).

Theoretical implications

Several theoretical contributions and implications of the current research are worth highlighting. First, compared with empirical investigations focused on how employees' deviance affects other organizations' members (Eissa, Lester, & Gupta, Reference Eissa, Lester and Gupta2020; Foulk, Woolum, & Erez, Reference Foulk, Woolum and Erez2016; Moon, Morais, de Moura, & Uskul, Reference Moon, Morais, de Moura and Uskul2021) and organizational effectiveness (Carpenter, Whitman, & Amrhein, Reference Carpenter, Whitman and Amrhein2021; Jetten & Hornsey, Reference Jetten and Hornsey2014), this research contributes to the workplace deviance literature by theorizing and validating a perpetrator-centric perspective that reveals the effects of deviance on the perpetrator's own emotional, psychological, and behavioral outcomes in the workplace. Specifically, this study demonstrated that workplace deviance also triggers the perpetrator's negative emotion of fear, with negative downstream behavioral consequences.

Second, this research extends recent literature on deviance's consequences by revealing that the perpetrator conducting deviance might engage in withdrawal behaviors because of their negative emotion of fear. Extant research showed that perpetrators may always conduct compensatory behaviors to amend deviant behaviors (e.g., Ilies et al., Reference Ilies, Peng, Savani and Dimotakis2013). Specifically, Ilies et al. (Reference Ilies, Peng, Savani and Dimotakis2013) found guilt motivates the perpetrator of deviance to engage in more reparatory behavior such as OCB. Our research showed that when we controlled for the mediating effect of guilt, an employee's prior deviance negatively affected his or her psychological state (decreased work engagement) and pro-social behavior (decreased OCB) via the negative emotion of fear. These findings suggest that although prior deviance can lead to the perpetrator's guilt and fear simultaneously, fear might be a more salient and perhaps dominating emotional reaction to deviance and further demotivate the perpetrator to engage in OCB.

The third contribution is to the benevolent leadership literature by identifying that benevolent leadership works in the context of workplace deviance. Specifically, prior benevolent leadership research has focused on its main, beneficial effects on employee outcomes, such as task performance and creativity (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Eberly, Chiang, Farh and Cheng2014; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ma, Zhang, Li and Jiang2018; Wang & Cheng, Reference Wang and Cheng2010). This research extends these findings by showing the indirect, beneficial effects of benevolent leadership on employee outcomes. Our results show that benevolent leaders can reduce deviance-driven fear and prevent employees from decreasing pro-social behaviors (OCB) after they experience fear. These findings support Toegel, Kilduff, and Anand (Reference Toegel, Kilduff and Anand2013) theoretical argument that leadership plays a vital role in alleviating employees' negative emotions.

Moreover, these findings suggest another beneficial effect of benevolent leadership: it prevents employees from generating unpleasant feelings (i.e., fear) concerning their prior deviant behaviors at work. The mitigating effects of benevolent leadership on both the fear activation stage and the fear implication stage may be attributed to the complex content of the individualized care that benevolent leaders express. Whereas general support and providing resources can mitigate the detrimental influences of fearful feelings on one's future behaviors, the mistake-forgiving and face-saving aspect of benevolent leadership may also mitigate the intra-person effect of prior deviance on an individual's experience of fear.

Practical implications

Clearly, employee deviance is unavoidable in the workplace, and the occurrence of deviance is usually beyond managers' control given the complex motives within employees who engage in deviance. Thus, the results suggest more managerial attention should be paid to the affective reactions of individuals who execute deviance in the workplace. Specifically, the findings revealed that perpetrators' negative emotion of fear is an inevitable affective outcome of prior deviance, with significant downstream implications for their psychological and behavioral outcomes. The findings highlight the beneficial role of leadership, particularly benevolent leadership, in reducing employees' fearful emotions associated with prior deviance. Moreover, benevolent leadership can also help perpetrators regulate their fearful emotions by preventing them from decreasing future OCB after they experienced fear.

According to Farh, Liang, Chou, and Cheng (Reference Farh, Liang, Chou, Cheng, Chen and Lee2008), specific behaviors managers can display to show benevolence include forgiving employees' mistakes, avoiding the public embarrassment of employees, providing coaching and mentoring, and showing concern for employees' career development. In addition to individualized care in the work context, managers can also show personal care to employees beyond the work domain, such as providing financial or social support to employees during personal crises and maintaining high-quality relationships with employees by treating them as family members (Farh, Tsui, Xin, & Cheng, Reference Farh, Tsui, Xin and Cheng1998). In sum, although employee deviance is almost unavoidable in the workplace, leaders can display benevolent leadership to deter the corrosive effects of deviance on perpetrators' work outcomes.

Limitations and future research directions

The present research has several limitations. One limitation is the sole focus of fear as the underlying mechanism linking deviance with the perpetrator's work outcomes. Although this study controlled for guilt as a parallel mediator in all regression analyses, we did not consider other emotions and did not control their influences. For example, past research has suggested that people might feel momentarily excited after engaging in unethical behavior (Ruedy, Moore, Gino, & Schweitzer, Reference Ruedy, Moore, Gino and Schweitzer2013). Thus, it is possible that workplace deviance leads to employees' positive emotions as well, which might motivate other outcomes beyond work engagement and OCB. Future research needs to take a more nuanced theoretical perspective to examine the influences of different discrete emotions in the relationships between deviance and the perpetrator's work outcomes.

Another limitation of the present research is that the data were collected in a specific Chinese firm, thus limiting our work's generalizability in two ways. On the one side, for example, China is still a transition economy, and moral regulation and penalty systems are not well established in Chinese firms. In other words, it is usually the leaders rather than formal policies that decide how deviant employees are treated. This condition strengthens the uncertainty perceptions associated with employees' past deviant behaviors. For this reason, the finding that fear overrides guilt in linking prior deviance with future work outcomes should be replicated in other cultural contexts. On the other side, although no evidence has been found in workplace deviance literature that collecting data from a certain firm might contaminate the results, we still encourage future research to retest our theory among more generalized sample participants.

Conclusion

Prior research has revealed the detrimental effects of deviance on both organizational functioning and its employees. More recent work has started to examine the surprisingly positive effects of deviance on the perpetrators themselves. The present research extends this literature by exploring the relationship between deviance and the perpetrator's work engagement and OCB via the mediating role of fear, as contingent on the contextual influence of benevolent leadership. Controlling for guilt, this research demonstrates that deviance is generally associated with reduced work engagement and OCB for the perpetrators because the emotion of fear overrides feelings of guilt. This research emphasizes the roles of emotion and leadership in understanding the effects of deviance on perpetrators' work outcomes.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 71602147).

References

Aquino, K., & Reed, A. II. (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 14231440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aquino, K., Tripp, T. M., & Bies, R. J. (2001). How employees respond to personal offense: The effects of blame attribution, victim status, and offender status on revenge and reconciliation in the workplace. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 5259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J., & Gil, K. M. (2006). Conceptualizing and testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 11, 142163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, R. J., Marasi, S., & Locklear, L. (2019) Workplace deviance. In Oxford research encyclopedia of business and management: 125. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bennett, R. J., & Robinson, S. L. (2000). Development of a measure of workplace deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 349360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, R. J., & Robinson, S. L. (2003). The past, present and future of workplace deviance research. In Greenberg, J., (Ed.), Organizational behavior: The state of the science (pp. 247281). Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum.Google Scholar
Bledow, R., Schmitt, A., Frese, M., & Kühnel, J. (2011). The affective shift model of work engagement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 12461257. doi: 10.1037/a0024532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonner, J. M., Greenbaum, R. L., & Quade, M. J. (2017). Employee unethical behavior to shame as an indicator of self-image threat and exemplification as a form of self-image protection: The exacerbating role of supervisor bottom-line mentality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102, 12031221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowling, N. A., & Beehr, T. A. (2006). Workplace harassment from the victim's perspective: A theoretical model and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 9981012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brislin, R. W. (1980). Translation and content analysis of oral and written material. In Triandis, H. C. & Berry, J. W. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 389444). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Carpenter, N. C., Whitman, D. S., & Amrhein, R. (2021). Unit-level counterproductive work behavior (CWB): A conceptual review and quantitative summary. Journal of Management, 47, 14981527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X. P., Eberly, M. B., Chiang, T. J., Farh, J. L., & Cheng, B. S. (2014). Affective trust in Chinese leaders linking paternalistic leadership to employee performance. Journal of Management, 40, 796819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, D. M. T., & Loxton, N. J. (2012). Fear, psychological acceptance, job demands and employee work engagement: An integrative moderated meditation model. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 893897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2013). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunlop, P. D., & Lee, K. (2004). Workplace deviance, organizational citizenship behavior, and business unit performance: The bad apples do spoil the whole barrel. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 6780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eissa, G., Lester, S. W., & Gupta, R. (2020). Interpersonal deviance and abusive supervision: The mediating role of supervisor negative emotions and the moderating role of subordinate organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 166, 577594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1982). What are the similarities and differences in facial behavior across cultures? In Ekman, P. (Ed.), Emotion in the human face (2nd ed., pp. 128143). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J., & Covington, M. V. (2001). Approach and avoidance motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 7392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Goldsmith, H. H. & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 572595). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Farh, J. L., & Cheng, B. S. (2000). A cultural analysis of paternalistic leadership in Chinese organizations. In Li, J., Tsui, A. & Weldon, E. (Eds.), Management and organizations in the Chinese context (pp. 84127). London: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farh, J. L., Liang, J., Chou, L. F., & Cheng, B. S. (2008). Paternalistic leadership in Chinese organizations: Research progress and future research directions. In Chen, C. C. & Lee, Y. T. (Eds.), Business leadership in China: Philosophies, theories, and practices (pp. 171205). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farh, J. L., Tsui, A. S., Xin, K., & Cheng, B. S. (1998). The influence of relational demography and guanxi: The Chinese case. Organization Science, 9, 471488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, R., & Gelfand, M. J. (2012). The forgiving organization: A multilevel model of forgiveness at work. Academy of Management Review, 37, 664688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, R., Yam, K. C., He, W., Chiang, J., & Wei, W. (2017). Polluted work: A self-control perspective on air pollution, organizational citizenship, and counterproductive work behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 143, 98110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferris, D. L., Lian, H., Brown, D. J., & Morrison, R. (2015). Ostracism, self-esteem, and job performance: When do we self-verify and when do we self-enhance? Academy of Management Journal, 58, 279297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferris, D. L., Yan, M., Lim, V. K. G., Chen, Y., & Fatimah, S. (2016). An approach-avoidance framework of workplace aggression. Academy of Management Journal, 59, 17771800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foulk, T., Woolum, A., & Erez, A. (2016). Catching rudeness is like catching a cold: The contagion effects of low-intensity negative behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (1999). A model of work frustration-aggression. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 915931.3.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions: Studies in emotion and social interaction. Paris: Maison de Sciences de l'Homme.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (2007). The laws of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & Ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J., & Barrett, F. L. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: One or two depends on your point of view. Emotion Review, 3, 816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R. & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852870). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harvey, P., Martinko, M. J., & Borkowski, N. (2017). Justifying deviant behavior: The role of attributions and moral emotions. Journal of Business Ethics, 141, 779795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, D. A., Griffin, M., & Gavin, M. (2000). The application of hierarchical linear modeling to organizational research. In Klein, K. J. & Kozlowski, S. W. J. (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organizations (pp. 467511). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Ilies, R., Peng, A. C., Savani, K., & Dimotakis, N. (2013). Guilty and helpful: An emotion-based reparatory model of voluntary work behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 10511059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion-cognition relations. Psychological Review, 99, 561565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jetten, J., & Hornsey, M. J. (2014). Deviance and dissent in groups. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 461485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Knowledge and appraisal in the cognition – emotion relationship. Cognition & Emotion, 2, 281300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lebel, R. D. (2017). Moving beyond fight and flight: A contingent model of how the emotional regulation of anger and fear sparks proactivity. Academy of Management Review, 42, 190206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, K., & Allen, N. J. (2002). Organizational citizenship behavior and workplace deviance: The role of affect and cognitions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 131142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, G. Q., Rubenstein, A. L., Lin, W. P., Wang, M., & Chen, X. W. (2018). The curvilinear effect of benevolent leadership on team performance: The mediating role of team action processes and the moderating role of team commitment. Personnel Psychology, 71, 369397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lian, H., Ferris, D. L., Morrison, R., & Brown, D. (2014). Blame it on the supervisor or the subordinate? Reciprocal relations between abusive supervision and organizational deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 651664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lian, H., Yam, K. C., Ferris, L., & Brown, D. (2017). Self-control at work. Academy of Management Annuals, 11, 703732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, W. P., Ma, J. J., Zhang, Q., Li, J. C., & Jiang, F. (2018). How is benevolent leadership linked to employee creativity? The mediating role of leader-member exchange and the moderating role of power distance orientation. Journal of Business Ethics, 152, 10991115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackey, J. D., McAllister, C. P., Ellen, B. P., & Carson, J. E. (2021). A meta-analysis of interpersonal and organizational workplace deviance research. Journal of Management, 47, 597622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, L. L., & Spector, P. E. (2013). Reciprocal effects of work stressors and counterproductive work behavior: A five-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 529539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milliken, F. J., Morrison, E. W., & Hewlin, P. F. (2003). An exploratory study of employee silence: Issues that employees don't communicate upward and why. Journal of Management Studies, 40, 14531476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miralles, C., Navarro, J., & Unger, D. (2015). Daily work events and state work engagement: The mediating role of affect. Revista de Psicología Social, 30, 264294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, C., Morais, C., de Moura, G. R., & Uskul, A. K. (2021). The role of organizational structure and deviant status in employees' reactions to and acceptance of workplace deviance. International Journal of Conflict Management, 32, 315339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., Baker, V. L., & Mayer, D. M. (2012). Why employees do bad things: Moral disengagement and unethical organizational behavior. Personnel Psychology, 65, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., Mayer, D. M., Chiang, F. F. T., Crossley, C., Karlesky, M. J., & Birtch, T. A. (2019). Leaders matter morally: The role of ethical leadership in shaping employee moral cognition and misconduct. Journal of Applied Psychology, 104, 123145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., & Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and future development. Emotion Review, 5, 119124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus user's guide: Statistical analysis with latent variables. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Öhman, A. (2008). Fear and anxiety. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M. & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 709729). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, I. J., Kim, P. B., Jung, H., Yun, D., & Hai, S. Y. (2021). A diary study of work consequences for hotel employees victimized by supervisor from a demand-resource perspective on employee emotions. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 92, 102714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellegrini, E. K., & Scandura, T. A. (2008). Paternalistic leadership: A review and agenda for future research. Journal of Management, 34, 566593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2008). Interracial interactions: Approach and avoidance. In Elliot, A. J. (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 571584). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Organ, D. W. (2006). Organizational citizenship behavior: Its nature, antecedents, and consequences. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Selig, J. P. (2012). Advantages of Monte Carlo confidence intervals for indirect effects. Communication Methods and Measures, 6, 7798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruedy, N. E., Moore, C., Gino, F., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2013). The cheater's high: The unexpected affective benefits of unethical behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 531548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaufeli, W. B., Bakker, A. B., & Salanova, M. (2006). The measurement of work engagement with a short questionnaire a cross-national study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66, 701716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-Romá, V., & Bakker, A. B. (2002). The measurement of engagement and burnout: A two sample confirmatory factor analytic approach. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 7192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1990). Emotion and adaptation. In Pervin, L. A. (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 609637). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 7, 233269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spector, P. E., & Fox, S. (2002). An emotion-centered model of voluntary work behavior: Some parallels between counterproductive work behavior and organizational citizenship behavior. Human Resource Management Review, 12, 269292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spector, P. E., & Fox, S. (2005). The stressor-emotion model of counterproductive work behavior. In Fox, S. & Spector, P. E. (Eds.), Counterproductive work behavior: Investigations of actors and targets (pp. 151174). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, J. R., Brown, D. J., Keeping, L. M., & Lian, H. (2014). Helpful today, but not tomorrow? Feeling grateful as a predictor of daily organizational citizenship behaviors. Personnel Psychology, 67, 705738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toegel, G., Kilduff, M., & Anand, N. (2013). Emotion helping by managers: An emergent understanding of discrepant role expectations and outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 56, 334357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1991). Loss aversion in riskless choice: A reference-dependent model. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106, 10391061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, A. C., & Cheng, B. S. (2010). When does benevolent leadership lead to creativity? The moderating role of creative role identity and job autonomy. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31, 106121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Liu, S., Liao, H., Gong, Y., Kammeyer-Mueller, J., & Shi, J. (2013). Can't get it out of my mind: Employee rumination after customer mistreatment and negative mood in the next morning. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 9891004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. Unpublished manuscript, University of Iowa, Iowa City.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Conceptual model.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations among variables

Figure 2

Table 2. Multilevel path analysis results for testing Hypotheses 1, 2a, and 2b

Figure 3

Figure 2. Unstandardized estimates of the path coefficients (H4–H6).

Figure 4

Figure 3. The moderating effect of benevolent leadership on the relationship between deviance and fear.

Figure 5

Table 3. Results of the cross-level moderating effects of perceived benevolent leadership

Figure 6

Figure 4. The moderating effect of benevolent leadership on the relationship between fear and OCB.