Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T18:20:42.826Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on witness's creative process engagement: roles of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 June 2021

Jie Huang
Affiliation:
School of Economics and Management, Southwest Jiaotong University, Chengdu, China
Chunyong Tang*
Affiliation:
School of Economics and Management, Southwest Jiaotong University, Chengdu, China
*
Author for correspondence: Chunyong Tang, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Emerging research in the idiosyncratic deals literature is to examine its negative effects. Thus far, much remains unknown about how and when idiosyncratic deals are associated with employee creative process engagement. Invoking fairness heuristic theory and trait activation theory, we propose and test a model that coworker's idiosyncratic deals have a negative association with witness's creative process engagement through psychological contract violation. Furthermore, we theorize and test the combination of the responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership as important boundary conditions, associate interact with coworker's idiosyncratic deals to strengthen the positive impact on psychological contract violation, thereby reducing witness's creative process engagement. We use two time-lagged studies to provide support for these mediation and moderation effects, and also discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these findings.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press and Australian and New Zealand Academy of Management 2021

Introduction

With the rapid development of sharing economy and Gig economy, comprising independent freelance workers and providing flexible working arrangements for them (e.g., Grimshaw, Cooke, Grugulis, & Vincent, Reference Grimshaw, Cooke, Grugulis and Vincent2010; Holtgrewe, Reference Holtgrewe2014; Lehdonvirta, Reference Lehdonvirta2018), as well as the emergence of public crisis events such as corona virus disease 2019 (COVID-19), enterprises are increasingly in the ‘VUCA era’ which is full of variability, uncertainty, complexity, and ambiguity (Mack, Khare, Krämer, & Burgartz, Reference Mack, Khare, Krämer and Burgartz2016). To attract valuable employees and maintain substantial competitive or innovation advantage, more organizations individually negotiated non-standardized idiosyncratic deals arrangements unique to employees, examples include flexible work hours, training opportunities, and sponsored on-the-job activities (Anand, Hu, Vidyarthi, & Liden, Reference Anand, Hu, Vidyarthi and Liden2018; Rosen, Slater, Chang, & Johnson, Reference Rosen, Slater, Chang and Johnson2013; Rousseau, Reference Rousseau2005). Then, how employees are likely to respond to the idiosyncratic deals in the creative process? This question is particularly important for organizations to promote employee innovation. Prior research shows that idiosyncratic deals reduce the recipient's turnover intentions (Ho & Tekleab, Reference Ho and Tekleab2016; Ng, Reference Ng2017) and enhance their organizational citizenship behavior (Anand et al., Reference Anand, Hu, Vidyarthi and Liden2018; Ho & Kong, Reference Ho and Kong2015; Ng & Lucianetti, Reference Ng and Lucianetti2016), job satisfaction (Liao, Wayne, Liden, & Meuser, Reference Liao, Wayne, Liden and Meuser2016), job performance (Anand et al., Reference Anand, Hu, Vidyarthi and Liden2018; Heras, Rofcanin, Bal, & Stollberger, Reference Heras, Rofcanin, Bal and Stollberger2017; Liao et al., Reference Liao, Wayne, Liden and Meuser2016), helping behavior (Liao et al., Reference Liao, Wayne, Liden and Meuser2016), and voice behavior (Marescaux, Winne, & Sels, Reference Marescaux, Winne and Sels2019; Ng & Lucianetti, Reference Ng and Lucianetti2016). However, various prior research studies acknowledge that idiosyncratic deals have negative implications in the workplace. If employee received a higher level of idiosyncratic deals, their peers would feel envious and choose to resign (Ng, Reference Ng2017). Marescaux et al. (Reference Marescaux, Winne and Sels2019) found that an employee who perceived a high level of coworker's idiosyncratic deals would complain and request compensation. These findings showed that peer's or coworker's idiosyncratic deals would trigger witness's negative psychological and behavioral, nevertheless, the empirical investigation remained limited on such perspective, how and when they are related remains little understood. In order to dig more deeply into the negative repercussions of coworker's idiosyncratic deals, other feasible mechanism investigations of the implications of perceived coworker's idiosyncratic deals for the witness's creative process engagement are necessary. In addition to this gap, prior researchers also omitted the boundary conditions of idiosyncratic deals' consequences. As Rousseau, Ho, and Greenberg (Reference Rousseau, Ho and Greenberg2006) noted, there were some conditions or scenarios (i.e., traits and working context) under which the coworker may perceive idiosyncratic deals as fair or unfair, and respond in different ways. Taken together, the question faced by organizations and the gap from previous literature provide inspiration for our present research, in which we integrate fairness heuristic theory with trait activation theory to explore a theoretical model that highlights how and when coworker's idiosyncratic deals impact witness's creative process engagement.

Fairness heuristic theory holds that employee's general fairness judgments are inspired by the fairness-relevant information (i.e., procedural and distributive information in the normal social interaction) in the workplace, which makes individual create positive/negative emotions and cognition, motivating them to concern about self-interest subsequently (Lind, Kulik, & Ambrose, Reference Lind, Kulik and Ambrose1993, Reference Lind, Kray and Thompson2001). When an employee bears witness to coworker's idiosyncratic deals, the inequity might inspire unfairness impression and give rise to psychological contract violation on account of a lack of what coworkers have. To restore the gap between an employee's and his coworker's deals, this employee may reduce creative process engagement. That is to explore the mediating effect of psychological contract violation. Trait activation theory argues that personality trait plays an increasingly important role in the workplace (Byrne, Stoner, Thompson, & Hochwarter, Reference Byrne, Stoner, Thompson and Hochwarter2005; Foo, Uy, & Baron, Reference Foo, Uy and Baron2009; Greenbaum, Hill, Mawritz, & Quade, Reference Greenbaum, Hill, Mawritz and Quade2017; Hirst, Knippenberg, & Zhou, Reference Hirst, Knippenberg and Zhou2009; Hochwarter, Witt, Treadway, & Ferris, Reference Hochwarter, Witt, Treadway and Ferris2006; Hoogh, Hartog, & Koopman, Reference Hoogh, Hartog and Koopman2005; Judge & Zapata, Reference Judge and Zapata2015; Kamdar & Van Dyne, Reference Kamdar and Van Dyne2007; Li, Liang, & Crant, Reference Li, Liang and Crant2010), traits and job characteristics jointly influence employee work outcomes (Barrick, Mount, & Li, Reference Barrick, Mount and Li2013). So our model positions responsibility for change as an important boundary condition. Moreover, trait activation theory points out that some contextual factors can conceal the effects of traits (Mischel, Reference Mischel1973, Reference Mischel1977). Fairness heuristic theory also shows that fairness-relevant information can be supplemented and strengthened mutually (Den Bos, Lind, Vermunt, & Wilke, Reference Den Bos, Lind, Vermunt and Wilke1997; Lind, Kray, & Thompson, Reference Lind, Kray and Thompson2001), so we concentrate on perceived exploitative leadership as another boundary condition, i.e., “leader acting egoistically, overburdening and manipulating employee” (Schmid, Pircher, & Peus, Reference Schmid, Pircher and Peus2019, p. 1404), as the feelings of injustice are activated when the employee feel exploited by their supervisors. Therefore, our tested model position psychological contract violation as a mediating factor that explain how coworker's idiosyncratic deals impact witness's creative process engagement, it also position responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership as combined moderating factor that determine whether coworker's idiosyncratic deals break witness's psychological contract translates into decreasing creative process engagement.

The present research makes several notable contributions to current literature through rich data collection and two studies. First, our theoretical model explains how an employee's creative process engagement is affected by the psychological state (psychological contract violation) of witnessing coworker's idiosyncratic deals, that is our model focus on an outcome variable, i.e., witness's creative process engagement, which has not been explored in idiosyncratic deals research to date. Second, our research contributes to psychological contract literatures empirically investigates its mediating effects. Third, we explain when an employee reduces creative process engagement to respond to the witnesses of coworker's idiosyncratic deals through frame responsibility for change and exploitative leadership as contingency factors, so we move research on the coworker's idiosyncratic deals – witness's creative process engagement association toward a refined moderating mechanism. Finally, we contribute to the exploitative leadership literature by advancing the moderating role of it in the creative process, which has begun to receive the attention of scholars recently (Schmid et al., Reference Schmid, Pircher and Peus2019; Syed, Akhtar, Saeed, & Husnain, Reference Syed, Akhtar, Saeed and Husnain2019).

Hypothesis development

Coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement

Creative process engagement differs from non-creative task involvement, the former entails stronger affective commitment and cognitive, to identify problems, search and encode relevant information, generate creative ideas by spending more time and effort (Amabile, Reference Amabile1983; Gilson, Mathieu, Shalley, & Ruddy, Reference Gilson, Mathieu, Shalley and Ruddy2005; Mollen & Wilson, Reference Mollen and Wilson2010; Reiter-Palmon & Illies, Reference Reiter-Palmon and Illies2004; Zhang & Bartol, Reference Zhang and Bartol2010a). These three processes (i.e., problem identification, information searching and encoding, and idea generation) of creative process engagement are essential activities to associate employee creativity in the workplace, as confirmed by extant studies (Cheung, Huang, Chang, & Wei, Reference Cheung, Huang, Chang and Wei2020; Huang, Krasikova, & Liu, Reference Huang, Krasikova and Liu2016; Mumford, Mobley, Reiter-Palmon, Uhlman, & Doares, Reference Mumford, Mobley, Reiter-Palmon, Uhlman and Doares1991; Reiter-Palmon & Illies, Reference Reiter-Palmon and Illies2004). The major research of creative process engagement focuses on its outcomes, such as creativity (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Huang, Chang and Wei2020; Henker, Sonnentag, & Unger, Reference Henker, Sonnentag and Unger2015), creative performance (Bakker & Demerouti, Reference Bakker and Demerouti2008; Mace & Ward, Reference Mace and Ward2002; Sapp, Reference Sapp1995), knowledge sharing intention (Piyathasanan, Mathies, Patterson, & De Ruyter, Reference Piyathasanan, Mathies, Patterson and De Ruyter2017), and job performance (Zhang & Bartol, Reference Zhang and Bartol2010b), but it does not account for the factors and mechanism that affect creative process engagement.

We propose that coworker's idiosyncratic deals may reduce witness's creative process engagement. A primary reason for the two constructs is that idiosyncratic deals signal the recognition of the dealer's competence and allow them to thrive, thereby enhancing the satisfaction of their competence need and organizational citizenship behavior (Ho & Kong, Reference Ho and Kong2015). Anand and colleagues maintain that the negotiability and respect in idiosyncratic deals create and sustain a high-quality leader−follower relationship, motivating an employee to engage in organizational citizenship behaviors (Anand et al., Reference Anand, Hu, Vidyarthi and Liden2018). Creative process engagement intends to identify workplace problems and generate relevant ideas, which is often considered a form of OCBs (LePine, Erez, & Johnson, Reference LePine, Erez and Johnson2002; Van Dyne & LePine, Reference Van Dyne and LePine1998). Additionally, the previous studies show that employee's idiosyncratic deals were positively related to their voice behavior, but perceptions of coworkers receiving idiosyncratic deals were the exact opposite (Ng & Lucianetti, Reference Ng and Lucianetti2016). A positive association between the voice of the individual and their creativity is also established in extant research (Chen & Hou, Reference Chen and Hou2016; Ng & Feldman, Reference Ng and Feldman2012; Zhou & George, Reference Zhou and George2001). Therefore, coworker's idiosyncratic deals may enhance coworker's creative process engagement, but decrease witness's creative process engagement.

The link between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement is best explained by the fairness heuristic theory. This theory suggests that people use fairness-relevant information, including diminished identity treatment (Huo, Smith, Tyler, & Lind, Reference Huo, Smith, Tyler and Lind1996; Lind & Tyler, Reference Lind and Tyler1988; Tyler & Lind, Reference Tyler, Lind and Zanna1992) and unequal distribution (Van den Bos, Reference Van den Bos1996; Van den Bos, Vermunt, & Wilke, Reference Van den Bos, Vermunt and Wilke1996, Reference Van den Bos, Vermunt and Wilke1997) across coworkers, to engage in the fairness heuristic processes, and to derive a general fairness impression subsequently (Lind et al., Reference Lind, Kray and Thompson2001; Tyler & DeGoey, Reference Tyler and DeGoey1995). This impression will be used to guide personal decisions and restore equity, including altering work outputs or inputs (Folger, Reference Folger1977; Folger, Rosenfield, Grove, & Corkran, Reference Folger, Rosenfield, Grove and Corkran1979; Marescaux et al., Reference Marescaux, Winne and Sels2019; Skiba & Rosenberg, Reference Skiba and Rosenberg2011; Walker, LaTour, Lind, & Thibaut, Reference Walker, LaTour, Lind and Thibaut1974). According to fairness heuristic theory, the unique coworker's idiosyncratic deals have the potential to distort the general unfair impression of witnesses, and hence lead to reduce his/her creative process engagement. To summarize, these arguments suggest that coworker's idiosyncratic deals are negatively related to witness's creative process engagement, the following hypotheses are proposed:

Hypothesis 1 Coworker's idiosyncratic deals are negatively related to witness's creative process engagement.

The mediating role of psychological contract violation

Psychological contract violation is an emotional and affective state, which refers to the feelings of betrayal when an employee perceives that the organization has failed to fulfill reciprocal obligations (Morrison & Robinson, Reference Morrison and Robinson1997; Robinson & Morrison, Reference Robinson and Morrison2000; Rousseau, Reference Rousseau1989; Schein, Reference Schein1965). Employees are more likely to feel contract violation if they were treated unfairly (Robinson & Morrison, Reference Robinson and Morrison2000). The different forms of coworker's idiosyncratic deal arrangements, such as workload reductions and flexible work hours, have the potential to break the fairness, lead to perceptions of psychological contract violation for the witnesses. Because the witnesses of coworker's idiosyncratic deals feel worse treated than their colleague, examples could be depriving training opportunities or valuable resources. Furthermore, psychological contract violation has been described as the experiences of frustration, anger, resentment, and unfairness perceptions (Ortony, Clore, & Collins, Reference Ortony, Clore and Collins1988; Robinson & Rousseau, Reference Robinson and Rousseau1994; Rousseau, Reference Rousseau1989; Schein, Reference Schein1965), these components will be witness's reaction towards the organization that granted the idiosyncratic deals for a coworker. On the other hand, psychological contract violation can lead to behavioral responses (Morrison & Robinson, Reference Morrison and Robinson1997; Pavlou & Gefen, Reference Pavlou and Gefen2005; Robinson, Reference Robinson1996; Robinson, Kraatz, & Rousseau, Reference Robinson, Kraatz and Rousseau1994; Rousseau, Reference Rousseau1989), as Robinson and Rousseau (Reference Robinson and Rousseau1994) found, psychological contract violation will decrease employee's obligation and contribution to the organization. Thus, we expect psychological contract violation to mediate the coworker's idiosyncratic deals – witness's creative process engagement relationship. This hypothesis is consistent with the results obtained by Garg and Fulmer (Reference Garg and Fulmer2017), who validated the mediating role of employee emotion (i.e., resentment, anger, and indifference) between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's behavioral reaction (i.e., display withdrawal and disengagement).

The mediation we suggest is in accordance with fairness heuristic theory, such that individual will use fairness-relevant information, including diminished identity treatment and the unfair distribution of outcomes across team members, to judge how fairly he/she is being treated (Den Bos et al., Reference Den Bos, Lind, Vermunt and Wilke1997). If the person derives a general unfair impression, he or she will feel excluded and exploited. To reduce psychological distress, individuals will invest less psychological or material resources for the organization (Lind et al., Reference Lind, Kray and Thompson2001). According to the theory, coworker's idiosyncratic deals result in witnesses perceiving distributively unfair and psychological contract violation, which trigger behavioral restoration, increasing the odds that employee minimize their creative process engagement to restore the balance of the psychological contract. The following hypotheses are formulated:

Hypothesis 2 Psychological contract violation mediates the negative relationship between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement.

The combined moderating role of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership

The coworker's idiosyncratic deals – witness's psychological contract violation link involves cognitive activities, which are affected by traits (Kong, Ho, & Garg, Reference Kong, Ho and Garg2020). Responsibility for change (Lee, Pak, Kim, & Li, Reference Lee, Pak, Kim and Li2019; Morrison & Phelps, Reference Morrison and Phelps1999) is the belief that an individual is obligated to bring about change in the workplace, Morrison and Phelps (Reference Morrison and Phelps1999) stated further that such a sense of responsibility can predict the willingness to take charge. Previous literature also proposed that employees with high responsibility for change display more proactive behaviors (Frese, Kring, Soose, & Zempel, Reference Frese, Kring, Soose and Zempel1996; Graham, Reference Graham, Gummings and Staw1986) and continuous improvement (Fuller, Marler, & Hester, Reference Fuller, Marler and Hester2006), because they tend to have a more positive valence to taking these behaviors (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Pak, Kim and Li2019). When employees with high levels of responsibility for change witness coworker's idiosyncratic deals, they may motivate or improve themselves rather than blame the organization which has not fulfilled its obligations or commitments, and feel less betrayed. The above reasoning shows high perceived responsibility for change weakens the coworker's idiosyncratic deals − witness's psychological contract violation association. In contrast, employees felt low responsibility for change may attach more negative valence to psychological contracts when they witness coworker's idiosyncratic deals, strengthens the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's psychological contract violation.

The effect of idiosyncratic deals are subject to the influence of the leader−follower context (Liao et al., Reference Liao, Wayne, Liden and Meuser2016), so the relationship between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's psychological contract violation also involves interpersonal interaction with leaders which leadership often play a vital role in. Employees often consider their immediate leader as a representative of the organization, a recent study has shown that abusive supervision negative impact on psychological contract violation and the norm of reciprocity (Blau, Reference Blau1964), since employees will feel unfair and disrespectful when perceiving them to be abusive (Pradhan, Srivastava, & Mishra, Reference Pradhan, Srivastava and Mishra2019). Likewise, exploitative leadership also belongs to destructive leadership, refers to leaders who are very selfish and exploitative of others, they often exert pressure on, place inappropriately job demands on, and manipulate (e.g., play followers off against each other) employees, and hinder employees' career development (May, Peus, & Frey, Reference May, Peus and Frey2010; Schilling, Reference Schilling2009; Stouten & Tripp, Reference Stouten and Tripp2009; Williams, Reference Williams2014). Schmid et al. (Reference Schmid, Pircher and Peus2019) have argued that exploitative leadership is likely to evoke broader negative emotions, negative cognitions, and perceptions of unequal treatment. Along the lines of argumentation and fairness heuristic theory (Den Bos et al., Reference Den Bos, Lind, Vermunt and Wilke1997), employees perceived exploitative behavior will defy the leader−follower equitable exchange relationships, and trigger a strong sense of inequity and violation (Kernan, Racicot, & Fisher, Reference Kernan, Racicot and Fisher2016). Hence, it is plausible that the positive relationship between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's psychological contract violation to be stronger in a workplace with high rather than low perceived exploitative leadership.

Employee's cognition and behavior are multiply determined (Ahadi & Diener, Reference Ahadi and Diener1989), trait activation theory assumes that understanding the behavioral expression of personality traits calls for consideration of relevant situational cues (Tett & Guterman, Reference Tett and Guterman2000), because situational effects (e.g., the effects of supervisors' behavior) can wash out trait effects (Tett & Burnett, Reference Tett and Burnett2003). So we turn our attention to explain how the combination of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership moderate the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's psychological contract violation. Our primary aim here is to assess whether the trait of responsibility for change may be underused in the context of exploitative leadership.

Earlier, we assumed an indirect association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement via psychological contract violation (Hypotheses 1 and 2). Integrating our theorization for the combined moderating role of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership in the coworker's idiosyncratic deals − witness's psychological contract violation association, we propose that the combinations of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership moderate the indirect association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement (via psychological contract violation). For the combination of low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership, the conditional indirect effect of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on witness's creative process engagement via psychological contract violation was significant and strongest because both low responsibility and high exploitative situational will strengthen the relationship between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's psychological contract violation, but for other combinations, including low responsibility and low exploitative leadership, high responsibility and high exploitative leadership, and high responsibility and low exploitative leadership, the conditional indirect effects were nonsignificant because the exploitative situation cannot activate the effect of responsibility among these three combinations. In summary, we suggest the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 3 The combinations of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership moderates the indirect association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement via psychological contract violation, such that the indirect association is significant and strongest when employees with low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership.

Overview of the studies

To increase confidence in our results (Lykken, Reference Lykken1968), two separate studies were conducted to test our theoretically grounded hypotheses and conceptual models. In study 1, we tested the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement and psychological contract violation as a mechanism linking coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement. In study 2, we replicated the results of study 1 and tested the combined moderating role of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership. To minimize common method bias (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee and Podsakoff2003) and enhance the generalizability of our findings, we used a 1-month time lag and 3-wave design in study 1, and a 2-month interval and 3-wave design in study 2. Empirically, a 1-month time lag has been shown to be long enough for average correlations to be lower than in concurrent conditions (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, & Podsakoff, Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie and Podsakoff2012). Yet, because creative engagement likely require several weeks to be executed (Madrid, Patterson, Birdi, Leiva, & Kausel, Reference Madrid, Patterson, Birdi, Leiva and Kausel2014), a 2-month time lag was adopted in study 2. This replication logic followed across the studies is consistent with recent recommendations from method experts to test theoretical models via improved and independent empirical studies (Cortina, Aguinis, & DeShon, Reference Cortina, Aguinis and DeShon2017). The samples of the two studies were employees working in different organizations in China. To ensure equivalency of meaning, all of the measures were translated from English to Chinese following Brislin (Reference Brislin, Lonner and Berry1986) translation and back-translation procedure.

Study 1: Methods

Sample and procedure

This research does n't involve sensitive information, respondents were informed that individual responses would be strictly confidential and only for academic research. Time-lagged dates were surveyed with part-time MBA students (i.e., communication and technology, finance, real estate, engineering) through an experiment and investigation class at a large public university in Sichuan Province, China. Performance on this survey directly contributed (20%) to parties' course grades, so they were motivated to participate in survey tasks, and received RMB 20 (approximately USD 3) for each wave survey. 351 participants should complete three wave surveys according to the roster, a QR code of the questionnaires was provided to them and directed them to report coworker's idiosyncratic deals, control variable and demographic information in the Time 1 survey, we received 344 usable questionnaires (98%). One month later, followers rated their psychological contract violation, we received 332 usable questionnaires (94%). Creative process engagement was reported in the Time 3 survey, we received 321 usable questionnaires (91%). The final sample consisted of 321 matched responses, the average age of whom was 27, about 63.6% were female, 64.2% were at least Bachelor's degree holders, 60.1% had non-management positions, their average working tenure was 6 years.

Measures

Respondents were asked to rate their coworkers or themselves on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Coworker's idiosyncratic deals was assessed using the 5-item scale from Anand et al. (Reference Anand, Hu, Vidyarthi and Liden2018), sample items include ‘Some coworker have more opportunities to participate in on-the-job activities than me’ and ‘Some coworker have more opportunities to participate in training than me’ (α = .942) (p. 702).

Psychological contract violation was assessed by the 5-item scale developed by Robinson and Morrison (Reference Robinson and Morrison2000), sample items include ‘Almost all the promises made by my employer during recruitment have been kept so far (reversed)’ and ‘My employer has broken many of its promises to me even though I’ ve upheld myside of the deal’ (α = .921) (p. 539).

Creative process engagement was measured with 11-item scale introduced by Zhang and Bartol (Reference Zhang and Bartol2010a, Reference Zhang and Bartolb), sample items are ‘I spend considerable time trying to understand the nature of the problem’ and ‘I think about the problem from multiple perspectives’ (α = .963)(p. 128).

Further, based on the previous literatures (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Huang, Chang and Wei2020), we included four demographic variables (working tenure, education, post category, and organizational level) as control variables. Over qualified employees have the ability to carry out divergent thinking, associational and analogical abilities, so as to promote the creative process (Livingstone, Nelson, & Barr, Reference Livingstone, Nelson and Barr1997), other scholars also argued that employees with over-qualification will use their surplus qualification constructively by engaging in creative process (Luksyte & Spitzmueller, Reference Luksyte and Spitzmueller2016), so we included participants' perceived over-qualification as a control variable. It was measured with 9-item scale introduced by Maynard, Joseph, and Maynard (Reference Maynard, Joseph and Maynard2006), sample items are ‘My job requires less education than I have’ and ‘The work experience that I have is not necessary to be successful on this job’ (α = .86) (p. 536).

Results

Reliability and validity analyses

Before testing our hypotheses, we first evaluated Cronbach's α or C.R. to test the overall or composite reliability and evaluated AVE to test the convergent validity of three variables (coworker's idiosyncratic deals, psychological contract violation, and creative process engagement) by using SPSS 24 software, these results in Table 1 showed that three scales' reliability and convergent validity met the statistical standard (all Cronbach's α > .8, C.R. >.7, and AVE >.5).

Table 1. Reliability and validity of study 1

Confirmatory factor analyses

We conducted a CFA using MPLUS software of the three variables in our model: Coworker's idiosyncratic deals (5 items), psychological contract violation (5 items), and creative process engagement (11 items), Table 2 indicated an acceptable three-factor measurement model fitted the data well (χ2/df <3, RMSEA <.08, IFI, TLI, CFI >.9). Importantly, the three-factor model also revealed a better fit than the alternative models, including two- and one-factor models. Taken together, these results support the distinctiveness of our hypothesized constructs.

Table 2. Confirmatory factor analyses of study 1

Note. Cid = coworker's idiosyncratic deals; Pcv = psychological contract violation; Cpe = witness's creative process engagement.

Descriptive statistics

Table 3 displays the descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, and correlations) among all variables, The findings presented that coworker's idiosyncratic deals had a statistically significantly positively association with witness's psychological contract violation (B = .437, p < .01) and a negative association with witness's creative process engagement (B = −.152, p < .01). Psychological contract violation was negatively associated with creative process engagement (B = −.176, p < .01).

Table 3. Means, standard deviations, and correlations of study 1

Note. ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05.

Hypothesis tests

As shown in Table 4 and Figure 1, we used Hayes (Reference Hayes2013) PROCESS macro to test the direct and indirect effects of our full model. Coworker's idiosyncratic deals was negatively related to witness's creative process engagement (B = −.209, SE = .056, p < .01) and positively related to witness's psychological contract violation (B = .371, SE = .051, p < .001), witness's psychological contract violation was negatively related to his/her creative process engagement (B = −.211, SE = .061, p < .01), so the mediating effect of psychological contract violation was significant (B = .371 × .211 = .078, p < .01). We also used a bootstrapping procedure (5,000 resamples) and set the confidence intervals at .95, the results revealed that the indirect effect of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on witness's creative process engagement through psychological contract violation equals .078 (Boot SE = .027, 95% CI [.031, .136]), thereby provided evidence for a significant mediation effect. These results supported Hypotheses 1 and 2.

Table 4. Mediation analyses of study 1

Note. ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05.

Figure 1. Study 1 tested model.

Discussion

Study 1 provided evidence coworker's idiosyncratic deals − witness's creative process engagement association occurs via psychological contract violation. Higher levels of coworker's idiosyncratic deals would result in higher levels of witness's psychological contract violation, which would result in lower levels of creative process engagement.

To rule out the specificity of the sample or measure (Cortina et al., Reference Cortina, Aguinis and DeShon2017), in the following section, we designed another independent study (study 2) to constructively replicate the results of study 1 and test the combined moderating role of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership.

Study 2: Methods

Sample and procedure

As a part of a large consulting project, through alumni and other social networks, we surveyed time-lagged data from employees working at six large privately owned firms in the High-tech Zone located in Chengdu and Shenzhen, China. These full-time employees worked in different industries, involving computer technicians, finance and accounting, and manufacturing, among others. Specifically, we entrust the HRM department to invite the employee to complete three waves of questionnaires at two-month intervals and count the list of volunteers, participants were paid RMB 20 (approximately USD 3) for each wave survey. According to the list, we sent prospective participants an email with a link to the online questionnaire and explained the purpose and confidentiality of this study to them. Since creative behaviors likely require several weeks to generate and execute (Madrid et al., Reference Madrid, Patterson, Birdi, Leiva and Kausel2014), we conducted a 2-month interval in this study rather than a 1-month interval in study 1. After removed careless responders, 485 participants reported demographic information, control variable, coworker's idiosyncratic deals, responsibility for change, and perceived exploitative leadership at time 1, 462 of the time 1 participant reported their psychological contract violation at time 2 (95.2%). Finally, 403 of the time 2 participants reported their creative process engagement at time 3 (83%). Of these 403 matched respondents, the average age was 28, average organizational tenure was 5 years, 61.5% were female, 63.1% had at least a bachelor's degree, and about 57.3% had non-management positions.

Measures

As in study 1, we used the same items and 7-point scale anchors to rate coworker's idiosyncratic deals (α = .931), psychological contract violation (α = .921), and creative process engagement (α = .96).

The employees also rated their responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership using a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree), responsibility for change was measured with 3-item developed by Morrison and Phelps (Reference Morrison and Phelps1999), a sample item is ‘I feel a personal sense of responsibility to bring about change at work’ (α = .898) (p. 411).

Perceived exploitative leadership was assessed using Schmid et al. (Reference Schmid, Pircher and Peus2019) 14-item scale, a sample item is ‘My leader takes it for granted that my work can be used for his or her benefit’ (α = .963) (p. 1407).

As in study 1, we also controlled for working tenure, education, post category, organizational level, and perceived over-qualification.

Results

Reliability and validity analyses

As the same strategies in study 1, we conducted reliability and validity analyses, the results showed that five scales' overall reliability, composite reliability, and convergent validity met the statistical standard (all Cronbach’ s α > .8, C.R. >.7, and AVE >.5) (Table 5).

Table 5. Reliability and validity of study 2

Confirmatory factor analyses

We subjected the five variables in Figure 2 to a series of CFAs to compare our hypothesized five-factor model with alternative constructs. As shown in Table 6, an acceptable five-factor measurement model fitted the data well (χ2/df <3, RMSEA <.08, IFI, TLI, CFI >.9), this baseline model also revealed a better fit than the alternative models, supporting the distinctiveness of our hypothesized model.

Figure 2. Study 2 tested model.

Table 6. Confirmatory factor analyses of study 2

Note. Cid = coworker's idiosyncratic deals; Pcv = psychological contract violation; Rc = responsibility for change; Pel = perceived exploitative leadership; Cpe = witness's creative process engagement.

Descriptive statistics

Table 7 reported the means, standard deviations, and correlations of the study’ s key variables, the conclusion of study 1 was confirmed again, and responsibility for change was significantly positively associated with creative process engagement (B = .756, p < .01), perceived exploitative leadership was negatively associated with creative process engagement (B = −.111, p < .05).

Table 7. Means, standard deviations, and correlations of study 2

Note. ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05.

Hypothesis tests

We also used PROCESS macro to test the mediation and moderation effect shown in Figure 2, the results are presented in Table 8. Coworker's idiosyncratic deals were negatively associated with witness's creative process engagement (B = −.282, SE = .056, p < .001) and positively associated with witness's psychological contract violation (B = .117, SE = .057, p < .05), witness's psychological contract violation was negatively associated with his/her creative process engagement (B = −.140, SE = .057, p < .05), the mediating effect of psychological contract violation was significant (B = .117 × .140 = .016, p < .05), thereby provided support for Hypotheses 1 and 2 again.

Table 8. Moderation analyses of study 2

Note. ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05.

We found the interaction effect of coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's responsibility for change on witness's psychological contract violation is negative and significant (B = −.125, SE = .038, p < .01, see Table 8), results of simple slopes analysis showed the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's psychological contract violation was positive and significant (B = .242, SE = .067, 95% CI [.110, .375]) when responsibility for change was low (M-1SD), it became nonsignificant (B = −.007, SE = .071, 95% CI [−.148, .133]) when responsibility for change was high. As the same analysis strategies, the interaction effect of coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's perceived exploitative leadership on witness's psychological contract violation equals .133 (SE = .033, p < .01, see Table 8), simple slopes analysis showed the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's psychological contract violation was positive and significant (B = .250, SE = .058, 95% CI [.135, .365]) when witness's perceived exploitative leadership was high (M + 1SD), it became nonsignificant (B = −.015, SE = .073, 95% CI [−.160, .129]) when perceived exploitative leadership was low.

We tested the conditional indirect effects of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on witness's creative process engagement at plus and minus one SD on witness's responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership, these results were shown in Table 9 and Figure 3. For the combination of low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership, the indirect effect of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on witness's creative process engagement through psychological contract violation was significant (B = .052, BootSE = .024, 95% CI [.011, .106]), but for all other combination of witness's responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership, the conditional indirect effects were nonsignificant. The indirect effect associated with low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership was stronger than the indirect effect for the association of low responsibility for change and low perceived exploitative leadership (B difference = .037, BootSE = .016, 95% CI [.008, .073]), high responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership (B difference = −.035, BootSE = .016, 95% CI [−.072, −.007]), and high responsibility for change and low perceived exploitative leadership (B difference = −.072, BootSE = .031, 95% CI [−.140, −.017]). Hypothesis 3 was supported, the conditional indirect effect of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on witness's creative process engagement via witness's psychological contract violation was significant and strongest when employees were low in responsibility for change and high in perceived exploitative leadership.

Table 9. Combined moderation of Rc and Pel of study 2

Note. Cid = coworker's idiosyncratic deals; Pcv = psychological contract violation; Rc = responsibility for change; Pel = perceived exploitative leadership; Cpe = witness's creative process engagement.

Figure 3. Combined moderating effect of study 2.

Discussion

Study 2 replicated the mediating effect of psychological contract violation found in study 1, Hypotheses 1 and 2 were supported again. Study 2 also indicated that the indirect effects of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on witness's creative process engagement was significant and strengthened at the combination of low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership. In contrast, the indirect effects were nonsignificant at the combinations of low responsibility for change and low perceived exploitative leadership, high responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership, high responsibility for change and low perceived exploitative leadership. Together, these results showed that the damages of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on the witness's psychological contract may be connected with creative process engagement only when employees have low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership.

General discussion

We explored and tested the theoretical connections between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement, proposing the mediating mechanism of psychological contract violation and the moderating mechanism of the combination of low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership. Through the lens of the fairness heuristic theory and the trait activation theory, converging evidence showed that coworker's idiosyncratic deals break witness's psychological contract, which reduces witness's creative process engagement when witnesses have low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership.

Theoretical implication

The current paper makes several notable contributions to the literature. First, as one of the few studies of coworker's idiosyncratic deals, we examined how it reduces witness's creative process engagement. Drawing on the fairness heuristic theory (i.e., Lind et al., Reference Lind, Kray and Thompson2001; Tyler & DeGoey, Reference Tyler and DeGoey1995), our finding is that psychological contract violation connects coworker's idiosyncratic deals with witness's creative process engagement shows that cognitive activities (i.e., psychological contract violation) can explain how coworker's idiosyncratic deals lower witness's creative process engagement in the workplace. From our perspective, the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement is very interesting and worthwhile. Our study extends the understanding of the negative influence of coworker's idiosyncratic deals in the small body of literature. Although previous studies have investigated an array of destructive effects of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on witness's outcomes, such as resulting in witness's turnover (Ng, Reference Ng2017) and deviant behaviors (Kong et al., Reference Kong, Ho and Garg2020), these impressive literature on coworker's idiosyncratic deals have ignored its influence on creative endeavors, including problem identification, information searching and encoding, and idea generation, known as creative process engagement (i.e., Amabile, Reference Amabile1983; Gilson et al., Reference Gilson, Mathieu, Shalley and Ruddy2005; Mollen & Wilson, Reference Mollen and Wilson2010). This limitation is regrettable and unfortunate because coworker's idiosyncratic deals occur frequently in real organizational settings, and may be associated with other organizational behaviors (Garg & Fulmer, Reference Garg and Fulmer2017). To address this omission, we present a model that integrates coworker's idiosyncratic deals with witness's creative process engagement. To have a comprehensive understanding of the destructive effects of coworker's idiosyncratic deals, we hope that this study and other pioneering research studies in the idiosyncratic deals arena will inspire further coworker's idiosyncratic deals research in diverse contexts.

Second, our study focuses on the mediating role of psychological contract violation, linking coworker's idiosyncratic deals to witness's creative process engagement, which adds to the explanatory mechanisms related to coworker's idiosyncratic deals. As prior research studies have noted, psychological contract violation has numerous insights into employee behavior. It is likely to reduce employees' job performance (Turnley & Feldman, Reference Turnley and Feldman1999), job satisfaction (Turnley & Feldman, Reference Turnley and Feldman2000), and organizational citizenship behavior (Robinson & Rousseau, Reference Robinson and Rousseau1994), lead to higher levels of counterproductive behavior and increased intent to exit (Lemire & Rouillard, Reference Lemire and Rouillard2005; Rusbult, Farrell, Rogers, & Mainus, Reference Rusbult, Farrell, Rogers and Mainus1988). Therefore, aside from linking coworker's idiosyncratic deals with witness's creative process engagement, psychological contract violation represents an important mediation through which one's creativity and creative behavior might be lowered by exposure to unfair treatment, which can be widely applied to other similar relationships in future studies.

Third, even though the interrelation between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and outcomes has been suggested in recent years, studies linking these two phenomena have mainly focused on the mediating mechanisms (i.e., Kong et al., Reference Kong, Ho and Garg2020; Ng, Reference Ng2017). What is less known is when coworker's idiosyncratic deals predict outcomes. Our paper contributes to restoring this gap by enriching the boundary conditions that influence the coworker's idiosyncratic deals − witness's creative process engagement association. We adopted the trait activation theory and fairness heuristic theory and found that coworker's idiosyncratic deals play a stronger role in affecting creative process engagement (via psychological contract violation) when an employee with low responsibility for change and high perceived exploitative leadership opens up a research avenue on the context in coworker's idiosyncratic deals to explain when the damages of coworker's idiosyncratic deals are more likely to manifest in the witness's creative process. Our examination of the combined boundary of these two factors consists of the work environment literature, which proposes that not only personality traits matter but also leadership (Agervold & Mikkelsen, Reference Agervold and Mikkelsen2004; Francioli et al., Reference Francioli, Conway, Hansen, Holten, Grynderup, Persson and Høgh2018). Specifically, we provide a clearer picture of the effect of exploitative leadership on employees with different responsibilities for change within the coworker's idiosyncratic deals − witness's psychological contract violation − witness's creative engagement process. Employees with high responsibility for change are more willing to bring about change in the workplace (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Pak, Kim and Li2019; Morrison & Phelps, Reference Morrison and Phelps1999). So they may not be as significantly impacted by coworker's idiosyncratic deals compared with low responsibility employees within a low exploitative leadership climate. On the other hand, low responsibility employees who are seldom thought to just take charge of their deals may result in more psychological contract violation when operating that ultimately reduces creative process engagement within a high exploitative leadership climate.

Finally, an exploitative approach to leadership that has emerged in recent years, is called exploitative leadership, which has a more negative impact on individuals and organizations than other destructive leadership (Syed et al., Reference Syed, Akhtar, Saeed and Husnain2019). Although research into exploitative leadership has developed a scale to measure its construct, we know very little about the impact of it on follower outcomes, so researchers suggested exploitative leadership as a potential area of research (Schmid et al., Reference Schmid, Pircher and Peus2019). This research contributes to the exploitative leadership literature by showing how exploitative leadership matters to employees' psychological contract and creative process engagement. We find that the combination of perceived exploitative leadership and responsibility for change moderates the indirect association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement demonstrates perceived exploitative leadership is an organizational barrier that unleashes the damage of coworker's idiosyncratic deals on the witness's creative process (via psychological contract violation). By showing the combined moderating role of them, this study pioneers the further probing into the harmful role of exploitative leadership on employee creative endeavors.

Managerial implications

Granting idiosyncratic deals to coworkers may produce dysfunctional consequences for witnesses, triggering psychological contract violation and consequently reducing creative process engagement in the workplace. Insofar as coworker's idiosyncratic deals may elicit witness's perceptions of a violation, managers should break the resource barriers between different functional departments (Mawritz, Dust, & Resick, Reference Mawritz, Dust and Resick2014; Zhang & Bednall, Reference Zhang and Bednall2016), ensure all employees have the same opportunity to negotiate idiosyncratic deals under similar situation and uphold transparency and justice of the criteria used to grant idiosyncratic deals. Through these strategies, organizations can encourage employees to undertake responsibility for change, employees with high responsibility will be more willing to engage in the creative process. On the other hand, managers may reduce the negative impacts of coworker's idiosyncratic deals by creating a supportive and developing rather than exploitative leadership climate where individuals with different levels of responsibility for change could view coworker's idiosyncratic deals from a more positive perspective. Specifically, supervisors need to provide learning opportunities and support followers' long-term development. The organization could help managers to obtain a more clear view of themselves by using 360-degree feedback (Dame & Gedmin, Reference Dame and Gedmin2013). On this basis, training in self-reflection will help supervisors to understand the interdependence with subordinates (Nesbit, Reference Nesbit2012). It is also useful that the indicators and features of exploitative leadership are included in structured interviews and situational judgment tests of leadership behavior, as a valid tool for leader selection (Peus, Braun, and Frey, Reference Peus, Braun and Frey2013). Furthermore, supportive and developing leadership is critical and should be jointly developed alongside any efforts to uphold the justice of idiosyncratic deals.

Limitation and future research directions

The current study has several limitations that should be mentioned for future research. First, despite we collected time-lagged data, the causality in the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals, witness's psychological contract violation, and creative process engagement could not be established. Future research could use scenario or field experiments to provide more evidence of causality. Second, in this study, we took a unique measure of the creative process engagement and is consistent with previous studies (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Huang, Chang and Wei2020; Zhang & Bartol, Reference Zhang and Bartol2010a, Reference Zhang and Bartolb), but the relation between antecedents and the three stages of it (i.e., problem identification, information searching and encoding, and idea generation) may be different according to the results obtained by Henker et al. (Reference Henker, Sonnentag and Unger2015). Future scholars could provide a closer look at how other antecedents are related to the different stages of the creative process or illustrate the importance of the different stages within the creative process as antecedents of employee outcomes. Third, although we examined psychological contract violation as the mediating mechanism in the coworker's idiosyncratic deals − witness's creative process engagement link, other mediators may exist. For example, Ho and Kong (Reference Ho and Kong2015) identified leader−member exchange and competence need satisfaction as two mechanisms linking idiosyncratic deals and organizational citizenship behavior. Future research could examine multiple mediating processes of other factors simultaneously, to the exam which mechanism is more important (Ng, Reference Ng2017). Fourth, we examined the combination of responsibility for change and perceived exploitative leadership as critical boundary conditions in the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals, witness's psychological contract violation, and creative process engagement. Future research could examine whether other factors, such as employees' regulatory focus (Brockner, Paruchuri, Idson, & Higgins, Reference Brockner, Paruchuri, Idson and Higgins2002), strengthen or weaken the association between coworker's idiosyncratic deals and witness's creative process engagement. Fifth, exploitative leadership may also inform other negative implications, such as job satisfaction, workplace deviance, and burnout (Schmid et al., Reference Schmid, Pircher and Peus2019). For future scholarship, it would be interesting to examine the mediating mechanisms between exploitative leadership and these work outcomes. And these associations may be impacted by employees' attribution and expectation about the qualities of supervisors (Peus, Braun, & Frey, Reference Peus, Braun and Frey2012; Schyns & Schilling, Reference Schyns and Schilling2013). Finally, both of our samples are collected in China, cultural factors (i.e., power distance) may influence our findings (Hofstede, Reference Hofstede2001). Future research could investigate data in certain national cultures to extend generalizability. Interesting research in this perspective refers to unfair idiosyncratic deals and exploitative leadership may be the norm.

Financial support

This study was funded by grants from National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant nos. 71832007; 71572157).

References

Agervold, M., & Mikkelsen, E. G. (2004). Relationships between bullying, work environment and individual stress reactions. Work and Stress, 18(4), 336351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahadi, S., & Diener, E. (1989). Multiple determinants and effect size. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(3), 398406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(2), 357376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anand, S., Hu, J., Vidyarthi, P., & Liden, R. C. (2018). Leader-member exchange as a linking pin in the idiosyncratic deals-performance relationship in workgroups. The Leadership Quarterly, 29(6), 698708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakker, A. B., & Demerouti, E. (2008). Towards a model of work engagement. Career Development International, 13(3), 209223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Li, N. (2013). The theory of purposeful work behavior: The role of personality, higher-order goals, and job characteristics. Academy of Management Review, 38(1), 132153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brislin, R. W. (1986). The wording and translation of research instruments. In Lonner, W. J., & Berry, J. W. (Eds.), Field Methods in Cross-Cultural Research (pp. 137164). Beverly Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
Brockner, J., Paruchuri, S., Idson, L. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Regulatory focus and the probability estimates of conjunctive and disjunctive events. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 87(1), 524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, Z. S., Stoner, J., Thompson, K. R., & Hochwarter, W. (2005). The interactive effects of conscientiousness, work effort, and psychological climate on job performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66(2), 326338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, A. S., & Hou, Y. (2016). The effects of ethical leadership, voice behavior and climates for innovation on creativity: A moderated mediation examination. Leadership Quarterly, 27(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, S. Y., Huang, E. G., Chang, S., & Wei, L. (2020). Does being mindful make people more creative at work? The role of creative process engagement and perceived leader humility. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 159(6), 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cortina, J. M., Aguinis, H., & DeShon, R. P. (2017). Twilight of Dawn or of evening? A century of research methods. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(3), 274290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dame, J., & Gedmin, J. (2013). Six principles for developing humility as a leader. Harvard Business Review. Retrieved from https://hbr.org/2013/09/sixprinciples-for-developing.Google Scholar
Den Bos, K. V., Lind, E. A., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. (1997). How do I judge my outcome when I do not know the outcome of others? The psychology of the fair process effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(5), 10341046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: Combined impact of “voice” and improvement on experienced inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(2), 108119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folger, R., Rosenfield, D., Grove, J., & Corkran, L. (1979). Effects of “voice” and peer opinions on responses to inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(12), 22532261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foo, M. D., Uy, M. A., & Baron, R. A. (2009). How do feelings influence effort? An empirical study of entrepreneurs’ effect and venture effort. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(4), 10861094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francioli, L. L., Conway, P. M., Hansen, ÅM, Holten, A., Grynderup, M. B., Persson, R., … Høgh, A. (2018). Quality of leadership and workplace bullying: The mediating role of social community at work in a two-year follow-up study. Journal of Business Ethics, 147(4), 889899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frese, M., Kring, W., Soose, A., & Zempel, J. (1996). Personal initiative at work: Differences between East and West Germany. Academy of Management Journal, 39(1), 3763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller, J. B., Marler, L. E., & Hester, K. (2006). Promoting felt responsibility for constructive change and proactive behavior: Exploring aspects of an elaborated model of work design. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 10891120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garg, S., & Fulmer, I. (2017). Ideal or an ordeal for organizations?: The spectrum of co-worker reactions to idiosyncratic deals. Organizational Psychology Review, 7(4), 281305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilson, L. L., Mathieu, J. E., Shalley, C. E., & Ruddy, T. M. (2005). Creativity and standardization: Complementary or conflicting drivers of team effectiveness? Academy of Management Journal, 48(3), 521531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J. W. (1986). Principled organizational dissent: A theoretical essay. In Gummings, L. L. & Staw, B. M. (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (vol. 8: pp. 152). Greenwich, CT: JAI.Google Scholar
Greenbaum, R. L., Hill, A., Mawritz, M. B., & Quade, M. J. (2017). Employee machiavellianism to unethical behavior: The role of abusive supervision as a trait activator. Journal of Management, 43(2), 585609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimshaw, D., Cooke, F., Grugulis, I., & Vincent, S. (2010). New technology and changing organisational forms: Implications for managerial control and skills. New Technology, Work and Employment, 17(3), 186203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Journal of Educational Measurement, 51(3), 335337.Google Scholar
Henker, N., Sonnentag, S., & Unger, D. (2015). Transformational leadership and employee creativity: The mediating role of promotion focus and creative process engagement. Journal of Business and Psychology, 30(2), 235247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heras, M. L., Rofcanin, Y., Bal, P. M., & Stollberger, J. (2017). How do flexibility i-deals relate to work performance? Exploring the roles of family performance and organizational context. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 38(8), 12801294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirst, G., Knippenberg, D. V., & Zhou, J. (2009). A cross-level perspective on employee creativity: goal orientation, team learning behavior, and individual creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 52(2), 280293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, V. T., & Kong, D. T. (2015). Exploring the signaling function of idiosyncratic deals and their interaction. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 131(1), 149161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, V. T., & Tekleab, A. G. (2016). A model of idiosyncratic deal-making and attitudinal outcomes. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 31(3), 642656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hochwarter, W. A., Witt, L. A., Treadway, D. C., & Ferris, G. R. (2006). The interaction of social skill and organizational support on job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(2), 482489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture's consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Holtgrewe, U. (2014). New new technologies: The future and the present of work in information and communication technology. New Technology Work & Employment, 29(1), 924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoogh, A. H. B. D., Hartog, D. N. D., & Koopman, P. L. (2005). Linking the five-factors of personality to charismatic and transactional leadership; perceived dynamic work environment as a moderator. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26(7), 839865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, L., Krasikova, D. V., & Liu, D. (2016). I can do it, so can you: The role of leader creative self-efficacy in facilitating follower creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 132(2), 4962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huo, Y. J., Smith, H. J., Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1996). Superordinate identification, subgroup identification, and justice concerns: Is separatism the problem, is assimilation the answer? Psychological Science, 7(1), 4045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Judge, T. A., & Zapata, C. P. (2015). The person-situation debate revisited: effect of situation strength and trait activation on the validity of the big five personality traits in predicting job performance. Academy of Management Journal, 58(4), 11491170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamdar, D., & Van Dyne, L. (2007). The joint effects of personality and workplace social exchange relationships in predicting task performance and citizenship performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(5), 12861298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kernan, M. C., Racicot, B. M., & Fisher, A. M. (2016). Effects of abusive supervision, psychological climate, and felt violation on work outcomes: A moderated mediated model. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 23(3), 309321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kong, D. T., Ho, V., & Garg, S. (2020). Employee and coworker idiosyncratic deals: Implications for emotional exhaustion and deviant behaviors. Journal of Business Ethics, 164(2), 593609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, H. W., Pak, J., Kim, S., & Li, L. (2019). Effects of human resource management systems on employee proactivity and group innovation. Journal of Management, 45(2), 819846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehdonvirta, V. (2018). Flexibility in the gig economy: Managing time on three online piecework platforms. New Technology, Work and Employment, 33(1), 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemire, L., & Rouillard, C. (2005). An empirical exploration of psychological contract violation and individual behaviour: The case of Canadian federal civil servants in Quebec. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 20(2), 150163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LePine, J. A., Erez, A., & Johnson, D. E. (2002). The nature and dimensionality of organizational citizenship behavior: A critical review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(1), 5265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, N., Liang, J., & Crant, J. M. (2010). The role of proactive personality in job satisfaction and organizational citizenship behavior: A relational perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(2), 395404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, C. W., Wayne, S. J., Liden, R. C., & Meuser, J. D. (2016). Idiosyncratic deals and individual effectiveness: The moderating role of leader-member exchange differentiation. The Leadership Quarterly, 28(3), 438450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lind, E. A., Kray, L., & Thompson, L. (2001). Primacy effects in justice judgments: Testing predictions from fairness heuristic theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85(2), 189210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lind, E. A., Kulik, C. T., & Ambrose, M. (1993). Individual and corporate dispute resolution: Using procedural fairness as a decision heuristic. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38(2), 224251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livingstone, L. P., Nelson, D. L., & Barr, S. H. (1997). Person-environment fit and creativity: An examination of supply-value and demand-ability versions of fit. Journal of Management, 23(2), 119146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luksyte, A., & Spitzmueller, C. (2016). When are overqualified employees creative? It depends on contextual factors. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 37(5), 635653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lykken, D. (1968). Statistical significance in psychological research. Psychological Bulletin, 70(3), 151159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mace, M. A., & Ward, T. (2002). Modeling the creative process: A grounded theory analysis of creativity in the domain of art making. Creativity Research Journal, 14(2), 179192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, O., Khare, A., Krämer, A., & Burgartz, T. (2016). Managing in a VUCA world. Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madrid, H. P., Patterson, M., Birdi, K., Leiva, P. I., & Kausel, E. E. (2014). The role of weekly high-activated positive mood, context, and personality in innovative work behavior: A multilevel and interactional model. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 35(2), 234256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marescaux, E., Winne, S. D., & Sels, L. (2019). Idiosyncratic deals from a distributive justice perspective: Examining co-workers’ voice behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 154(1), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mawritz, M. B., Dust, S. B., & Resick, C. J. (2014). Hostile climate, abusive supervision, and employee coping: Does conscientiousness matter? Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(4), 737747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, D., Peus, C., & Frey, D. (2010). Negative Führung: Merkmale, Konsequenzen und Bezüge zu Konstruktiver Führung [Negative leadership: Characteristics, consequences and relation to constructive leadership]. Presented at the 7th Workshop of the Section for Work and Organizational Psychology of the German Psychological Society, Münster, Germany.Google Scholar
Maynard, D. C., Joseph, T. A., & Maynard, A. M. (2006). Underemployment, job attitudes, and turnover intentions. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(4), 509536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review, 80(4), 252283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W. (1977). The interaction of person and situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mollen, A., & Wilson, H. (2010). Engagement, telepresence and interactivity in online consumer experience: Reconciling scholastic and managerial perspectives. Journal of Business Research, 63(9), 919925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, E. W., & Phelps, C. (1999). Taking charge at work: Extra role efforts to initiate workplace change. Academy of Management Journal, 42(4), 403419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, E. W., & Robinson, S. L. (1997). When employees feel betrayed: A model of how psychological contract violation develops. Academy of Management Review, 22(1), 226256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Reiter-Palmon, R., Uhlman, C. E., & Doares, L. M. (1991). Process analytic models of creative capacities. Creativity Research Journal, 4(2), 91122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nesbit, P. L. (2012). The role of self-reflection, emotional management of feedback, and self-regulation processes in self-directed leadership development. Human Resource Development Review, 11(2), 203226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ng, T. W. H. (2017). Can idiosyncratic deals promote perceptions of competitive climate, felt ostracism, and turnover? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 99(4), 118131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2012). Employee voice behavior: A meta-analytic test of the conservation of resources framework. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33(2), 216234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ng, T. W. H., & Lucianetti, L. (2016). Goal striving, idiosyncratic deals, and job behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 37(1), 4160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emofions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavlou, P. A., & Gefen, D. (2005). Psychological contract violation in online marketplaces: Antecedents, consequences, and moderating role. Information Systems Research, 16(4), 372399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peus, C., Braun, S., & Frey, D. (2012). Despite leaders’ good intentions? The role of follower attributions in adverse leadership − A multilevel model. Journal of Psychology/Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 220(4), 241250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peus, C., Braun, S., & Frey, D. (2013). Situation-based measurement of the full range of leadership model: Development and validation of a situational judgment test. The Leadership Quarterly, 24(5), 777795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piyathasanan, B., Mathies, C., Patterson, P. G., & De Ruyter, K. (2017). Continued value creation in crowdsourcing from creative process engagement. Journal of Services Marketing, 32(1), 1933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2012). Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 539569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pradhan, S., Srivastava, A., & Mishra, D. K. (2019). Abusive supervision and knowledge hiding: The mediating role of psychological contract violation and supervisor directed aggression. Journal of Knowledge Management, 24(2), 216234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiter-Palmon, R., & Illies, J. J. (2004). Leadership and creativity: Understanding leadership from a creative problem-solving perspective. The Leadership Quarterly, 15(1), 5577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, S. L. (1996). Trust and breach of the psychological contract. Administrative Science Quarterly, 41(4), 574599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, S. L., Kraatz, M. S., & Rousseau, D. M. (1994). Changing obligations and the psychological contract: A longitudinal study. Academy of Management Journal, 37(1), 137152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, S. L., & Morrison, E. W. (2000). The development of psychological contract breach and violation: A longitudinal study. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21(5), 525546.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, S. L., & Rousseau, D. M. (1994). Violating the psychological contract: Not the exception but the norm. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15(3), 245259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosen, C. C., Slater, D. J., Chang, C. D., & Johnson, R. E. (2013). Let's make a deal: Development and validation of the ex-post i-deals scale. Journal of Management, 39(3), 709742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rousseau, D. M. (1989). Psychological and implied contracts in organizations. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 2(2), 121139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rousseau, D. M. (2005). I-deals: Idiosyncratic deals employees bargain for themselves. Armonk, NY: M.E. Sharpe, Inc.Google Scholar
Rousseau, D. M., Ho, V. T., & Greenberg, J. (2006). I-deals: Idiosyncratic terms in employment relationships. Academy of Management Review, 31(4), 977994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rusbult, C. E., Farrell, D., Rogers, G., & Mainus, A. G. (1988). Impact of exchange variables on exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect: An integrative model of responses to declining job satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 31(3), 599627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapp, D. D. (1995). Creative problem-solving in art: A model for idea inception and image development. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 29(3), 173185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schein, E. H. (1965). Organizational psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Schilling, J. (2009). From ineffectiveness to destruction: A qualitative study of the meaning of negative leadership. Leadership, 5(1), 102128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, E. A., Pircher, V. A., & Peus, C. (2019). Shedding light on leaders’ self-interest: Theory and measurement of exploitative leadership. Journal of Management, 45(4), 14011433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schyns, B., & Schilling, J. (2013). How bad are the effects of bad leaders? A meta-analysis of destructive leadership and its outcomes. The Leadership Quarterly, 24(1), 138158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skiba, M., & Rosenberg, S. (2011). The disutility of equity theory in contemporary management practice. Journal of Business and Economic Studies, 17(2), 119.Google Scholar
Stouten, J., & Tripp, T. M. (2009). Claiming more than equality: Should leaders ask for forgiveness? The Leadership Quarterly, 20(3), 287298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Syed, F., Akhtar, M. W., Saeed, K., & Husnain, M. (2019). Interplay of exploitative leadership & fear of negative evaluation on knowledge hiding & outcomes. Academy of Management Proceedings, 2019(1), 152158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tett, R. P., & Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based interactionist model of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 500517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tett, R. P., & Guterman, H. A. (2000). Situation trait relevance, trait expression, and cross-situational consistency: Testing a principle of trait activation. Journal of Research in Personality, 34(4), 397423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turnley, W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (1999). A discrepancy model of psychological contract violations. Human Resource Management Review, 9(3), 367385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turnley, W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2000). Re-examining the effects of psychological contract violations: Unmet expectations and job dissatisfaction as mediators. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21(1), 2542.3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, T. R., & DeGoey, P. (1995). Collective restraint in social dilemmas: Procedural justice and social identification effects on support for authorities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(3), 482497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In Zanna, M. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115191). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Van den Bos, K. (1996). Procedural justice and conflict. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1996). The consistency rule and the voice effect: The influence of expectations on procedural fairness judgments and performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26(3), 411428.3.0.CO;2-2>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. A. M. (1997). Procedural and distributive justice: What is fair depends more on what comes first than on what comes next. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(1), 95104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Dyne, L., & LePine, J. A. (1998). Helping and voice extra-role behaviors: Evidence of construct and predictive validity. Academy of Management Journal, 41(1), 108119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, L., LaTour, S., Lind, E. A., & Thibaut, J. (1974). Reactions of participants and observers to modes of adjudication. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 4(4), 295310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, M. J. (2014). Serving the self from the seat of power: Goals and threats predict leaders’ self-interested behavior. Journal of Management, 40(5), 13651395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, X., & Bartol, K. M. (2010a). Linking empowering leadership and employee creativity: The influence of psychological empowerment, intrinsic motivation, and creative process engagement. Academy of Management Journal, 53(1), 107128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, X., & Bartol, K. M. (2010b). The influence of creative process engagement on employee creative performance and overall job performance: A curvilinear assessment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(5), 862873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y., & Bednall, T. C. (2016). Antecedents of abusive supervision: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Business Ethics, 139(5), 455471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, J., & George, J. M. (2001). When job dissatisfaction leads to creativity: Encouraging the expression of voice. Academy of Management Journal, 44(4), 682696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Reliability and validity of study 1

Figure 1

Table 2. Confirmatory factor analyses of study 1

Figure 2

Table 3. Means, standard deviations, and correlations of study 1

Figure 3

Table 4. Mediation analyses of study 1

Figure 4

Figure 1. Study 1 tested model.

Figure 5

Table 5. Reliability and validity of study 2

Figure 6

Figure 2. Study 2 tested model.

Figure 7

Table 6. Confirmatory factor analyses of study 2

Figure 8

Table 7. Means, standard deviations, and correlations of study 2

Figure 9

Table 8. Moderation analyses of study 2

Figure 10

Table 9. Combined moderation of Rc and Pel of study 2

Figure 11

Figure 3. Combined moderating effect of study 2.