Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T23:24:12.611Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The naturalness of (many) social institutions: evolved cognition as their foundation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 June 2011

PASCAL BOYER*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Campus Box 1125, Washington University, 1 Brookings Drive, St Louis, MO 63130, USA
MICHAEL BANG PETERSEN
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

Abstract:

Most standard social science accounts only offer limited explanations of institutional design, i.e. why institutions have common features observed in many different human groups. Here we suggest that these features are best explained as the outcome of evolved human cognition, in such domains as mating, moral judgment and social exchange. As empirical illustrations, we show how this evolved psychology makes marriage systems, legal norms and commons management systems intuitively obvious and compelling, thereby ensuring their occurrence and cultural stability. We extend this to propose under what conditions institutions can become ‘natural’, compelling and legitimate, and outline probable paths for institutional change given human cognitive dispositions. Explaining institutions in terms of these exogenous factors also suggests that a general theory of institutions as such is neither necessary nor in fact possible. What are required are domain-specific accounts of institutional design in different domains of evolved cognition.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The JOIE Foundation 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainslie, G. (2005), ‘Précis of Breakdown of Will’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28: 635673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alford, J. R. and Hibbing, J. R. (2004), ‘The Origin of Politics: An Evolutionary Theory of Political Behavior’, Perspectives on Politics, 2 (4): 707723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., Lam, D., and Lancaster, J. (1999a), ‘Paternal Care by Genetic Fathers and Stepfathers II: Reports by Xhosa High School Students’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 20 (6): 433451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, K. G., Kaplan, H., and Lancaster, J. (1999b), ‘Paternal Care by Genetic Fathers and Stepfathers I: Reports from Albuquerque Men’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 20 (6): 405431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balvig, F. (2005), ‘When Law and Order Returned to Denmark’, Journal of Scandinavian Studies in Criminology and Crime Prevention, 5 (2): 167187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (eds.) (1992), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2005a), ‘Adaptations to Predators and Prey’, in Buss, D. M. (ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 200223.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2005b), ‘Enzymatic Computation and Cognitive Modularity’, Mind and Language, 20 (3): 259287.Google Scholar
Bicchieri, C. (2006), The Grammar Of Society: The Nature and Dynamics of Social Norms, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (1998), ‘Cognitive Tracks of Cultural Inheritance: How Evolved Intuitive Ontology Governs Cultural Transmission’, American Anthropologist, 100 (4): 876889.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brousseau, E. and Glachant, J.-M. (2008), New Institutional Economics: A Guidebook, Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1989), ‘Sex Differences in Human Mate Preferences: Evolutionary Hypotheses Tested in 37 Cultures’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12 (1): 149.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2000), The Dangerous Passion: Why Jealousy is as Necessary as Love and Sex, New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (ed.) (2005), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. and Duntley, J. D. (2008), ‘Adaptations for Exploitation’, Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 12 (1): 5362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardenas, J. C., Stranlund, J., and Willis, C. (2002), ‘Local Environmental Control and Institutional Crowding-Out’, World Development, 28 (10): 17191733.Google Scholar
Carvalho, J.-P. and Koyama, M. (2010), ‘Instincts and institutions: The Rise of the Market’, in Butos, W. N. (ed.), The Social Science of Hayek's ‘The Sensory Order’, Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing Limited, pp. 285309.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (1994), ‘Origins of Domain-Specificity: The Evolution of Functional Organization’, in Hirschfeld, L. A. and Gelman, S. (eds.), Mapping the Mind: Domain-Specificity in Culture and Cognition, New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (eds.) (2005), Neurocognitive Adaptations Designed for Social Exchange, Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (2006), ‘Evolutionary Psychology, Moral Heuristics, and the Law’, in Gigerenzer, G. and Engel, C. (eds.), Heuristics and the Law, Cambridge, MA and Berlin: MIT Press and Dahlem University Press, pp. 175206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M. and Wilson, M. (1988), Homicide, New York: Aldine.Google ScholarPubMed
Darley, J. M. and Pittman, T. S. (2003), ‘The Psychology of Compensatory and Retributive Justice’, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7 (4): 324336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Quervain, D. J.-F., Fischbacher, U., Treyer, V., Schellhammer, M., Schnyder, U., Buck, A., and Fehr, E. (2004), ‘The Neural Basis of Altruistic Punishment’, Science, 305 (5688): 12541258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demorest, R. A., Posner, J. C., Osterhoudt, K., and Henretig, F. M. (2004), ‘Poisoning Prevention Education during Emergency Department Visits for Childhood Poisoning’, Pediatric Emergency Care, 20 (5): 281284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dequech, D. (2006), ‘Institutions and Norms in Institutional Economics and Sociology’, Journal of Economic Issues, 40 (2): 473–309.Google Scholar
Dopfer, K. (2005), The Evolutionary Foundations of Economics, New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., Barrett, L., and Lycett, J. (2005), Evolutionary Psychology: A Beginner's Guide, Oxford: Oneworld.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J. (1993), ‘The Evolution of Sexual Attraction: Evaluative Mechanisms in Women’, in Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (eds.), The Adapted Mind. Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 267288.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., Pettit, G. S., and Woodward, L. (2003), ‘Does Father Absence Place Daughters at Special Risk for Early Sexual Activity and Teenage Pregnancy?’, Child Development, 74 (3): 801821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enright, R. D. (1984), ‘Distributive Justice Development: Cross-Cultural, Contextual, and Longitudinal Evaluations’, Child Development, 55 (5): 17371751.Google Scholar
Ermer, E., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (eds.) (2007), Functional Specialization and the Adaptationist Program, New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., Schmidt, K. M., Kolm, S.-C., and Ythier, J. M. (2006), ‘The Economics of Fairness, Reciprocity and Altruism – Experimental Evidence and New Theories’, in Kolm, S.-C. and Ythier, J. M. (eds.), Handbook of the Economics of Giving, Altruism and Reciprocity: Foundations, vol. 1, New York: Elsevier Science, pp. 615691.Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T. (2001), ‘Emotions and Cost–Benefit Assessment: The Role of Shame and Self-Esteem in Risk Taking’, in Gigerenzer, G. and Selten, R. (eds.), Bounded Rationality: The Adaptive Toolbox, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 191214.Google Scholar
Frank, R. H. (1988), Passions Within Reason. The Strategic Role of The Emotions, New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Garland, D. (1990), Punishment and Modern Society. A Study in Social Theory, Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Gellner, E. (1985), Relativism and the Social Sciences, Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (2002), Adaptive Thinking: Rationality in the Real World, New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. and Todd, P. M. (1999), Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart, New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goody, J. (1986), The Logic of Writing and the Organization of Society, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Greene, J. (2005), ‘Cognitive Neuroscience and the Structure of the Moral Mind’, in Carruthers, P. (ed.), The Innate Mind: Structure and Contents, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 338352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greif, A. (2006), Institutions and the Path to the Modern Economy: Lessons from Medieval Trade, Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gudeman, S. (1986), Economics as Culture: Models and Metaphors of Livelihood, London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Gürerk, Ö., Irlenbusch, B., and Rockenbach, B. (2006), ‘The Competitive Advantage of Sanctioning Institutions’, Science, 312 (5770): 108111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagen, E. H. and Hammerstein, P. (2006), ‘Game Theory and Human Evolution: A Critique of Some Recent Interpretations of Experimental Games’, Theoretical Population Biology, 69 (3): 339348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2001), ‘The Emotional Dog and its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment’, Psychological Review, 108 (4): 814834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2007), ‘The New Synthesis in Moral Psychology’, Science, 316 (5827): 9981002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heintz, C. (2007), ‘Institutions as Mechanisms of Cultural Evolution: Prospects of the Epidemiological Approach’, Biological Theory, 2 (3): 244249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hibbing, J. R. and Alford, J. R. (2004), ‘Accepting Authoritative Decisions: Humans as Wary Cooperators’, American Journal of Political Science, 48 (1): 6276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodgson, G. M. (1999), Evolution And Institutions: On Evolutionary Economics and the Evolution of Economics, Cheltenham and Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar.Google Scholar
Hoebel, E. A. (1964), The Law of Primitive Man: A Study in Comparative Legal Dynamics, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A. (eds.) (1982), Jugdments Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanazawa, S. (2002), ‘Bowling with our Imaginary Friends’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 23: 167168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, H. S. and Gangestad, S. W. (2005), ‘Life History Theory and Evolutionary Psychology’, in Buss, D. M. (ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 6895.Google Scholar
Knight, J. (1995), ‘Models, Interpretations and Theories: Constructing Explanations of Institutional Emergence and Change’, in Knight, J. and Sened, I. (eds.), Explaining Social Institutions, Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, pp. 1526.Google Scholar
Knight, J. and North, D. C. (1997), ‘Explaining Economic Change: The Interplay Between Cognition and Institutions’, Legal Theory, 3 (3): 211226.Google Scholar
Knight, J. and Sened, I. (eds.) (1995), Explaining Social Institutions, Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1981), The Philosophy of Moral Development: Moral Stages and the Idea of Justice, San Francisco: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Kuran, T. (1995), Private Truths, Public Lies: The Social Consequences of Preference Falsification, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R., Descioli, P., and O'Brien, E. (2007), ‘Audience Effects on Moralistic Punishment’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 28 (2): 7584.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R. and Leary, M. R. (2001), ‘Evolutionary Origins of Stigmatization: The Functions of Social Exclusion’, Psychological Bulletin, 127 (2): 187208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurzban, R. and Neuberg, S. (2005), ‘Managing Ingroup and Outgroup Relationships’, in Buss, D. M. (ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 653675.Google Scholar
Lieberman, D., Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (2003), ‘Does Morality have a Biological Basis? An Empirical Test of the Factors Governing Moral Sentiments Relating to Incest’, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences, 270 (1517): 819826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, G. and O'Donoghue, T. (2005), ‘Animal Spirits. Affective and Deliberative Processes in Economic Behavior’, unpublished manuscript, Carnegie Mellon University.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., and Calvini, G. (1999), ‘Seeing More Than We Can Know: Visual Attention and Category Activation’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35 (6): 590602.Google Scholar
Maine, H. S. S. (1963), Ancient Law; its Connection with the Early History of Society and its Relation to Modern Ideas, Boston, MA: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
McCabe, K. A. and Smith, V. L. (2001), ‘Goodwill Accounting and the Process of Exchange’, in Gigerenzer, G. and Selten, R. (eds.), Bounded Rationality: The Adaptive Toolbox, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 319340.Google Scholar
McClosky, H. and Brill, A. (2003), Dimensions of Tolerance, New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
McDermott, R. (2006), ‘Editor's Introduction’, Political Psychology, 27 (3): 347358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merton, R. K. (1996), On Social Structure And Science, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
New, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (2007), ‘Category-Specific Attention for Animals Reflects Ancestral Priorities, Not Expertise’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104 (42): 1659816603.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. and Knobe, J. (2007), ‘Moral Responsibility and Determinism: The Cognitive Science of Folk Intuitions’, Nôus, 41, 663685.Google Scholar
Nørager, R. (2009), ‘Why Evolutionary and Cognitive Developmental Psychology? The Importance of Non-Human Primates and Human Infants for Understanding Adult Use of Modern Computerized Technology’, in Høgh-Olesen, H., Tønnesvang, J. and Bertelsen, P. (eds.), Human Characteristics, Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, pp. 117149.Google Scholar
North, D. C. (1990), Institutions, Institutional Change, and Economic Performance, Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
North, D. C. (2005), Understanding the Process of Economic Change, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ostrom, E. (1990), Governing the Commons: The Evolution of Institutions for Collective Action, Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ostrom, E. (2005), Understanding Institutional Diversity, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Petersen, M. B. (2010), ‘Distinct Emotions, Distinct Domains: Anger, Anxiety and Perceptions of Intentionality’, Journal of Politics, 72: 357365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, M. B., Sell, A., Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (2010), ‘Evolutionary Psychology and Criminal Justice: A Recalibrational Theory of Punishment and Reconciliation’, in Høgh-Olesen, H. (ed.), Human Morality and Sociality, Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 72131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierson, P. (2004), Politics in Time: History, Institutions and Social Analysis, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Pooley, A. J. and Fiddick, L. (2010), ‘Social Referencing “Mr. Yuk”: The Use of Emotion in a Poison Prevention Program’, Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 35 (4): 327339.Google Scholar
Posner, E. A. (2000), Law And Social Norms, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Posner, E. A. (2007), Social Norms, Nonlegal Sanctions, and the Law, Cheltenham and Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar.Google Scholar
Posner, R. A. (1981), The Economics of Justice, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Price, M. E., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (2002), ‘Punitive Sentiment as an Anti-Free Rider Psychological Device’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 23 (3): 203231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinlan, R. J. (2003), ‘Father Absence, Parental Care, and Female Reproductive Development’, Evolution and Human Behavior, 24 (6): 376390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J. V., Stalans, L. J., Indermaur, D., and Hough, M. (2002), Penal Populism and Public Opinion: Lessons from Five Countries, New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. H., Kurzban, R., and Jones, O. (2007), ‘The Origins of Shared Intuitions of Justice’, Vanderbilt Law Review, 60 (6): 16331688.Google Scholar
Saad, G. and Gill, T. (2000), ‘Applications of Evolutionary Psychology in Marketing’, Psychology and Marketing, 17 (12): 10051034.3.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, M. (2006), ‘Parasites, Behavioral Defenses, and the Social Psychological Mechanisms Through Which Cultures are Evoked’, Psychological Inquiry, 17 (2): 96137.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1995), The Construction of Social Reality, New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Sidanius, J. and Veniegas, R. C. (2000), ‘Gender and Race Discrimination: The Interactive Nature of Disadvantage’, in Oskamp, S. (ed.), Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Sigelman, C. K. and Waitzman, K. A. (1991), ‘The Development of Distributive Justice Orientations: Contextual Influences on Children's Resource Allocations’, Child Development, 62 (6): 13671378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. B., Larimer, C. W., Littvay, L., and Hibbing, J. R. (2007), ‘Evolutionary Theory and Political Leadership: Why Certain People do not Trust Decision Makers’, Journal of Politics, 69 (2): 285299.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996), Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach, Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sugden, R. (1986), The Economics of Rights, Co-operation and Welfare, Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Symons, D. (1979), The Evolution of Human Sexuality, New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (1992), ‘The Psychological Foundations of Culture’, in Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (eds.) The Adapted Mind. Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1994), ‘The Development of Social-Conventional and Moral Concepts’, in Bill, P. (ed.), Fundamental Research in Moral Development. Moral Development: A Compendium, vol. 2, New York: Garland Publishing, Inc., pp. 255293.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (2002), The Culture of Morality: Social Development, Context, and Conflict, New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tyler, T. R. (2001), ‘A Psychological Perspective on the Legitimacy of Institutions and Authorities’, in Jost, J. T. and Major, B. (eds.), The Psychology of Legitimacy, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 416436.Google Scholar
Van Gennep, A. (1909), Les rites de passage; étude systématique des rites de la porte et du seuil, de l'hospitalité, de l'adoption, de la grossesse et de l'accouchement, de la naissance, de l'enfance, de la puberté, de l'initiation, de l'ordination, du couronnement des fiançailles et du mariage, des funérailles, des saisons, etc., Paris: É. Nourry.Google Scholar
Van Schaik, C. P. and Hooff, J. A. Van (1983), ‘On the Ultimate Causes of Primate Social Systems’, Behaviour, 85 (1/2): 91117.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (2002), The Protestant Ethic and the Spirit of Capitalism [translated by Baehr, P. and Wells, G. C.], Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. and Daly, M. (1992), ‘The Man who Mistook his Wife for a Chattel’, in Barkow, J. H. and Cosmides, L. (eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, London: Oxford University Press, pp. 289322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. and Daly, M. (1998), ‘Lethal and Nonlethal Violence Against Wives and the Evolutionary Psychology of Male Sexual Proprietariness’, in Dobash, R. E. and Dobash, R. P. (eds.), Rethinking Violence Against Women, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc., pp. 199230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagishi, T. (1992), ‘Group Size and the Provision of a Sanctioning System in a Social Dilemma’, in Liebrand, W. B. G., Messick, D. M. and Wilkie, H. A. M. (eds.), Social Dilemmas: Theoretical Issues and Research Findings, Oxford: Pergamon, pp. 267288.Google Scholar