Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:32:59.180Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Who is ‘Daddy’ revisited: the status of two-year-olds' over-extended words in use and comprehension

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Jean R. Thomson
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin
Robin S. Chapman
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin

Abstract

Diary observations of two-year-olds' over-extended word use have been interpreted as arising from the word's underlying semantic feature structure, retrieval error, or limited vocabulary. The first interpretation was tested by analysing whether the same few features systematically characterized most over-extensions of a word (picture-naming task), and whether the same word was over-extended in comprehension (picture-pointing task). Four instances of over-extended use were studied in each of 5 children aged 1; 9 to 2; 3. A semantic feature interpretation is rejected, as is the view that overextensions are used by the child deliberately, to refer to objects which he knows are not properly named by the words. The need to construct models of early word meaning which reflect the early instability of conceptual organization, the probability of retrieval error in recall tasks, and the developmental history of individual words – particularly parental acceptance or rejection of use – is discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1977

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. M. (1970). The growth of word meaning. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1975). On the extension of the child's first terms of reference. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, April.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1973) What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. (1973). Who is dada? L & S 16. 3443.Google Scholar
Heaf, R. A. & Bell, R. Q. (1967). A facial dimension in visual discrimination by human infants. ChDev 38. 893–99.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. (1974) The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. L. (ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology. Potomac, Md.: Eribaum.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1970). The acquisition of language. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word and sentence: interrelations in acquisition and development. PsychRev 81. 267–85.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Press, M. L. (1974). Semantic features in lexical acquisition. PRCLD 8. 129–41.Google Scholar
Reich, P. (1976). The early acquisition of word meaning. JChLang 3. 117–23.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Discover the deep sea: a sticker book. Racine, Wi.: Western Publishing Company, Inc., 1974.Google Scholar
The golden book of animal stamps. Racine, Wi.: Western Publishing Company, Inc., 1966.Google Scholar
The golden stamp book of cats. Racine, Wi.: Western Publishing Company, Inc., 1971.Google Scholar
The golden stamp book of dogs. Racine, WI.: Western Publishing Company, Inc., 1966.Google Scholar
The golden stamp book of horses. Racine, Wi.: Western Publishing Company, Inc., 1971.Google Scholar
The golden stamp book of pets. Racine, Wi.: Western Publishing Company, Inc., 1972.Google Scholar