Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T14:13:33.369Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

That's not what you said earlier: preschoolers expect partners to be referentially consistent*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2013

SUSAN A. GRAHAM*
Affiliation:
University of Calgary
JULIE SEDIVY
Affiliation:
University of Calgary
MELANIE KHU
Affiliation:
University of Calgary
*
Address for correspondence: S. Graham, Dept. of Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB T2N 1N4Canada. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

In a conversation, adults expect speakers to be consistent in their use of a particular expression. We examine whether four-year-olds expect speakers to use consistent referential descriptions and whether these expectations are partner-specific. Using an eye-tracking paradigm, we presented four-year-olds with arrays of objects on a screen. During training, Experimenter 1 (E1) used a target expression to identify one object (i.e. “the spotted dog” to identify a dog that is both spotted and fluffy). Following training, either E1 or a new conversational partner (E2) presented children with test trials. Here, the target objects were referred to using either the original expression (e.g. “the spotted dog”) or a new expression (e.g. “the fluffy dog”). Eye-movements indicated that preschoolers were quicker to identify the target referent when the original expression was used by the same speaker. This suggests that four-year-olds, like adults, expect communicative partners to adhere to referential pacts.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by funds from the Canada Foundation for Innovation and from the Canada Research Chairs program and by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada awarded to SG. We thank the parents and children who participated as well as Mekale Kibreab, Vanessa Schell, Monica Hernandez, Charissa Ho, and Bretton Lang for their assistance.

References

REFERENCES

Barr, D. J. & Keysar, B. (2002). Anchoring comprehension in linguistic precedents. Journal of Memory and Language 46(2), 391418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, J. M. J., Chambers, C. G. & Graham, S. A. (2010). Preschoolers' appreciation of speaker vocal affect as a cue to referential intent. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 107, 8799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brennan, S. E. & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22, 1482–93.Google ScholarPubMed
Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language 61, 171–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition, 133. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1988) On the logic of contrast. Journal of Child Language 15, 317–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (1990). On the pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language 17, 417–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (1997). Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition. Cognition 64, 137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diesendruck, G. (2005). The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Developmental Psychology 41, 451–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diesendruck, G. & Markson, L. (2001). Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology 37, 630–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A., Thorpe, K. & Marchman, V. A. (2010). Blue car, red car: Developing efficiency in online interpretation of adjective–noun phrases. Cognitive Psychology 60, 190217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L. & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In Sekerina, I. A., Fernandez, E. M. & Clahsen, H. (eds), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children's language processing, 96135. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Garrod, S. & Anderson, A. (1987). Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition 27(2), 181218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horton, W. S. (2007). The influence of partner-specific memory associations on language production: Evidence from picture naming. Language and Cognitive Processes 22, 1114–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, A. B. & Makin, V. S. (1998). Referential communication and category acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 127, 331–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, D., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (2010). What's in a manner of speaking? Children's sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents. Developmental Psychology 46(4), 749–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metzing, C. & Brennan, S. E. (2003). When conceptual pacts are broken: Partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expression. Journal of Memory and Language 49, 201213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E. S. & Graham, S. A. (2009). The relations between children's communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology 58, 220–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsen, E. S., Graham, S. A., Smith, S. L. & Chambers, C. G. (2008). Preschoolers' sensitivity to referential ambiguity: Evidence for a dissociation between implicit understanding and explicit behavior. Developmental Science 11(4), 556–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plumert, J. M. (1996). Young children's ability to detect ambiguity in descriptions of location. Cognitive Development 11, 375–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shintel, H. & Keysar, B. (2007). You said it before and you'll say it again: Expectations of consistency in communication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33, 357–69.Google Scholar
Shintel, H. & Keysar, B. (2009). Less is more: A minimalist account of joint action in communication. Topics in Cognitive Science 1, 260–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (2000). The rebirth of children's learning. Child Development 71, 2635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snedeker, J. & Trueswell, J. C. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology 49(3), 238–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M. & Sedivy, J. E. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science 268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M. & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition 73, 89134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Der Wege, M. (2009). Lexical entrainment and lexical differentiation in reference phrase choice. Journal of Memory and Language 60, 448–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar