Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T10:54:36.594Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stress patterns of early child language*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Leslie A. Wieman
Affiliation:
University of Washington

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Notes and discussion
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1976

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bolinger, D. (1972). Accent is predictable (if you're a mind reader). Lg 48. 633–44.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1973). Early syntactic development. London: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J. (1971). Sentence stress and syntactic transformations. Lg 47. 257–81.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. (1970). Meaning and the structure of language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. (1971). Some problems for case grammar. Ohio State University Working Papers in Linguistics 10. 245–65.Google Scholar
Miller, W. & Ervin, S. (1964). The development of grammar in child language. In Bellugi, U. & Brown, R. (eds), The acquisition of language. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmerling, S. (1971). Presupposition and the notion of normal stress. Papers from the Seventh Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. 242–53.Google Scholar
Schmerling, S. (1974). A re-examination of ‘normal stress’. Lg 50. 6673.Google Scholar