Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:26:46.141Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure[*]

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

John J. Kim*
Affiliation:
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Gary F. Marcus
Affiliation:
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Steven Pinker
Affiliation:
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Michelle Hollander
Affiliation:
University of Michigan
Marie Coppola
Affiliation:
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
*
Address for correspondence: Institute for Research in Cognitive Science, University of Pennsylvania, 3401 Walnut Street, Room 407c, Philadelphia, PA 19104–6228, USA.

Abstract

What is the input to the mental System that computes inflected forms like walked, came, dogs, and men? Recent connectionist models feed a word's phonological features into a single network, allowing it to generalize both regular and irregular phonological patterns, like stop-stopped, step-stepped and fling-flung, cling-clung. But for adults, phonological input is insufficient: verbs derived from nouns like ring the city always have regular past tense forms (ringed), even if they are phonologically identical to irregular verbs (ring the bell). Similarly, nouns based on names, like two Mickey Mouses, and compounds based on possessing rather than being their root morpheme, such as two sabertooths, take regular plurals, even when they are homophonous with irregular nouns like mice and teeth. In four experiments, testing 70 three- to ten-year-old children, we found that children are sensitive to such nonphonological information: they were more likely to produce regular inflected forms for forms like to ring (‘to put a ring on’) and snaggletooth (a kind of animal doll with big teeth) than for their homophonous irregular counterparts, even when these counterparts were also extended in meaning. Children's inflectional Systems thus seem to be like adults': irregular forms are tied to the lexicon but regular forms are computed by a default rule, and words are represented as morphological tree structures reflecting their derivation from basic word roots. Such structures, which determine how novel complex words are derived and interpreted, also govern whether words with irregular sound patterns will be regularized: a word can be irregular only if its structure contains an irregular root in ‘head’ position, allowing the lexically stored irregular information to percolate up to apply to the word as a whole. In all other cases, the inflected form is computed by a default regular rule. This proposal fits the facts better than alternatives appealing to ambiguity reduction or semantic similarity to a word's central sense. The results, together with an analysis of adult speech to children, suggest that morphological structure and a distinction between mechanisms for regular and irregular inflection may be inherent to the design of children's language Systems.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We would like to thank Sandeep Prasada, Annie Senghas, Fei Xu and two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions. We would also like to thank the Bowen After School Care Program, Inc. in Newton Center, Bright Horizons Children's Center in Boston, Bright Horizons Children's Center in Kendall Square, Children's Village in Cambridge, the Dandelion School in Cambridge, and the Lotus Children's Center in Cambridge, the MIT Summer Day Camp, and the Newton-Wellesley Children's Corner in Newton Lower Falls for participating in our studies. This research was supported by NIH Grant HD 18381 and NSF Grant BNS 91–09766 to Steven Pinker. The first and second authors were supported by NDSEG Fellowships.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, S. R. (1992). A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Beard, R. & Szymanek, B. (eds) (1988). Bibliography of morphology 1900–1988. Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L. (1985). Morphology: a study of the relation between meaning and sound. Philadelphia: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L. & Moder, C. L. (1983). Morphological classes as natural categories. Language 59, 251–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. & Clark, H. (1979). When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55, 767811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. (1985). Level-ordering in lexical development. Cognition 21, 7393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halle, M. & Mohanon, K. P. (1985). Segmental phonology of modern English. Linguistic Inquiry 16, 57116.Google Scholar
Hare, M. & Elman, J. (1992). A connectionist account of English inflectional morphology: evidence from language change. In Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Harris, C. L. (1992). Understanding English past-tense formation: the shared meaning hypothesis. In Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E., McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). Distributed representations. In Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. & the PDP Research Group (eds), Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Vol. 1. Foundations. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1942). A modern English grammar on historical principles. Part VI. Morphology (Reprinted 1961). London: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Kim, J. J., Pinker, S., Prince, A. S. & Prasada, S. (1991). Why no mere mortal has ever flown out to center field. Cognitive Science 15, 173218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1982 a). From cyclical to lexical phonology. In van der Hulst, H. & Smith, N. (eds), The structure of phonological representations. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1982 b). Lexical phonology and morphology. In Yang, I. S. (ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm. Seoul: Hansin.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1983). Word-formation and the lexicon. In Ingemann, F. (ed.), Proceedings of the 1982 Mid-America Linguistics Conference. Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas.Google Scholar
Kucera, H. & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. (1978). Children's judgments of grammatical and ungrammatical irregular past tense verbs. Child Development 49, 319–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Connectionist explanations in linguistics: some thoughts on recent anticonnectionist papers. Unpublished electronic manuscript, ARPAnet.Google Scholar
Lieber, R. (1980). On the organization of the lexicon. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1990). The CHILDES Project: computational tools for analyzing talk. Version 0.88. Pittsburgh, PA: Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Leinbach, J. (1991). Implementations are not conceptualizations: revising the verb learning model. Cognition 40, 121–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Snow, C. E. (1985). The Child Language Data Exchange System. Journal of Child Language 12, 271–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. (1988). Rules and regularities in the acquisition of the English past tense. Center for Research on Language Newsletter, University of California, San Diego, 2 (4).Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J. & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 57 (4, Serial No. 228).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., Woest, A. & Pinker, S. (1993). German inflection: the exception that proves the rule. MIT Center for Cognitive Science Occasional Paper No 47.Google Scholar
Mencken, H. L. (1936). The American language. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: the acquisition of argument structure, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science 253, 530–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. & Prince, A. S. (1988). On language and connectionism: analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition 28, 73193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. & Prince, A. S. (1991). Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological status of rules of grammar. In Sutton, L. A., Johnson, C. & Shields, R. (eds), Proceedings of the 17th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K. & Marchman, V. (1990). From rote learning to System building (Tech. Rep. No. 9020). La Jolla: University of California, San Diego, Center for Research in Language.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K. & Marchman, V. (1991). U-Shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perceptron: implications for child language acquisition. Cognition 38, 160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prasada, S. & Pinker, S. (1993). Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes 8, 156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. & the PDP Research Group (eds), Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Vol. 2. Psychological and biological models. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. & Daugherty, K. (1992). Rules or connections? The past tense revisited. In Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Selkirk, E. O. (1982). The syntax of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spencer, A. (1990). Morphological theory. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Williams, E. (1981). On the notions ‘lexically related’ and ‘head of a word’. Linguistics Inquiry 12, 245–74.Google Scholar
Xu, F. & Pinker, S. (1992). Weird.past tense forms. Paper presented at the Seventeenth Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development.Google Scholar