Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T14:43:29.807Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of input frequency and semantic transparency in the acquisition of verb meaning: evidence from placement verbs in Tamil and Dutch*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2010

BHUVANA NARASIMHAN*
Affiliation:
University of Colorado at Boulder
MARIANNE GULLBERG*
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen and Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
*
Address for correspondence: Bhuvana Narasimhan, Department of Linguistics, Hellems 290, 295 UCB, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309-0295, USA. tel: 303-492-8456; email: [email protected]
Marianne Gullberg, Centre for Languages and Literature, Lund University, PB Box 201, 221 00 Lund, Sweden. tel: +46-46-222 0389; email: [email protected]

Abstract

We investigate how Tamil- and Dutch-speaking adults and four- to five-year-old children use caused posture verbs (‘lay/stand a bottle on a table’) to label placement events in which objects are oriented vertically or horizontally. Tamil caused posture verbs consist of morphemes that individually label the causal and result subevents (nikka veyyii ‘make stand’; paDka veyyii ‘make lie’), occurring in situational and discourse contexts where object orientation is at issue. Dutch caused posture verbs are less semantically transparent: they are monomorphemic (zetten ‘set/stand’; leggen ‘lay’), often occurring in contexts where factors other than object orientation determine use. Caused posture verbs occur rarely in Tamil input corpora; in Dutch input, they are used frequently. Elicited production data reveal that Tamil four-year-olds use infrequent placement verbs appropriately whereas Dutch children use high-frequency placement verbs inappropriately even at age five. Semantic transparency exerts a stronger influence than input frequency in constraining children's verb meaning acquisition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We gratefully acknowledge the cooperation of the teachers and students of the Ramakrishna Mission Tamil Medium School (Chennai, India) and Kindercentrum Dribbel (Molenhoek, the Netherlands). We are also grateful for funding from the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. We wish to express our thanks to Judith Bindels, Pauline Chew, Bregje Esmeijer, Anke Jolink, Femke Uijtdewilligen, Arna Van Doorn, Shanmugam Mohan and R. Devi for help with the data collection and analysis, and Melissa Bowerman, Asifa Majid and Leah Roberts for comments on the design and analysis of the study. Our thanks also go to the reviewers of this paper who provided us with valuable suggestions. Any remaining errors are solely ours.

References

REFERENCES

Asher, R. E. (1985). Tamil. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R. & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Bomba, P. C. (1984). The development of orientation categories between 2 and 4 months of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 37, 609–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowerman, M. (1978). Systematizing semantic knowledge: Changes over time in the child's organization of word meaning. Child Development 49, 977–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P. (2001). Learning to talk about motion UP and DOWN in Tzeltal: Is there a language-specific bias for verb learning? In Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. C. (eds), Language acquisition and conceptual development, 512–43. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, S. & Bowerman, M. (1991). Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition 41 83–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (2004) Language builds on cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 472–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Essock, E. A. & Siqueland, E. R. (1981). Discrimination of orientation by human infants. Perception 10, 245–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, V. (1982). Decrements in children's responses to big and tall: A reconsideration of the potential cognitive and semantic causes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 34, 156–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Kuczaj, S. A. (ed.), Language development: vol. 2: Language, thought and culture, 301334. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Howell, D. (2002). Statistical methods for psychology, 5th edn.Pacific Grove, CA: Duxbury.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. L. (ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola symposium, 331–68. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. M. & Glucksberg, S. (1977). Social and nonsocial speech. Scientific American 236, 100105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemmens, M. (2006). Caused posture: Experiential patterns emerging from corpus research. In Stefanowitsch, A. & Gries, S. (eds), Corpora in cognitive linguistics. Corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis, 263–98. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, 3rd edn.Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Narasimhan, R. (1981). Modeling language behavior. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plumert, J. M., Ewert, K. & Spear, S. J. (1995). The early development of children's communication about nested spatial relations. Child Development 66, 959–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, P. C. & Bomba, P. C. (1986). Evidence of a general category of oblique orientations in four-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 42, 345–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senft, G. & Smits, R. (eds) 2000. The Annual Report of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 2000. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1985). Cross-linguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition, vol. 2, 1157–256. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Rowland, C. F. (2002). ‘Going, going, gone’: The acquisition of the verb ‘go’. Journal of Child Language 29, 783811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Rowland, C. F. (2004). Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language 31, 6999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, J. R. & Chapman, R. S. (1977). Who is ‘Daddy’ revisited: The status of two-year-olds' over-extended words in use and comprehension. Journal of Child Language 4, 359–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wijnen, F. & Bol, G. (1993). The escape from the optional infinitive stage. In de Boer, A., de Jong, J. & Landeweerd, R. (eds), Language and cognition, vol. 3, 239–48. Groningen: Department of Linguistics, University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Yule, G. (1997). Referential communication tasks. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar