Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:57:01.789Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonemic structures of delayed phonological systems*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Judith A. Gierut*
Affiliation:
Indiana University
Christina L. Simmerman
Affiliation:
Indiana University
Heidi J. Neumann
Affiliation:
Indiana University
*
Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA.

Abstract

The phonemic inventories of 30 children (aged 3;4–5;7) with phonological delays were examined in terms of featural distinctions in order to address universal vs. individual accounts of acquisition. Phonetic inventories of these same children were also identified for comparison purposes. Across children, four hierarchical and implicationally related types of phonemic inventory were identified. The typology uniquely captured common distinctions maintained by all children, and at the same time, allowed for individual differences in the specific phonemic composition of each system. These cross-sectional results have theoretical implications for the longitudinal course of phonemic acquisition. In particular, children appear to have a number of linguistic choices that relate to the course, the specifics, and the mechanism of change in acquisition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health (DC 00433, DC 01694, DC 00076) to Indiana University. The results are an extension of a master's thesis completed by Christina Simmerman. Steven Chin, Daniel Dinnsen and Mary Hughes provided helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper. We also appreciate the comments of David Ingram and an anonymous JCL reviewer. William Bowers, Elizabeth Connell Anttonen, Mary Hughes and Faith Salesin served as reliability judges. Portions of this paper were presented at the 1991 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Convention, Atlanta, the 1992 Child Phonology Conference, Urbana, IL, and the 1992 Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston.

References

REFERENCES

Archangeli, D. (1988). Aspects of underspecification theory. Phonology 5, 183207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camarata, S. M. & Gandour, J. (1984). On describing idiosyncratic phonologic systems. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 49, 262–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N. (1991). Place of articulation in consonants and vowels: a unified theory. In Laks, B. & Rialland, A. (eds), L'architecture et la géométrie des représentations phonologiques. Paris: CNRS.Google Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B. & Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language 67, 297319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A. (1984). Methods and empirical issues in analyzing functional misarticulation. In Elbert, M., Dinnsen, D. A. & Weismer, G. (eds), Phonological theory and the misarticulating child. Rockville, MD: ASHA.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A. (1992). Variation in developing and fully developed phonetic inventories. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds), Phonological development: models, research, implications. Timonium, MD: York.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A., Chin, S. B. & Elbert, M. (1992). On the lawfulness of change in phonetic inventories. Lingua 86, 207–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A., Chin, S. B., Elbert, M. & Powell, T. W. (1990). Some constraints on functionally disordered phonologies: phonetic inventories and phonotactics. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 33, 2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. A. & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition: English initial consonants in the first fifty words. Language 51, 410–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gierut, J. A. (1985). On the relationship between phonological knowledge and generalization learning in misarticulating children. Ph.D. Dissertation. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Gierut, J. A. (1989). Developing descriptions of phonological systems: a surrebuttal. Applied Psycholinguistics 10, 469–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gierut, J. A. (1992). The conditions and course of clinically-induced phonological change. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 35, 1049–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gierut, J. A. (in press). Cyclicity in the acquisition of phonemic distinctions. Lingua.Google Scholar
Goldman, R. & Fristoe, M. (1986). Goldman–Fristoe test of articulation. Circles Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1988). Jakobson revisited: some evidence from the acquisition of Polish. Lingua 75, 5582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, D. (1989). First language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1990). The acquisition of the feature [voice] in normal and phonologically delayed English children. Paper presented at the American Speech–Language–Hearing Association Convention, Seattle.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941/1968). Child language, aphasia, and phonological universals (Keiler, A. R., trans.). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Newhoff, M. & Mesalam, L. (1980). Individual differences in early child phonology. Applied Psycholinguistics 1, 730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Rowan, L. E., Morris, B. & Fey, M. E. (1982). Intra-word phonological variability in young children. Journal of Child Language 9, 5569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, M. N. (1986). Leiter international performance scale: a handbook. Chicago: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1985). Hungarian language acquisition as an exemplification of a general model of grammatical development. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 2. Theoretical issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maddieson, I. (1984). Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moskowitz, B. A. (1980). Idioms in phonology acquisition and phonological change. Journal of Phonetics 8, 6983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, C. & Prunet, J. F. (1991). Phonetics and phonology. Vol. 2. The special status of coronals: internal and external evidence. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rice, K. & Avery, P. (1991). On the relationship between laterality and coronality. In Paradis, C. & Prunet, J. F. (eds), Phonetics and phonology. Vol. 2. The special status of coronals: internal and external evidence. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Robbins, J. & Klee, T. (1987). Clinical assessment of oropharyngeal motor development in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 52, 271–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slobin, D. I. (1971). Data for the symposium. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Smit, A. B., Hand, L., Freilinger, J. J., Bernthal, J. E. & Bird, A. (1990). The Iowa articulation norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 55, 770–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: a case study. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Sommerstein, A. H. (1977). Modern phonology. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Spencer, A. (1986). Toward a theory of phonological development. Lingua 68, 338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stampe, D. (1969). The acquisition of phonetic representation. Papers from the Fifth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistics Society, 433–44.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1985). Phonetic inventories, 15–24 months: a longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 28, 505–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gammon, C. & Cooper, J. A. (1984). Patterns of early lexical and phonological development. Journal of Child Language 11, 247–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velten, H. (1943). The growth of phonemic and lexical patterns in infant speech. Language 19, 281–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Ferguson, C. A. & Elbert, M. (1986). Phonological development from babbling to speech: common tendencies and individual differences. Applied Psycholinguistics 7, 340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar