Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T13:45:01.308Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Non-dialogue speech during preschool interactions*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Debra Schober-Peterson*
Affiliation:
Speech Pathology Services, Inc.
Cynthia J. Johnson
Affiliation:
University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
*
Speech Pathology Services, Inc. 2790 Sandy Plains Road, Suite 202, Marietta, GA 30066, USA.

Abstract

Young children's skilfulness in conversational interaction is of great importance in understanding pragmatic aspects of language development. The purpose of this investigation was to describe the portion of preschool verbal interaction which could not be considered successful dialogue. Ten dyads of four-year-olds were videotaped during free play. Segments of talk were identified as dialogue or non-dialogue. Non-dialogue was coded when one child did not appear to direct his/her utterances to the other or when an attempted topic failed to capture the interest of the other child. Thirteen types of non-dialogue were identified. Results revealed that all dyads exhibited non-dialogue, but differences were noted in regard to amount and type. Dyads labelled as less co-operative displayed greater amounts of self-directed talk (monologue).

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

An earlier version of this article was presented in a paper entitled ‘Non-dialogue talk in preschool interaction’ at the meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association in New Orleans, LA, November, 1987.

References

REFERENCES

Bedrosian, J., Wanska, S., Sykes, K., Smith, A. & Dalton, B. (1988). Conversational turn-taking violations in mother-child interactions. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 31. 81–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, H. & Gallagher, T. (1979). The structural characteristics of monologues in the speech of normal children: syntactic nonconversational aspects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 22. 4662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dore, J. (1979). Conversation and preschool language development. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Dorval, B. & Eckerman, C. (1984). Developmental trends in the quality of conversation achieved by small groups of acquainted peers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 49. 191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, S. (1986). Learning discourse topic management in the preschool years. Journal of Child Language 13. 231–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, L., Lucariello, J., Seidman, S. & Nelson, K. (1985). The influence of discourse content and context on preschoolers' use of language. In Galda, L. & Pellegrini, A. (eds), Play, language and stories. Norwood, NJ: AblexGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, T. & Craig, H. (1978). Structural characteristics of monologues in the speech of normal children: semantic and conversational aspects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 21. 103–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, T. & Craig, H. (1982). An investigation of overlap in children's speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 11, 6375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garvey, C. (1977). Contingent queries. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: WileyGoogle Scholar
Garvey, C. (1984). Children's talk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Garvey, C. & Hogan, R. (1973). Social speech and social interaction: egocentrism revisited. Child Development 44. 562–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, S. W. & Krantz, M. (1981). The effects of preferred partnerships on communication adequacy. The Journal of Psychology 109. 245–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1974). Conversational competence in children. Journal of Child Language 1. 163–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E. O. (1977). Making it last: repetition in children's discourse. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L., Yaeger, J. & Hjertholm, E. (1968). Private speech: four studies and a review of theories. Child Development 39. 691736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L. & Wertsch, J. (1987). Language and the development of thought. In Kohlberg, L. (ed.), Child psychology and childhood education: a cognitive developmental view. New York: LongmanGoogle Scholar
Lee, L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis. Evanston: Northwestern University PressGoogle Scholar
McTear, M. (1985). Children's conversation. New York: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. (1981). Assessing language production in children. Baltimore: University Park PressGoogle Scholar
Mueller, E. (1972). Maintenance of verbal exchanges between young children. Child Development 43. 930–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. & Gruendel, J. (1979). At morning it's lunchtime: a scriptal view of children's dialogues. Discourse Processes 2. 7394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1926). The language and thought of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan PaulGoogle Scholar
Sachs, J., Goldman, J. & Chaille, C. (1985). Narratives in preschoolers' sociodramatic play: the role of knowledge and communicative competence. In Galda, L. & Pellegrini, A. (eds), Play, language and stories. Norwood N. J.: AblexGoogle Scholar
Saville-Troike, M., McClure, E. & Fritz, M. (1984). Communicative tactics in children's second language acquisition. In Eckman, F., Bell, L. & Nelson, D. (eds), Universals of second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury HouseGoogle Scholar
Schober-Peterson, D. & Johnson, C. J. (1989). Conversational topics of four-year-olds. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 32. 857–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38. 137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Dijk, T. A. (1980). Macrostructures. Hillsdale, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1956). Selected psychological investigations. Moscow: Izdatel'stvo Nauk SSSRGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1960). The development of higher mental functions. Moscow: Izdatel'stvo Nauk SSSRGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Wanska, S. & Bedrosian, J. (1986). Topic and communicative intent in mother-child discourse. Journal of Child Language 13. 523–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wanska, S., Bedrosian, J. & Pohlman, J. (1986). Effects of play materials on topic performance of preschool children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 17. 152–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weir, R. (1962). Language in the crib. The Hague: MoutonGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G. & Pond, R. E. (1979). Preschool Language Scale. Columbus, Ohio: MerrillGoogle Scholar