Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:36:05.567Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A longitudinal study of infant vocalizations during mother–infant games*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Tibie Rome-Flanders*
Affiliation:
Hôpital Marie Enfant
Carolyn Cronk
Affiliation:
Université de Montréal
*
Centre de Recherche, Hôpital Marie Enfant, 5200 Bélanger St East, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, H1T 1C9.

Abstract

This study explored the development of verbal behaviours of infants during two mother–infant games. Twenty-five infants were videotaped as they played peek-a-boo and ball with their mothers at 0;6, 0;9, 1;0, 1;3, 1;6, and 2;0. The frequencies of eight categories of vocal/verbal behaviours were analysed as they evolved over time in both games. Despite differences in the structure and level of difficulty of peek-a-boo and ball, the development of these behaviours proved to be similar in the two games. One category, PRIMITIVE VOCALIZATIONS, which did not change in frequency over time, was seen to have a pragmatic rather than a linguistic function. Another category, PRELEXICAL COMMENT, demonstrated an early capacity for conveying topic and comment together in the form of speech sounds combined with an attentiongetting gesture well before the emergence of multiple-word utterances. The vocal behaviours produced during games were compared with the results of language tests administered during the experimental sessions. Strong correlations were found between the results of these two measures of language. The predictive nature of vocal behaviours during games is discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported in part by a post-doctoral fellowship to the first author granted by the Faculty of Medicine of the Université de Montréal.

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill—Palmer Quarterly 21, 205–24.Google Scholar
Blishen, B. & McRoberts, H. (1976). A revised socioeconomic index for occupation in Canada. Revue Canadienne de Sociologie et Anthropologie 13, 71–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975 a). From communication to language – a psychological perspective. Cognition 3, 255–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975 b). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language 21, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1976). Prelinguistic prerequisites of speech. In Campbell, R. & Smith, P. (eds), Recent advances in the psychology of language, Vol 4. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1978 a). Acquiring the uses of language. Canadian Journal of Psychology 32, 204–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S. (1978 b). Learning how to do things with words. In Bruner, J. S. & Garton, A. (eds), Human growth and development: Wolfson College lectures. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1983). The acquisition of pragmatic commitments. In Golinkoff, R. M. (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. & Sherwood, V. (1975). Peekaboo and the learning of rule structures. In Bruner, J. S., Jolly, A. & Sylva, K. (eds), Play, its role in development and evolution. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. & Ninio, A. (1978). The achievement of antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language 5, 115.Google Scholar
Bzoch, K. R. & League, R. (1980). Assessing language skills in infancy. Baltimore: University Park Press (1st Edn, 1971).Google Scholar
Camaioni, L. & Laicardi, C. (1985). Early social games and the acquisition of language. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 3, 31–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, A. (1978). From sensorimotor vocalization to words: a case study of the evolution of attention-direction communication in the second year. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Carter, A. (1979). Prespeech meaning relations: an outline of one infant's sensorimotor morpheme development. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1975). Holophrases, speech acts and linguistic universals. Journal of Child Language 2, 2140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dore, J., Franklin, M. B., Miller, R. T. & Ramer, A. L. (1976). Transitional phenomena in early language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 3, 343–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1978). Learning to pronounce: the earliest stages of phonological development in the child. In Minifie, F. D. & Lloyd, L. L. (eds), Communication and cognitive abilities – early behavioural assessment. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Flanders, T. & Ricard, M. (1992). Infant timing of vocalizations in two mother–infant games: a longitudinal study. First language 12, 285–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanders, T., Cossette, L., Ricard, M. & Décarie, (in press). Comprehension of rules and structures in mother–infant games: a longitudinal study of the first two years of life. International Journal of Behavioural Development.Google Scholar
Freedle, R. & Lewis, M. (1977). Prelinguistic conversations. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. & Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Grieve, R. & Hoogenraad, R. (1979). First words. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Griffiths, M. (1967). The abilities of babies: a study in mental measurement. University of London Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean: explorations in the development of language. London: Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McShane, J. (1979). The development of naming. Linguistics 17, 879905.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. & Luccariello, J. (1985). The development of meaning in first words. In Barrett, M. D. (ed.), Children's single-word speech. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Norusis, M. J. (1988). SPSS/PC+ Studentware. Chicago, Ill: SPSS.Google Scholar
Nover, A. G. (1985). Mother–infant interactive play: theory and practical application. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal 2, 2235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. A. (1980). Overextensions in early language development. Journal of Child Language 7, 321–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynell, J. (1977). Reynell Developmental Language Scales (rev. ed.). Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Ricard, M. & Décarie, T. G. (1984). Observations du nourrisson et micro-ordinateur: considérations méthodologiques. Ecologie Ethologie Humaines 3, 129–37.Google Scholar
Shore, C. (1986). Combinatorial play, conceptual development and early multiword speech. Developmental Psychology 22, 184–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. B., Adamson, L. B. & Bakeman, R. (1988). Interactional predictors of early language. First Language 8, 143–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Bornstein, M. (1990). Language, play, and attention at one year. Infant Behaviour and Development 13, 8598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ungerer, J. A. & Sigman, M. (1984). The relation of play and sensorimotor behaviour to language in the second year. Child Development 55, 1448–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vibbert, S. & Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Specific associations between domains of mother–child interaction and toddler referential language and pretence play. Infant Behaviour and Development 12, 163–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar