Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-30T22:23:19.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How do parents of school-aged children respond to their children's extending gestures?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 February 2019

Charlotte WRAY*
Affiliation:
Oxford University
Natalie SAUNDERS
Affiliation:
University College London
Courtenay FRAZIER NORBURY
Affiliation:
University College London
*
*Corresponding author. University of Oxford, Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, OX3 7JX. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Gesture plays an important role in early language development, as how parents respond to their children's gestures may help to facilitate language acquisition. Less is known about whether parental responses facilitate language learning later in childhood and whether responses vary depending on children's language ability. This study explored parental responses to extending gestures in a sample of school-aged children (aged six to eight years) with developmental language disorder, low-language and educational concerns, and typically developing children. Overall there were no group differences in the types of responses parents provided to extending gestures. Parents predominantly responded with positive feedback but also displayed moderate proportions of verbal translations and clarification requests. Within the DLD group, the proportion of parent translations was negatively associated with language ability. Our finding suggests that parent responses serve to enhance communication and engage children in tasks, but there is limited evidence that they support new language learning at this age.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, C., Cooke, R., Hesketh, A., & Reeves, D. (2011). Assessment of Comprehension and Expression 6–11. Retrieved from <https://www.gl-assessment.co.uk/products/assessment-of-comprehension-and-expression-6-11-ace6-11/>..>Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: we think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(6), 593613.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). The Children's Communication Checklist: CCC-2. London: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Blake, J., Myszczyszyn, D., Jokel, A., & Bebiroglu, N. (2008). Gestures accompanying speech in specifically language-impaired children and their timing with speech. First Language, 28(2), 237–53.Google Scholar
Brownell, R. (2000a). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Brownell, R. (2000b). Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test: Manual. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Capone, N. C., & McGregor, K. K. (2004). Gesture development: a review for clinical and research practices. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 173–86.Google Scholar
Church, R., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23(1), 4371.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Colletta, J.-M., Pellenq, C., & Guidetti, M. (2010). Age-related changes in co-speech gesture and narrative: evidence from French children and adults. Speech Communication, 52(6), 565–76.Google Scholar
Dimitrova, N., Özçalışkan, Ş., & Adamson, L. B. (2015). Parents’ translations of child gesture facilitate word learning in children with autism, Down Syndrome and typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 221–31.Google Scholar
Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., & McNeil, N. M. (2001). Divergence of verbal expression and embodied knowledge: evidence from speech and gesture in children with specific language impairment. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2/3), 309–31.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., & Iverson, J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10(6), 778–85.Google Scholar
Grieco, F., Loijens, L., Zimmermann, P., & Spink, A. (2013). The Observer XT (Version 11.5). Wageningen: Noldus information Technology bv.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M., & Braddock, B. A. (2011). Gesture and motor skill in relation to language in children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 54(1), 7286.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9(1), 2343.Google Scholar
Marinis, T., Armon-Lotem, S., Piper, J., & Roy, P. (2011). School-age sentence imitation test-E32. Retrieved from <http://www.city.ac.uk/health/research/centre-for-language-communication-sciences-research/veps-very-early-processing-skills/veps-assessments>..>Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1982). Mothers’ responses to infants’ object-related gestures: influences on lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 9(1), 2330.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1995). Infants’ early verbal imitation and their later lexical development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41(3), 286306.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F., & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants’ spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(4), 405–26.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: what gestures reveal about thought. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Miller, J., & Iglesias, A. (2012). Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT). Middleton, WI: SALT Software, LLC.Google Scholar
Norbury, C. F., Gooch, D., Wray, C., Baird, G., Charman, T., Simonoff, E., … Pickles, A. (2016). The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(11), 1247–57.Google Scholar
Olson, J., & Masur, E. F. (2011). Infants’ gestures influence mothers’ provision of object, action and internal state labels. Journal of Child Language, 38(5), 1028–54.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., Adamson, L. B., & Dimitrova, N. (2016). Early deictic but not other gestures predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism. Autism, 20(6), 754–63.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(6), 1143–62.Google Scholar
Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Do parents lead their children by the hand? Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 481505.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323(5916), 951–3.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L., Özçalışkan, Ş., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: gesture's role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language, 28(2), 182–99.Google Scholar
Tellier, M. (2009). The development of gesture. In de Bot, K., Makoni, S., & Schrauf, R. (Eds.), Language development across the life-span (pp. 191216). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wray, C., & Norbury, C. F. (2018). Parents modify gesture according to task demands and child language needs. First Language: https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723718761729Google Scholar
Wray, C., Saunders, N., McGuire, R., Cousins, G., & Norbury, C. F. (2017). Gesture production in language impairment: it's quality, not quantity, that matters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(4), 969–82.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Wray et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S2

Download Wray et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15 KB