Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:22:34.390Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exposure to Foreign Languages through Live Interaction Can Facilitate Children’s Acceptance of Multiple Labeling Conventions across Languages

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 November 2022

Hyuna LEE
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Education, Yonsei University, 50 Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 03722, Republic of Korea
Hyun-joo SONG*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Yonsei University, 50 Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 03722, Republic of Korea
*
*Corresponding author: Dr. Hyun-joo Song, Department of Psychology, Yonsei University, 50 Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 03722, Korea. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The current research examined whether children’s expectations about labeling conventions can be influenced by limited exposure to a foreign language. Three- to four-year-old Korean children were presented with two speakers who each assigned a novel label either in Korean or Spanish to a novel object. Children were asked whether both labels were acceptable for the object. Children who had more exposure to a foreign language through live social interaction, but not through media, were more likely to accept both Korean and Spanish labels. These findings indicate the influence of social interaction in foreign language exposure on children’s understanding of different labeling conventions.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Every language community has its own agreements on how to use the language – that is, conventions of the language (Clark, Reference Clark2007). For example, a round, edible fruit with shiny red or green skin is referred to as an “apple” in English, but it is referred to as “sakwa” in Korean and “manzana” in Spanish. To learn a foreign language, one must accept that different languages have different labeling conventions. Do young children understand that an object can have multiple labels across languages? What kind of linguistic experience might help children to develop an awareness of different labeling conventions across languages? The current research investigated how experience with foreign languages influences children’s awareness of different labeling conventions across languages.

Previous research has explored children’s understanding of different labeling conventions by assessing whether they can override mutual exclusivity (i.e., the assumption that each object has only one label (e.g., Markman & Wachtel, Reference Markman and Wachtel1988)) when inferring the meaning of a novel word from a foreign language (Au & Glusman, Reference Au and Glusman1990; Byers-Heinlein, Chen & Xu, Reference Byers-Heinlein, Chen and Xu2014; Haryu, Reference Haryu1998; Merriman & Kutlesic, Reference Merriman and Kutlesic1993). From early in development, young children readily rely on this assumption to constrain the meaning of new words (Diesendruck & Markson, Reference Diesendruck and Markson2001; Markman, Wasow & Hansen, Reference Markman, Wasow and Hansen2003; Merriman & Bowman, Reference Merriman and Bowman1989). However, preschoolers can suspend mutual exclusivity when inferring the meaning of a novel word from a foreign language (Au & Glusman, Reference Au and Glusman1990; Byers-Heinlein et al., Reference Byers-Heinlein, Chen and Xu2014; Haryu, Reference Haryu1998; Lee, Kim, & Song, Reference Lee, Kim and Song2016; Merriman & Kutlesic, Reference Merriman and Kutlesic1993). For example, in Au and Glusman’s (Reference Au and Glusman1990) experiment, 3–5-year-old children were asked to find the referent of a novel word from a foreign language in the presence of two objects, one of which already had a name in their native language and the other of which was nameless. If they had not understood that an object can have multiple labels across languages, they should have chosen the nameless object under the mutual exclusivity assumption. However, the children chose objects randomly, suggesting that preschoolers have some implicit understanding that an object can have different labeling conventions across languages.

Children’s acceptance of different labels across languages is influenced by their linguistic experiences (Akhtar, Menjivar, Hoicka, & Sabbagh, Reference Akhtar, Menjivar, Hoicka and Sabbagh2012; Byers-Heinlein et al., Reference Byers-Heinlein, Chen and Xu2014; Menjivar & Akhtar, Reference Menjivar and Akhtar2017; Rojo & Echols, Reference Rojo and Echols2018). Specifically, bilingualism has been proposed to modulate children’s use of mutual exclusivity from infancy. In contrast to their monolingual peers, bilingual infants are less likely to use the mutual exclusivity strategy when interpreting the meaning of new words (Byers-Heinlein & Werker, Reference Byers‐Heinlein and Werker2009; Houston-Price, Caloghiris, & Raviglione, Reference Houston-Price, Caloghiris and Raviglione2010). When a novel label is presented with a familiar object, bilingual infants accept it as a second label whereas their monolingual counterparts interpret the word as describing a salient property of the object (Kandhadai, Hall, & Werker, Reference Kandhadai, Hall and Werker2017). Furthermore, bilingual toddlers have a better understanding of the fact that object labels differ across languages than monolingual children (Byers-Heinlein et al., Reference Byers-Heinlein, Chen and Xu2014).

Not only bilingual but also monolingual children who have mere exposure to (but not fluency in) a foreign language more readily accept different labels from foreign languages compared to those who have not had such exposure (Akhtar et al., Reference Akhtar, Menjivar, Hoicka and Sabbagh2012; Menjivar & Akhtar, Reference Menjivar and Akhtar2017; Rojo & Echols, Reference Rojo and Echols2018). For example, Rojo and Echols (Reference Rojo and Echols2018) tested English-speaking children who had been merely exposed to a nonnative language. In their experiment, children were asked whether a novel object can have two different novel labels, one from English and the other from Spanish. The results showed that children who had greater exposure to foreign languages were more likely to accept both the English and Spanish labels compared to children with less foreign language exposure.

Taken together, previous studies suggest that children’s amount of foreign language exposure influences their acceptance of different labeling conventions across languages. However, these studies do not address the characteristics of foreign language exposure that contribute to children’s understanding of different labeling conventions. The current research thus examined whether social aspects of foreign language exposure may be important for children to learn different communicative conventions across different languages, following theoretical considerations that traditionally consider social experience a critical factor that guides early language learning (Goldstein, King, & West, Reference Goldstein, King and West2003; Kuhl, Reference Kuhl2007; Nelson, Reference Nelson2007; Sage & Baldwin, Reference Sage and Baldwin2010; Vygotsky, Reference Vygotsky1962; Yusa, Kim, Koizumi, Sugiura, & Kawashima, Reference Yusa, Kim, Koizumi, Sugiura and Kawashima2017). Infants are more attentive to a human speaker than a non-human source of speech (e.g., an audio-speaker or TV monitor; Kuhl, Tsao, & Liu, Reference Kuhl, Tsao and Liu2003). Social capacities such as sensitivity to others’ eye gaze, establishing joint attention with others, and understanding others’ referential intentions are found to be the key building blocks of language acquisition (Baldwin, Reference Baldwin, Moore and Dunham1995; Bloom, Reference Bloom2002; Moll & Tomasello, Reference Moll and Tomasello2007). These findings suggest that language exposure in social interaction is very critical for children’s language learning in general.

Most of the previous evidence on the importance of social interaction in language learning comes from controlled experiments (Krcmar, Grela, & Lin, Reference Krcmar, Grela and Lin2007; Roseberry, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, Reference Roseberry, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2014; Roseberry, Hirsh-Pasek, Parish-Morris, & Golinkoff, Reference Roseberry, Hirsh-Pasek, Parish-Morris and Golinkoff2009). Children are typically presented with some pseudo-words from social or non-social sources in experimental settings and then their word learning is assessed. However, less is known about the effects of social interaction on children’s language learning in natural language situations. There is much more complexity and variation in natural language than in the limited linguistic input provided in laboratories for research. Although rare, there are relevant studies on the effect of social interaction on language learning in natural language-learning settings (DeLoache et al., Reference DeLoache, Chiong, Sherman, Islam, Vanderborght, Troseth and O’Doherty2010; Kuhl et al., Reference Kuhl, Tsao and Liu2003). For example, in Kuhl et al. (Reference Kuhl, Tsao and Liu2003), American 9-month-old infants heard large amounts of the natural complex language of Mandarin in three different ways, by live speakers, videos, or audio-only recordings. Infants were able to learn the phonetic features of Mandarin when they were exposed to it by live speakers but not when exposed via video or audio recordings. These findings suggest that live interaction exposure in natural language settings plays a critical role in children’s phonetic learning in a foreign language. More research is needed to investigate if live interaction exposure in natural language affects other aspects of language learning besides phonetic learning. It is possible that naturalistic foreign language exposure through social interaction may support not only children’s ability to discriminate different phonetic features of a foreign language but also children’s acquisition of labeling conventions across different languages.

The Current Research

The goal of the current study was to identify the types of foreign language experience that contribute to children’s acceptance of different labeling conventions across languages. Three- to four-year-old monolingual children who varied in their amounts of foreign language exposure were tested. We chose to test 3- and 4-year-olds because from the age of three, children understand that native and foreign language speakers do not share word knowledge (Diesendruck, Reference Diesendruck2005; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Kim and Song2016; Schell, Reference Schell2016). Children’s foreign language exposure was categorized into two different types: live interaction and media. A modified version of the paradigm developed by Rojo and Echols (Reference Rojo and Echols2018) was used to assess children’s acceptance of different labeling conventions. It was hypothesized that if foreign language exposure through social interaction supports children’s understanding of different labeling conventions, the extent of foreign language exposure through live interaction, but not through media, would be associated with children’s understanding of different labeling conventions.

Additionally, we examined the influences of other factors that are considered potential contributors to children’s acceptance of non-native words, such as foreign language fluency (Rojo & Echols, Reference Rojo and Echols2018), native language vocabulary (Koenig & Woodward, Reference Koenig and Woodward2012), and age (Haryu, Reference Haryu1998).

Method

Participants

Participants were 73 three- to four-year-old native Korean-speaking children (36 boys and 37 girls; age range = 3.56–4.89, M = 4.26 years, SD = 0.36 years). Another 17 children were excluded due to a parental report of language delay (n = 1), or failure to complete the task (n = 1) or to answer the test questions properly (n = 15). The last 15 children, who did not answer the test questions properly either, always said, “I don’t know.” (n = 2), said nonsense words that were never used in the current experiment (n = 3), or provided responses that were irrelevant to the test question (i.e., “My name is …”). There was no significant difference in mean age or foreign language exposure between these 15 participants and the final sample (ps > .21), but these 15 participants had significantly lower receptive and expressive vocabulary than the participants in the final sample (Receptive Vocabulary, t(80) = 3.54, p = .001; Expressive Vocabulary, t(82) = 2.89, p = .01).

Participants were recruited from Seoul, South Korea, and its surrounding areas. This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines and approval of the Institutional Ethics Review Board at the authors’ affiliated organization. The parents were offered reimbursement for their transportation expenses, and the children were given their choice of a book from a collection of age-appropriate books.

Design

To assess the effect of the types of foreign language exposure on children’s acceptance of different labeling conventions across languages, children’s weekly foreign language exposure was categorized into two different types (i.e., live interaction and media). Additionally, data on children’s native language vocabulary, fluency in a foreign language, and age were collected to investigate potential influences of these variables on children’s willingness to accept different labeling conventions. To measure children’s acceptance of different labels across languages, we used the labeling convention task, which was a modified version of the task from Rojo and Echols (Reference Rojo and Echols2018). The task had two test trials, and each trial tested children’s willingness to accept two different labeling conventions (Korean and Spanish) for a novel object.

Materials

Objects and Labels

Three familiar objects (a shoe, a teddy bear, and a toy bus) and two novel objects (unfamiliar, toy-like objects created for this study; see Figure 1) were used in the labeling convention task. The Korean labels for the familiar objects were “sinpal,” “komtoli,” and “pesu,” respectively, while the Spanish labels for these objects were “zapato,” “oso,” and “bus,” respectively. Novel pseudo-words described the two novel objects. The Korean labels for the two novel objects were “mwuppi” and “kkati,” and the corresponding Spanish labels were “bufo” and “chisa.”

Figure 1. Schematic for Labeling Convention Task

Note. *Korean translation into English.

Video Stimulus

The labeling convention task also used a video stimulus. In the video, two female Korean–Spanish bilingual speakers appeared in succession and took turns labeling the objects. Throughout the video, one speaker consistently spoke Korean, while the other consistently spoke Spanish. The language each speaker spoke and the order in which the two languages were presented were counterbalanced across participants.

Foreign Language Exposure Survey

The children’s parents were asked to complete a foreign language exposure survey in which they reported the average number of minutes that their child was exposed to a foreign language for each exposure type (live social interaction and media) per week. Each exposure type was further divided into three subtypes for live interaction (reading storybooks, having conversations, and taking language classes) and two subtypes for media (watching video content and listening to audio materials). Parents also indicated their children’s fluency in a foreign language on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (the child cannot understand any foreign languages) to 5 (the child can join a conversation in a foreign language).

Procedure

The experiment took place in a university laboratory. Parents were informed of the nature of the tasks and written informed consent was obtained from each child’s parents prior to the experiment. Parents also completed the foreign language exposure survey, while their child participated in the experiment in a separate room.

During the experiment, the experimenter sat next to the participant and conducted the labeling convention task which consisted of three phases: introduction, familiarization, and test. During the introduction phase, the experimenter presented a video in which a Korean speaker and a Spanish speaker appeared in succession and introduced themselves by using either a Korean name or a Spanish name. During the following familiarization phase, the speakers on the video took turns labeling three familiar objects (shoe, teddy bear, toy bus) in each language. Each speaker displayed one object at a time and labeled each object while holding it in both hands (see Appendix A). The speakers presented the object labels both in isolation and in a sentence. Each time they uttered an object label, the speaker moved the object slightly up and down once to draw the child’s attention to the referent. After both speakers labeled an object, children were presented with a scene in which the object was in the center and each of the speakers appeared in the upper-left or upper-right portions of the screen, respectively. Each speaker uttered the label twice in isolation in succession (e.g., “신발! 신발!” or “Zapato! Zapato!”). It was easy to tell which speaker was labeling the object from their mouth movement. The experimenter then asked the children to recall each of the labels to verify that they had attended to the video and had an opportunity to reflect on the different labels before being asked to endorse them in the test phase.

During the test phase, the children completed two test trials. In each test trial, children watched a video in which one novel object appeared, and each speaker labeled it either with a novel Korean or Spanish label. Once both speakers had labeled each object, the children were asked by the experimenter to recall the labels for the novel objects. Then, the experimenter tested the children’s willingness to accept the different labeling conventions. The experimenter asked the question twice, with the presentation of the novel labels in the question being reversed the second time. The following is an example sequence:

I cangnankam-uy olbalun irum-un mwe-ra-ko sayngkak-hay?

This toy-GEN right-ADJ name-NOM what-COP-COMP thought-do

‘Which do you think is the right name for this toy?

Mwuppi? Chisa? Ani-myen twul-ta kwaynchanh-a?

[Korean novel label]? [Spanish novel label]? not-if two-all okay-SentEnder

[Korean label]? [Spanish label]? Or are both okay?

Chisa? Mwuppi? Ani-myen twul ta kwaynchanh-a?”

[Spanish novel label]? [Korean novel label]? not-if two-all okay-SentEnder

[Spanish label]? [Korean label]? Or are both okay?

The objects and labels used in the test phase differed between the two test trials. Figure 1 depicts a schematic of this task. After the labeling convention task, children’s Korean vocabulary was assessed by the Receptive and Expressive Vocabulary Test (REVT; Kim, Hong, Kim, Jang, & Lee, Reference Kim, Hong, Kim, Jang and Lee2009). The test is subdivided into the Receptive (REVT-R) and the Expressive (REVT-E) Vocabulary Test, and the children completed the REVT-E and REVT-R in order, after which the experimenter thanked them and invited them to choose a book as a gift.

Coding

For each test trial, a score of 1 was assigned if a child accepted both labels, with a maximum score of 2 across the two test trials. Only responses made after the experimenter had finished asking the question (e.g., “What do you think is the right name for this toy? Mwuppi? Chisa? Or are both okay?”) were counted as children’s answers. Children’s responses to the test questions were coded by the experimenter in real time and videotaped as well for further off-line coding. Approximately 50% of the responses (n = 36) were coded again by an off-line coder watching the recorded video. The intercoder agreement was 100%.

Results

Labeling Convention Task

In the labeling convention task, 31 out of 73 children (42.5%) received a score of 2 by accepting both the Korean and Spanish labels in both test trials. Nineteen children (26.0%) received a score of 1 by accepting both the Korean and Spanish labels in one of the two test trials, and 23 children (31.5%) received a score of 0 by accepting only one label in both of the test trials. When children accepted only one label, children chose the Korean label (i.e., “mwuppi” or “kkati”) in 83.1% of the trials.

Foreign Language Exposure Survey

Most of the 73 children had been exposed to a foreign language. Seventy children (95.9%) had been exposed to English, and eight of them had also been exposed to Japanese or Chinese. Three children (4.1%) had no prior exposure to any foreign languages.

Regarding types of exposure, 46 children (63.0%) had been exposed to a foreign language through live interaction and media, 12 children (16.4%) through live interaction only, and 12 children (16.4%) through media only. The amount of exposure varied across the different types of exposure. On average, children’s amount of weekly foreign language exposure was 100 minutes (range = 0–600) through live interaction and 167 minutes (range = 0–1,260) through media. Table 1 shows the mean amount of foreign language exposure by subtype.

Table 1. Mean Amount of Weekly Foreign Language Exposure in Minutes by Type and Subtype

In terms of fluency, 15 children (20.6%) did not understand any foreign languages, 13 (17.8%) could understand only a few words in a foreign language, 39 (53.4%) could speak a few words in a foreign language (but could not understand a conversation in a foreign language), four (5.5%) could understand a conversation in a foreign language, and two (2.7%) could join a conversation in a foreign language. Fluency in a foreign language was considered as a continuous variable ranging from 1 (the child did not understand any foreign languages) to 5 (the child could join a conversation in a foreign language) in a later analysis.

Foreign Language Exposure and Acceptance of Labeling Conventions

The goal of the present study was to examine the types of foreign language exposure that predict children’s acceptance of different labeling conventions across languages. Before running an ordinal logistic regression, correlations among the potential independent variables were examined (see Appendix B). There was a strong correlation between REVT-R and REVT-E scores, r(67) = 0.73, p = .00. When the correlation coefficient is higher than .7, the multicollinearity problem may occur (Nijsse et al., Reference Nijsse, van Heugten, van Mierlo, Post, de Kort and Visser-Meily2017; Yu, Jiang, & Land, Reference Yu, Jiang and Land2015). Therefore, in order to minimize the multicollinearity, only REVT-E was included in the subsequent analyses as there were fewer missing data in REVT-E (n = 4) than in REVT-R (n = 6).

An ordinal logistic regression was conducted in order to evaluate whether the following variables predicted participants’ response scores: age, REVT-E score, fluency in a foreign language, foreign language exposure through live interaction, and foreign language exposure through media. Ordinal logistic regression analyses require that there be no missing data; therefore, data from children who did not complete the REVT-E (n = 4) were excluded. The results showed that only foreign language exposure through live interaction significantly predicted children’s response scores (p = .03, Nagelkerke pseudo-R 2 = .13). See Table 2 for the model summary of this analysis.

Table 2. Ordinal Regression Results

Note. REVT-E = Expressive Vocabulary Test. *p < .05.

Discussion

What types of foreign language exposure facilitate children’s awareness of different labeling conventions across languages? The current research explored this question by expanding the work of Rojo and Echols (Reference Rojo and Echols2018) in terms of the range of foreign language exposure sources. Previous research uncovered an association between exposure to foreign languages and children’s increased willingness to accept different labels across languages, but it only measured exposure through social interaction (Rojo & Echols, Reference Rojo and Echols2018). We gathered information on children’s foreign language exposure via media as well as through live social interaction and assessed whether each type of exposure has different effects on children’s acceptance of labels across languages. The results showed that 3–4-year-old children with more exposure to a foreign language through live social interaction were more likely to accept both the Korean and Spanish labels for an object. However, foreign language exposure through media was not related to children’s acceptance of different labeling conventions across languages. Thus, live interaction, but not media, appears to be the critical source of foreign language exposure that can help children better understand different labeling conventions across languages.

The current finding is consistent with theories describing the crucial contributions of social interaction to language acquisition (e.g., Kuhl et al., Reference Kuhl, Tsao and Liu2003; Roseberry et al., Reference Roseberry, Hirsh-Pasek, Parish-Morris and Golinkoff2009). Although media can provide easy access to native-speaker input, children’s understanding of labeling conventions in our study was only supported by exposure to foreign language through live interaction, which is assumed to be provided by Korean parents or teachers who are not always native speakers of the foreign language.

Then, what differentiates the experiences of live interactions and media in terms of their contributions to children’s understanding of different labeling conventions? Kuhl (Reference Kuhl2007) suggested two broad mechanisms to explain the advantage of social interaction in language learning: motivation and information. Social interactions enhance children’s attention and arousal. Although the current research did not measure children’s attention levels according to the type of exposure, relevant research suggests that children pay more attention to a live person than to other media sources (Kuhl et al., Reference Kuhl, Tsao and Liu2003). Additionally, social interaction may provide more information for children to use when learning words. Joint attention (Baldwin, Reference Baldwin, Moore and Dunham1995; Moll & Tomasello, Reference Moll and Tomasello2007), eye gaze (Baldwin, Reference Baldwin1993; Bloom, Reference Bloom2002), and contingent responses (Kuhl, Reference Kuhl2007; Roseberry et al., Reference Roseberry, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2014) in live social interactions are known to facilitate children’s language learning. In contrast, traditional media do not provide these social cues. Thus, our participants may have been more attentive to the foreign language and received richer information when they were exposed to it through live social interaction compared to media.

Our research participants had very limited foreign language exposure, and none of them was fluent in a foreign language. Prior research has suggested that bilingual children are more likely than monolinguals to accept multiple labels across languages (e.g., Byers-Heinlein et al., Reference Byers-Heinlein, Chen and Xu2014). Our findings suggest that relatively limited exposure to foreign languages can have impacts on children’s acceptance of multiple labels that are similar to the effects of bilingualism, regardless of children’s fluency in a foreign language. Like bilingual children, children with just a small amount of exposure to a foreign language might notice that, across the two languages, different labels are used for the same object. Furthermore, children who have just started to learn another language may be particularly interested in the fact that different languages can use different words to label the same objects (Akhtar et al., Reference Akhtar, Menjivar, Hoicka and Sabbagh2012). Notably, none of the children in our study had any prior exposure to Spanish. This implies that children accepted novel Spanish words as the labels for the novel objects not because they could use their previous knowledge of the language, but because they could infer that any language can provide valid labels for objects.

Our study has several limitations. First, we used parental self-reports to measure children’s foreign language exposure through live interaction and media. We acknowledge that it might have been difficult for parents to provide accurate assessments of their children’s media exposure. Second, children’s attention levels might have varied depending on the content and context of exposure, but the current research did not include a measure of children’s attention level during foreign language exposure. Live interaction, unlike video or audio, could provide children with explicit and focused demonstration of labeling an object in another language. For instance, parents can point to an object and mention that it has a different name in another language while reading a book. On the other hand, media exposure usually involves less parent-child interaction (Kirkorian, Pempek, Murphy, Schmidt, & Anderson, Reference Kirkorian, Pempek, Murphy, Schmidt and Anderson2009), and media might be played in the background, resulting in children’s reduced attention. Future studies should consider examining the effects of live exposure and focused media exposure on children’s acceptance of different labeling conventions after controlling for children’s attention level and the content of the foreign language exposure.

To conclude, the current research has shed light on how foreign language experiences may shape children’s understanding of different languages. Although the influence of the amount of foreign language exposure has been well studied, limited work has focused on the effect of the quality of such exposure on children’s language development. Our findings provide the first explicit assessment of the claim that social interaction in foreign language exposure is critical in shaping children’s understanding of different labeling conventions. Moreover, the current study has practical implications for foreign language education in linguistically homogeneous communities, highlighting the importance of social interaction in foreign language experience.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Yonsei University Research Grant of 2020. We wish to thank the parents and children who participated in the research.

Declarations of Interest Statement

None

Appendix A Example Script of the Speakers and the Experimenter

Appendix B Correlations among the variables

Notes. Fluency in a foreign language 1 = the child does not understand any foreign languages, 2 = the child can understand a few words in a foreign language, 3 = the child can speak a few words in a foreign language, 4 = the child can understand a conversation in a foreign language, 5 = the child can join a conversation in a foreign language

**p < .01, *p < .05.

References

Akhtar, N., Menjivar, J., Hoicka, E., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2012). Learning foreign labels from a foreign speaker: The role of (limited) exposure to a second language. Journal of Child Language, 39, 11351149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, T. K., & Glusman, M. (1990). The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: To honor or not to honor. Child Development, 61, 14741490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20, 395418 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P.J. (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131158). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2002). Mindreading, communication and the learning of names for things. Mind & Language, 17, 3754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Chen, K. H., & Xu, F. (2014). Surmounting the Tower of Babel: Monolingual and bilingual 2-year-olds’ understanding of the nature of foreign language words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 87100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers‐Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2009). Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: Infants’ language experience influences the development of a word‐learning heuristic. Developmental Science, 12, 815823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (2007). Conventionality and contrast in language and language acquisition. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2007(115), 1123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLoache, J. S., Chiong, C., Sherman, K., Islam, N., Vanderborght, M., Troseth, G. L., & O’Doherty, K. (2010). Do babies learn from baby media? Psychological Science, 21, 15701574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diesendruck, G. (2005). The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: an empirical examination. Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2001). Children’s avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37, 630641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P., & West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 80308035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haryu, E. (1998). Effects of knowledge about cross-language equivalents on children’s use of mutual exclusivity in interpreting novel labels. Japanese Psychological Research, 40(2), 8291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houston-Price, C., Caloghiris, Z., & Raviglione, E. (2010). Language experience shapes the development of the mutual exclusivity bias. Infancy, 15, 125150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandhadai, P., Hall, D. G., & Werker, J. F. (2017). Second label learning in bilingual and monolingual infants. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, Y., Hong, K., Kim, K., Jang, H., & Lee, J. (2009). Receptive Expressive Vocabulary Test (REVT) guidelines. Seoul Community Rehabilitation Center.Google Scholar
Kirkorian, H. L., Pempek, T. A., Murphy, L. A., Schmidt, M. E., & Anderson, D. R. (2009). The impact of background television on parent–child interaction. Child Development, 80, 13501359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, M. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2012). Toddlers learn words in a foreign language: The role of native vocabulary knowledge. Journal of Child Language, 39, 332337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krcmar, M., Grela, B., & Lin, K. (2007). Can toddlers learn vocabulary from television? An experimental approach. Media Psychology, 10, 4163.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning “gated” by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 90969101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, H., Kim, E., & Song, H. (2016). Monolingual two- to three-year-old children’s understanding of foreign words. Korean Journal of Child Studies, 37(4), 159168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, E. M., Wasow, J. L., & Hansen, M. B. (2003). Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 241275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menjivar, J., & Akhtar, N. (2017). Language experience and preschoolers’ foreign word learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 642648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merriman, W. E., & Bowman, L. L. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children’s word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54(3–4), 1129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, W. E., & Kutlesic, V. (1993). Bilingual and monolingual children’s use of two lexical acquisition heuristics. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 229249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How 14‐ and 18‐month‐olds know what others have experienced. Developmental Psychology, 43, 309317 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning, and memory. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nijsse, B., van Heugten, C. M., van Mierlo, M. L., Post, M. W., de Kort, P. L., & Visser-Meily, J. M. (2017). Psychological factors are associated with subjective cognitive complaints 2 months post-stroke. Neuropsychological rehabilitation, 27, 99115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rojo, D. P., & Echols, C. H. (2018). Non-native language exposure promotes children’s willingness to accept labels in two languages. Journal of Cognition and Development, 19, 107118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85, 956970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Parish-Morris, J., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2009). Live action: Can young children learn verbs from video? Child Development, 80, 13601375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sage, K. D., & Baldwin, D. (2010). Social gating and pedagogy: Mechanisms for learning and implications for robotics. Neural Networks, 23, 10911098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schell, V. (2016). Preschoolers restrict the scope of labels within their own linguistic group (Doctoral dissertation).Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, H., Jiang, S., & Land, K. C. (2015). Multicollinearity in hierarchical linear models. Social science research, 53, 118136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yusa, N., Kim, J., Koizumi, M., Sugiura, M., & Kawashima, R. (2017). Social interaction affects neural outcomes of sign language learning as a foreign language in adults. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Schematic for Labeling Convention TaskNote. *Korean translation into English.

Figure 1

Table 1. Mean Amount of Weekly Foreign Language Exposure in Minutes by Type and Subtype

Figure 2

Table 2. Ordinal Regression Results