Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T00:48:09.789Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of emotional cues on novel word learning in typically developing children in relation to broader autism traits

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 March 2021

Melina J WEST*
Affiliation:
School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
Anthony J ANGWIN
Affiliation:
School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
David A COPLAND
Affiliation:
School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia University of Queensland Center for Clinical Research, Brisbane, Australia
Wendy L ARNOTT
Affiliation:
School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
Nicole L NELSON
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
*
Address for correspondence: Melina J West Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road, U-1020, Storrs CT USA 06269. E-mail: [email protected];

Abstract

Emotion can influence various cognitive processes. Communication with children often involves exaggerated emotional expressions and emotive language. Children with autism spectrum disorder often show a reduced tendency to attend to emotional information. Typically developing children aged 7 to 9 years who varied in their level of autism-like traits learned the nonsense word names of nine novel toys, which were presented with either happy, fearful, or neutral emotional cues. Emotional cues had no influence on word recognition or recall performance. Eye-tracking data showed differences in visual attention depending on the type of emotional cues and level of autism-like traits. The findings suggest that the influence of emotion on attention during word learning differs according to whether the children have lower or higher levels of autism-like traits, but this influence does not affect word learning outcomes.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aguert, M., Laval, V., Lacroix, A., Gil, S., & Bigot, L. L. (2013). Inferring emotions from speech prosody: Not so easy at age five. Plos One, 8(12), 18. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083657CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Allison, C. (2008). The autism spectrum quotient: Children's version (AQ-Child). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 12301240. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0504-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker's direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68(1), 4857. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01924.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 517. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedford, R., Gliga, T., Frame, K., Hudry, K., Chandler, S., Johnson, M. H, Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Elsabbagh, M., Fernandes, J., Garwood, H., Pasco, G., Tucker, L., & Volein, A. (2013). Failure to learn from feedback underlies word learning difficulties in toddlers at risk for autism. Journal of Child Language, 40, 2946. doi: 10.1017/S0305000912000086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begeer, S., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Stockman, L. (2006). Attention to facial emotion expressions in children with autism. Autism, 10(1), 3751. doi: 10.1177/1362361306057862CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergen, P. V., Wall, J., & Salmon, K. (2015). The good, the bad, and the neutral: The influence of emotional valence on young children's recall. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4, 2935. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, M. J., Graham, S. A., Callaway, D., & Chambers, C. G. (2013). Preschoolers use emotion in speech to learn new words. Child Development, 85(5), 17911805. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12074CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). CCC-2; Children's Communication Checklist-2, United States Edition, manual. San Antonio, TX: PearsonGoogle Scholar
Boucher, J. (2012). Research review: Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder – characteristics and causes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(3), 219233. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02508.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brady, K. W., & Goodman, J. C. (2014). The type, but not the amount, of information available influences toddlers’ fast mapping and retention of new words. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(2), 120133. doi: 10.1044/2013_AJSLP-13-0013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 1729.Google Scholar
Christodoulou, J., & Burke, D. M. (2016). Mood congruity and episodic memory in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 221229. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clement, F., Bernard, S., Grandjean, D., & Sander, D. (2013). Emotional expression and vocabulary learning in adults and children. Cognition and Emotion, 27(3), 539548. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.724012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70(4), 853865. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, D., Luo, Z., & Burden, M. J. (2001). Children's recall of emotional behaviours, emotional labels, and nonemotional behaviours: Does emotion enhance memory? Cognition and Emotion, 15(1), 126. doi: 10.1080/02699930125794CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Carver, L., Panagiotides, H., & McPartland, J. (2004). Young children with autism show atypical brain responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions of emotion. Developmental Science, 7(3), 340359. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00352.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doan, S. N. (2010). The role of emotion in word learning. Early Child Development and Care, 180(8), 10651078. doi: 10.1080/03004430902726479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). Understanding others, and individual differences in friendship interactions in young children. Social Development, 8(2), 201219. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00091CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Floccia, C., Keren-Portnoy, T., DePaolis, R., Duffy, H., Luche, C. D., Durrant, S., White, L., Goslin, J., & Vihman, M. (2016). British English infants segment words only with exaggerated infant-direct speech stimuli. Cognition, 148, 19. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, D., & Mallery, P. (2010). SPSS for Windows step by step : a simple guide and reference, 17.0 update. 10th ed. Boston: Allyn & BaconGoogle Scholar
Halberda, J. (2006). Is this a dax which I see before me? Use of the logical argument disjunctive syllogism supports word-learning in children and adults. Cognitive Psychology, 53(4), 310344. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herold, D. S., Nygaard, L. C., Chicos, K. A., & Namy, L. L. (2011). The developing role of prosody in novel word interpretation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 229241. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinojosa, J. A., Rtolo, C. M. N.-b., & Pozo, M. A. (2010). Looking at emotional words is not the same as reading emotional words: Behavioral and neural correlates. Psychophysiology, 47, 748757. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00982.xGoogle Scholar
Holodynski, M. (2004). The miniaturization of expression in the development of emotional self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 40(1), 1628. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.1.16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horst, J. S., & Hout, M. C. (2014). The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: a collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Hosokawa, M., Nakadoi, Y., Watanabe, Y., Sumitani, S., & Ohmori, T. (2015). Association of autism tendency and hemodynamic changes in the prefrontal cortex during facial expression stimuli measured by multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 69, 145152. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12240CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Immordino-Yang, M. H. (2016). Emotion, sociality, and the brain's default mode network: Insights for educational practice and policy. Policy Insights from the Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 3(2), 211219. doi: 10.1177/2372732216656869CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingersoll, B. (2010). Broader autism phenotype and nonverbal sensitivity: Evidence for an association in the general population. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 590598. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0907-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnels, J. A., Hovey, D., Zurcher, N., Hippolyte, L., Lemonnier, E., Gillberg, C., & Hadjikhani, N. (2017). Autism and emotional face viewing. Autism Research, 10, 901910. doi: 10.1002/aur.1730CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 345360. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventon, J. S., & Bauer, P. J. (2016). Emotion regulation during the encoding of emotional stimuli: Effects on subsequent memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 312333. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melling, R., & Swinson, J. M. (2016). The relationship between Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and uneven intellectual development in school-age children. Cogent Psychology, 3, 112. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2016.1149136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mondloch, C. J. (2012). Sad or fearful? The influence of body posture on adults’ and children's perception of facial displays of emotion. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 180196. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. G. K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Jusczyk, P. W., & Cassidy, K. W. (1989). How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. Journal of Child Language, 16, 5568. doi: 10.1017/S030500090001343XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C. F., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2010). Sound before meaning: Word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsychologia, 48, 40124019. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuske, H. J., Vivanti, G., & Dissanayake, C. (2014). Reactivity to fearful expressions of familiar and unfamiliar people in children with autism: An eye-tracking pupillometry study. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6(14), 116. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-6-14CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petalas, M. A., Hastings, R. P., Nash, S., Hall, L. M., Joannidi, H., & Dowey, A. (2012). Psychological adjustment and sibling relationships in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders: Environmental stressors and the broad autism phenotype. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 546555. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.07.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piven, J., Palmer, P., Landa, R., Santangelo, S., Jacobi, D., & Childress, D. (1997). Personality and language characteristics in parents from multiple-incidence autism families. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 74, 398411. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19970725)74:4<398::AID-AJMG11>3.0.CO;2-D3.0.CO;2-D>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poljac, E., Poljac, E., & Wagemans, J. (2012). Reduced accuracy and sensitivity in the perception of emotional facial expressions in individuals with high autism spectrum traits. Autism, 17(6), 668680. doi: 10.1177/1362361312455703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prizant, B. M. (1983). Language acquisition and communicative behaviour in autism: Toward an understanding of the “whole” of it. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 296307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruzich, E., Allison, C., Smith, P., Ring, H., Auyeung, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2016). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient in siblings of people with autism. Autism Research, 9(10), 11141123. doi: 10.1002/aur.1689CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sauter, D. A., Panattoni, C., & Happe, F. (2013). Children's recognition of emotions from vocal cues. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 97113. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012.02081.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, D., Izard, C. E., & Bear, G. A. (2004). Children's emotion processing: Relations to emotionality and aggression. Development & Psychopathology, 16, 371387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2004). Preference and processing: The role of speech affect in early spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 173189. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.04.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenenbaum, E. J., Amso, D., Abar, B., & Sheinkopf, S. J. (2014). Attention and word learning in autistic, language delayed, and typically developing children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(490), 19. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thurman, A. J., McDuffe, A., Kover, S. T., Hagerman, R., Channell, M. M., Mastergeorge, A., & Abbeduto, L. (2015). Use of emotional cues for lexical learning: A comparison of autism spectrum disorder and fragile X syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 10421061. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2260-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M., & Desjardins, R. N. (2000). Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science, 11(3), 188195. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00240CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsang, V. (2016). Eye-tracking study on facial emotion recognition tasks in individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 22(2), 110. doi: 10.1177/1362361316667830Google Scholar
Tyng, C. M., Amin, H. U., Saad, M. N. M., & Malik, A. S. (2017). The influences of emotion on learning and memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1454), 122. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Snitzer Reilly, J., Pasqualetti, P., Vizzotto, A., & Caltagirone, C. (2000). Recognition of facial expressions of emotions in school-aged children: The intersection of perceptual and semantic categories. Acta Paediatrica, 89, 836845. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2000.tb00392.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, M. J., Copland, D. A., Arnott, W. L., Nelson, N. L., & Angwin, A. J. (2017). Effects of emotional prosody on novel word learning in relation to autism-like traits. Motivation and Emotion, 41(6), 749759. doi: 10.1007/s11031-017-9642-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widen, S. C. (2013). Children's interpretation of facial expressions: The long path from valence-based to specific discrete categories. Emotion Review, 5(1), 7277. doi: 10.1177/1754073912451492CrossRefGoogle Scholar