Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T08:25:22.045Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Auditory perception and word recognition in Cantonese-Chinese speaking children with and without Specific Language Impairment*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2015

JOANNA C. KIDD
Affiliation:
The University of Hong Kong
KATHY K. SHUM
Affiliation:
The University of Hong Kong
ANITA M.-Y. WONG
Affiliation:
The University of Hong Kong
CONNIE S.-H. HO
Affiliation:
The University of Hong Kong
TERRY K. AU*
Affiliation:
The University of Hong Kong
*
Address for correspondence: Terry Kit-fong Au, Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong, China. tel: (852) 3917 2383; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Auditory processing and spoken word recognition difficulties have been observed in Specific Language Impairment (SLI), raising the possibility that auditory perceptual deficits disrupt word recognition and, in turn, phonological processing and oral language. In this study, fifty-seven kindergarten children with SLI and fifty-three language-typical age-matched controls were assessed with a speech-gating task to measure spoken word recognition, psychophysical tasks to measure auditory Frequency Modulation (FM) detection and Frequency Discrimination (FD), and standardized psychometric tests of phonological processing and oral language. As a group, children with SLI took significantly longer than language-typical controls to recognize words with high neighborhood density, perhaps reflecting subpar phonological representations. FM, but not FD, was significantly worse in SLI. However, while both poorer speech-gating performance and poorer auditory thresholds (FM) were evident in SLI, spoken word recognition did not mediate any relation between auditory perception and either phonological processing or oral language.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We thank the children, parents, and staff of participating kindergartens, speech-language therapists at the Child Assessment Services (Hong Kong SAR), and our team of dedicated research assistants for their support. We thank K. K. Luke for sharing his Cantonese spoken-word corpus, and John Hogben and Genevieve McArthur for helpful discussions. This work was supported by the Hong Kong Research Grant Council (Grant GRF753308H to Anita Wong) and the University of Hong Kong (Grant 200611159229 to Terry Au, and a post-doc fellowship to Joanna Kidd).

References

REFERENCES

Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language 38, 419–39.Google Scholar
Anthony, J. L., Williams, J. M., Aghara, R. G., Dunkelberger, M., Novak, B. & Mukherjee, A. D. (2010). Assessment of individual differences in phonological representation. Reading & Writing 23, 969–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, P. J. & Snowling, M. J. (2002). Auditory processing and the development of language and literacy. British Medical Bulletin 63, 135–46.Google Scholar
Bauer, R. S. & Benedict, P. K. (1997). Modern Cantonese phonology. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Benasich, A. A. & Tallal, P. (2002). Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment. Behavioral Brain Research 136, 3149.Google Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin 107, 238–46.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: development and disorder in language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Carlyon, R. P., Deeks, J. M. & Bishop, S. J. (1999). Auditory temporal processing impairment: neither necessary nor sufficient for causing language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42, 1295–310.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin 130, 858–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bollen, K. A. (1989). A new incremental fit index for general structural equation models. Sociological Methods & Research 17, 303–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briscoe, J., Bishop, D. V. & Norbury, C. F. (2001). Phonological processing, language, and literacy: a comparison of children with mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss and those with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 329–40.Google Scholar
Charles-Luce, J. & Luce, P. A. (1990). Similarity neighbourhoods of words in young children's lexicons. Journal of Child Language 17, 205–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ching, Y. C. (1984). Lexical tone pattern learning in Cantonese children. Language Learning and Communication 3, 317–34.Google Scholar
Ciocca, V. & Lui, J. Y.-K. (2003). The development of the perception of Cantonese lexical tones. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders 1, 141–7.Google Scholar
Claessen, M. & Leitão, S. (2012). Phonological representations in children with SLI. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 28, 211–23.Google Scholar
Claessen, M., Leitão, S., Kane, R. & Williams, C. (2013). Phonological processing skills in specific language impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 15, 471–83.Google Scholar
Corriveau, K., Pasquini, E. & Goswami, U. (2007). Basic auditory processing skills and specific language impairment: a new look at an old hypothesis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50, 647–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C. (1998). Spoken word recognition in children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 19, 193207.Google Scholar
Estes, K. G., Evans, J. L. & Else-Quest, N. M. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50, 177–95.Google Scholar
Fowler, A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In Brady, S. & Shankweiler, D. (eds), Phonological processes in literacy: a tribute to Isabelle Y. Libermann (pp. 97117). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Frisina, R. D. (2001). Subcortical neural coding mechanisms for auditory temporal processing. Hearing Research 158, 127.Google Scholar
Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C. & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency, and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language 45, 468–92.Google Scholar
Griffiths, Y. M. & Snowling, M. J. (2001). Auditory word identification and phonological skills in dyslexic and average readers. Applied Psycholinguistics 22, 419–39.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1980). Spoken word recognition processes and the gating paradigm. Perception & Psychophysics 28, 267–83.Google Scholar
Halliday, L. F. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). Is poor frequency modulation detection linked to literacy problems? A comparison of specific reading disability and mild to moderate sensorineural hearing loss. Brain and Language 97, 200–13.Google Scholar
Heath, S. M., Bishop, D. V. M., Hogben, J. H. & Roach, N. W. (2006). Psychophysical indices of perceptual functioning in dyslexia: a psychometric analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology 23, 905–29.Google Scholar
Heath, S. M., Hogben, J. H. & Clark, C. D. (1999). Auditory temporal processing in disabled readers with and without oral language delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40, 637–47.Google Scholar
Hick, R. F., Botting, N. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2005). Short-term memory and vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 47, 532–8.Google Scholar
Hill, P. R., Hogben, J. H. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2005). Auditory frequency discrimination in children with specific language impairment: a longitudinal study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48, 1136–46.Google Scholar
Ho, C. S. H., Leung, M. T. & Cheung, H. (2011). Early difficulties of Chinese preschoolers at familial risk for dyslexia: deficits in oral language, phonological processing skills, and print-related skills. Dyslexia 17, 143–64.Google Scholar
Hu, L. T. & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 6, 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joanisse, M. F. & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Specific language impairment: a deficit in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2, 240–7.Google Scholar
Kidd, J. C., Shum, K. K. M., Ho, C. S. H. & Au, T. K. F. (2015). Phonological representations and early literacy in Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading 19, 89113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, R. B. (2004). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guildford.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Linguistic Society of Hong Kong (1994). The LSHK Cantonese Romanization Scheme. Hong Kong: Linguistic Society of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Mainela-Arnold, E., Evans, J. L. & Coady, J. (2008). Lexical representation in children with SLI: evidence from a frequency-manipulated gating task. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 51, 381–93.Google Scholar
Marshall, C. R. & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2008). Recognition of gated verbs by children with Grammatical-Specific Language Impairment: effects of inflection and frequency. Journal of Neurolinguistics 22, 433–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition 25, 71102.Google Scholar
McArthur, G. M. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2001). Auditory perceptual processing in people with reading and oral language impairments: current issues and recommendations. Dyslexia 7, 150–70.Google Scholar
McArthur, G. M. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004a). Frequency discrimination deficits in people with specific language impairment: reliability, validity, and linguistic correlates. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47, 527–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. M. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004b). Which people with specific language impairment have auditory processing deficits? Cognitive Neuropsychology 21, 7994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArthur, G. M. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2005). Speech and non-speech processing in people with specific language impairment: a behavioural and electrophysiological study. Brain and Language 94, 260–73.Google Scholar
McArthur, G. M., Ellis, D., Atkinson, C. M. & Coltheart, M. (2008). Auditory processing deficits in children with reading and language impairments: Can they (and should they) be treated? Cognition 107, 946–77.Google Scholar
McMurray, B., Munson, C. & Tomblin, J. B. (2014). Individual differences in language ability are related to variation in word recognition, not speech perception: evidence from eye movements. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 57, 1344–62.Google Scholar
McMurray, B., Samelson, V. M., Lee, S. H. & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology 60, 139.Google Scholar
Mengler, E. D., Hogben, J. H., Mitchie, P. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2005). Poor frequency discrimination is related to oral language disorder in children: a psychoacoustic study. Dyslexia 11, 155–73.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. (1997). An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken-word recognition. Memory & Cognition 25, 4756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J. L., Stavrinos, D. & Walley, A. C. (2009). Children's spoken word recognition and contributions to phonological awareness and nonword repetition: a 1-year follow-up. Applied Psycholinguistics 30, 101–21.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (eds), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. (1999). Recognition of gated words by children with specific language impairment: an examination of lexical mapping. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42, 735–43.Google Scholar
Nickisch, A. & Massinger, C. (2009). Auditory processing in children with specific language impairments: Are there deficits in frequency discrimination, temporal auditory processing or general auditory processing? International Journal of Phoniatrics, Speech Therapy and Communication Pathology 61, 323–32.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1986). Hong Kong supplement guide to the Standard Progressive Matrices. Hong Kong: HKSAR Government, Education Department.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. (2013). Language growth and genetics of specific language impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 15, 223–33.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Redmond, S. M. & Hoffman, L. (2006). Mean length of utterance in children with specific language impairment and in younger control children shows concurrent validity and stable and parallel growth trajectories. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 49, 793808.Google Scholar
Rinker, T., Kohls, G., Richter, C., Maas, V., Schultz, E. & Schecker, M. (2007). Abnormal frequency discrimination in children with SLI as indexed by mismatch negativity (MMN). Neuroscience Letters 413, 99104.Google Scholar
Roach, N. W., Edwards, V. T. & Hogben, J. H. (2004). The tale is in the tail: an alternative hypothesis for psychophysical performance variability in dyslexia. Perception 33, 817–30.Google Scholar
Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: Is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? Journal of Phonetics 31, 509–27.Google Scholar
Sereno, J. A. & Lee, H. (2015). The contribution of segmental and tonal information in Mandarin spoken word processing. Language and Speech 58, 131–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., McClean, R. & Matthews, R. (2002). Temporal processing and reading disability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 15, 151–78.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.Google ScholarPubMed
Stefanatos, G. A., Green, G. G. R. & Ratcliff, G. G. (1989). Neurophysiological evidence of auditory channel anomalies in developmental aphasia. Achieves of Neurology 46, 871–5.Google Scholar
Stokes, S. F. (2014). The impact of phonological neighborhood density on typical and atypical emerging lexicons. Journal of Child Language 41, 634–57.Google Scholar
Sutcliffe, P. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2005). Psychophysical design influences frequency discrimination performance in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 91, 249–70.Google Scholar
Sutherland, D. & Gillon, G. T. (2005). Assessment of phonological representations in children with speech impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 36, 294307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language 9, 182–98.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. (2000). Experimental studies of language learning impairments: from research to remediation. In Bishop, D. V. M. & Leonard, L. B. (eds), Speech and language impairments in children: causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 131155). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia 12, 8393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
To, C., Cheung, H., & T'sou, B. (2007). Hong Kong Cantonese Oral Language Assessment Scale: a further validation study. Paper presented at the First China International Conference for Speech Therapy, Beijing.Google Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Abbas, P. J., Records, N. L. & Brenneman, L. M. (1995). Auditory evoked responses to frequency-modulated tones in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 387–92.Google Scholar
T'sou, B., Lee, T. H.-T., Tung, P., Man, Y., Chan, A., To, C. K. S. & Chan, Y. (2006). Hong Kong Cantonese Oral Language Assessment Scale. Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L. & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 16, 520.Google Scholar
Wetherill, G. B. & Levitt, H. (1965). Sequential estimation of points on a psychometric function. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology 18, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wightman, F., Allen, P., Dolan, T., Kistler, D. & Jamieson, D. (1989). Temporal resolution in children. Child Development 60, 611–24.Google Scholar
Wong, A. M. Y., Ciocca, V. & Yung, S. (2009). The perception of lexical tone contrasts in Cantonese children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 52, 1493–509.Google Scholar
Wong, A. M. Y., Kidd, J. C., Ho, C. S. H. & Au, T. K. (2010a). Characterizing the overlap between SLI and dyslexia in Chinese: the role of phonology and beyond. Scientific Studies of Reading 14, 3057.Google Scholar
Wong, A. M. Y., Klee, T., Stokes, S. F., Fletcher, P. & Leonard, L. B. (2010b). Differentiating Cantonese-speaking preschool children with and without SLI using MLU and lexical diversity (D). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53, 794–9.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C. & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin 121, 329.Google Scholar