Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-01T12:23:36.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessing the impact of conversational overlap in content on child language growth*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 April 2017

ELIZABETH S. CHE*
Affiliation:
The College of Staten Island and The Graduate Center, CUNY
PATRICIA J. BROOKS*
Affiliation:
The College of Staten Island and The Graduate Center, CUNY
MARIA F. ALARCON
Affiliation:
Long Island University
FRANCIS D. YANNACO
Affiliation:
The Graduate Center, CUNY
SEAMUS DONNELLY
Affiliation:
Australian National University
*
Address for correspondence: Elizabeth Che or Patricia Brooks, Department of Psychology, College of Staten Island, CUNY, 2800 Victory Blvd. 4S-108, Staten Island, NY 10314. e-mail: [email protected]; [email protected]
Address for correspondence: Elizabeth Che or Patricia Brooks, Department of Psychology, College of Staten Island, CUNY, 2800 Victory Blvd. 4S-108, Staten Island, NY 10314. e-mail: [email protected]; [email protected]

Abstract

When engaged in conversation, both parents and children tend to re-use words that their partner has just said. This study explored whether proportions of maternal and/or child utterances that overlapped in content with what their partner had just said contributed to growth in mean length of utterance (MLU), developmental sentence score, and vocabulary diversity over time. We analyzed the New England longitudinal corpus from the CHILDES database, comprising transcripts of mother–child conversations at 14, 20, and 32 months, using the CHIP command to compute proportions of utterances with overlapping content. Rates of maternal overlap, but not child overlap, at earlier time-points predicted child language outcomes at later time-points, after controlling for earlier child MLU. We suggest that maternal overlap plays a formative role in child language development by providing content that is immediately relevant to what the child has in mind.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Preliminary analyses were presented at the 2015 Boston University Conference on Language Development. We thank Brian MacWhinney for his assistance with the CLAN commands and David Rindskopf for his advice on the statistical analysis. First authorship is shared by Elizabeth S. Che and Patricia J. Brooks.

References

REFERENCES

Akhtar, N., Dunham, F., & Dunham, P. J. (1991). Directive interactions and early vocabulary development: the role of joint attentional focus. Journal of Child Language, 18(1), 4149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K., Russell, A., & Wassenberg, K. (1987). Turn taking affects the quality of infant vocalizations. Journal of Child Language, 14, 211227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L., Hood, L., & Lightbown, P. (1974). Imitation in language development: if, when, and why. Cognitive Psychology, 6(3), 380420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohannon, J. N., & Stanowicz, L. B. (1988). The issue of negative evidence: adult responses to children's language errors. Developmental Psychology, 24(5), 684689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C. S., & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 867874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk: learning to use language. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy, 8(3), 253278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Che, E. S., Alarcon, M. F., Yannaco, F. D., & Brooks, P. J. (2016). Maternal overlap of child utterances predicts language outcomes for typical and late-talking children. Proceedings of the 40th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Volume 1 (pp. 4760). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Chouinard, M. M., & Clark, E. V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 637669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). The now-or-never bottleneck: a fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e62. Online: <DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1500031X>.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (2007). Young children's uptake of new words in conversation. Language in Society, 36(2), 157182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (2010). Adult offer, word-class, and child uptake in early lexical acquisition. First Language, 30(3/4), 250269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., & Bernicot, J. (2008). Repetition as ratification: how parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language, 35(2), 349371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, R., & Spivey, M. (2006). Unraveling the dyad: using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers in conversation. Language Learning, 56(3), 391430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demetras, M., Post, K., & Snow, C. (1986). Feedback to first-language learners. Journal of Child Language, 13, 275292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doupe, A. J., & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 567631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunst, C. J., Gorman, E., & Hamby, D. W. (2010). Effects of adult verbal and vocal contingent responsiveness on increases in infant vocalizations. Center for Early Literacy Learning Reviews, 3(1), 111.Google Scholar
Farrar, M. (1990). Discourse and acquisition of grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language, 17, 607624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrar, M. (1992). Negative evidence and grammatical morpheme acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 28, 9199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldstein, S., Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M., Fox, H., & Gordon, S. (1993). Coordinated interpersonal timing in adult–infant vocal interactions: a cross-site replication. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 455470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(2), 181195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Clark, A. (1993). The development of dialogue co-ordination skills in schoolchildren. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(1), 101126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P., & West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(13), 80308035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19(5), 515523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., & King, A. P. (2014). Maternal responsiveness and the development of directed vocalizing in social interactions. Infancy, 19(4), 385408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., & King, A. P. (2016). The influence of interactive context on prelinguistic vocalizations and maternal responses. Language Learning and Development, 12(3), 280294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Treiman, R., & Schneiderman, M. (1984). Brown & Hanlon revisited: mothers’ sensitivity to ungrammatical forms. Journal of Child Language, 11(1), 8188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsburg, E. (1990). Maternal speech and the child's development of syntax: a further look. Journal of Child Language, 17, 8599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. S. (2007). Imitation in infancy: the development of mimicry. Psychological Science, 18(7), 593599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, A. P., West, M. J., & Goldstein, M. H. (2005). Non-vocal shaping of avian song development: parallels to human speech development. Ethology, 111, 101117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., … & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277(5326), 684686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant vocalizations in response to speech: vocal imitation and developmental change. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100(401), 24252438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(15), 90969101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, L., & Canter, S. (1971). Developmental sentence scoring: a clinical procedure for estimating syntactic development in children's spontaneous speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 36, 315340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Locke, J. L. (2001). First communion: the emergence of vocal relationships. Social Development, 10(3), 294308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk, 3rd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, C. (1985). The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language, 12, 271295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masur, E. F. (1995). Infants’ early verbal imitation and their later lexical development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41(3), 286306.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F., & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants’ spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(4), 405426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGillion, M. L., Herbert, J. S., Keren-Portnoy, T., Vihman, M. M., & Matthews, D. E. (2013). Supporting early vocabulary development: What sort of responsiveness matters? IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 5(3), 240248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKee, G., Malvern, D., & Richards, B. (2000). Measuring vocabulary diversity using dedicated software. Literacy & Linguistic Computing, 15(3), 323338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A., Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A., & Rollins, P. R. (1994). Classifying communicative acts in children's interactions. Journal of Communications Disorders, 27, 157187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papoušek, M., & Papoušek, H. (1989). Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants. First Language, 9(26), 137157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2015). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. [Computer software] Vienna:R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Online: <https://www.r-project.org/>..>Google Scholar
Rochat, P. (2007). Intentional action arises from early reciprocal exchanges. Acta Psychologica, 124(1), 825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sasaki, A., Sotnikova, T. D., Gainetdinov, R. R., & Jarvis, E. D. (2006). Social context-dependent singing-regulated dopamine. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 90109014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, L., Morgan, J. L., & Best, C. T. (2002). Infants’ listening preferences: Baby talk or happy talk? Infancy, 3(3), 365394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. (1989). Understanding social interaction and language acquisition: sentences are not enough. In Bornstein, M. H. & Bruner, J. S. (Eds.), Interaction in human development (pp. 83103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A., Imbens-Bailey, A., & Herman, J. (1996). Learning how to say what one means: a longitudinal study of children's speech act use. Social Development, 5(1), 5684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolov, J. L. (1993). A local contingency analysis of the fine-tuning hypothesis. Developmental Psychology, 29(6), 10081023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolov, J. L., & MacWhinney, B. (1990). The CHIP framework: automatic coding and analysis of parent–child conversational interaction. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 22(2), 151161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Syal, S., & Finlay, B. (2011). Thinking outside the cortex: social motivation in the evolution and development of language. Developmental Science, 14(2), 417430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72(3), 748767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). Intention reading and imitative learning. In Hurley, S. & Chater, N. (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: from neuroscience to social science: Vol. 2. Imitation, human development, and culture (pp. 133148). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wu, Z., & Gros-Louis, J. (2014). Infants’ prelinguistic communicative acts and maternal responses: relations to linguistic development. First Language, 34(1), 7290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar