Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T22:09:53.975Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age-relation, reliability and grammatical validity of measures of utterance length

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Jean A. Rondal*
Affiliation:
University of Liège
Martine Ghiotto
Affiliation:
University of Liège
Serge Bredart
Affiliation:
University of Liège
Jean-François Bachelet
Affiliation:
University of Liège
*
Laboratory for Language Psychology, University of Liège, B-32, Sart Tilman, 4000 Liège, Belgium

Abstract

This study evaluated the age-relationship, intra-sample reliability and grammatical validity of MLU-like measures of utterance length in the free speech of 21 normally developing children aged 1;8 to 2;8 with MLU between 1·05 and 3·06. Contrary to a previous study by Klee & Fitzgerald (1985). it was found that measures of utterance length hold a good relation with age, are reliable and predict grammatical development well in the age and MLU range considered. It is argued that there are identifiable points in the developmental evolution of MLU and MLU-like indices beyond which their reliability and validity have serious problems. Below these points, they constitute sound measures of syntactic development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D. (1979). Working with LARSP. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Crystal, D., Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (1976). The grammatical analysis of language disability: a procedure for assessment and remediation. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Cunningham, C., Reuler, J., Blackwell, M. & Deck, J. (1981). Behavioral and linguistic developments in the interactions of normal and retarded children with their mothers. Child Development 52. 6270.Google Scholar
Darley, F. & Moll, K. (1960). Reliability of language measures and size of language sample. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 3. 166–73.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. & de Villiers, P. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2. 267–78.Google Scholar
Erreich, A. (1980). Why you won't play with me? Paper presented to the Fifth Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development.Google Scholar
Hollingshead, A. (1957). Two-factor index of social position. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University.Google Scholar
Klee, T. & Fitzgerald, M. (1985). The relation between grammatical development and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Child Language 12. 251–69.Google Scholar
Kramer, C., James, S. & Saxman, J. (1979). A comparison of language samples elicited at home and in the clinic. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 44. 321–30.Google Scholar
Lord, F. & Novick, M. (1968). Statistical theories of mental test scores. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Miller, J. & Chapman, R. (1981). The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 24. 154–61.Google Scholar
Minifie, F., Darley, F. & Sherman, D. (1963). Temporal reliability of seven language measures. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 6. 139–49.Google Scholar
Morehead, D. & Ingram, D. (1973). The development of base syntax in normal and linguistically deviant children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 16. 330–52.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1978). Maternal speech to normal and Down's syndrome children matched for mean length of utterance. In Meyers, C. (ed.), Quality of life in severely and profoundly mentally retarded people: research foundations for improvement. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Deficiency.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers' and mothers' speech in early language development. Journal of Child Language 7. 353–69.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1985). Linguistic and prelinguistic development in moderate and severe mental retardation. In Dobbing, J. (ed.), Scientific studies in mental retardation. London: the Royal Society of Medicine and the Macmillan Press.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A., Bachelet, J. F. & Pérée, F. (1985). Analyse du langage et des interactions verbales adulte-enfant. Bulletin d'Audiophonologie 5/6. 507–36.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. & Defays, D. (1978). Reliability of mean length of utterance as a function of sample size in early language development. Journal of General Psychology 133. 305–06.Google Scholar
Siegel, G. (1963). Adult verbal behavior in ‘play therapy’ sessions with retarded children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 10. 34–8.Google Scholar
Templin, M. (1957). Certain language skills in children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar