Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T00:57:50.040Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The emergence of productive speech and language in Spanish-learning paediatric cochlear implant users*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2013

IGNACIO MORENO-TORRES*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Málaga
*
Address for correspondence: Universidad de Málaga – Filología Española II y Teoría de la Literatura, Campus de Teatinos s/n Málaga 29018, Spain. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

It has been proposed that cochlear implant users may develop robust categorical perception skills, but that they show limited precision in perception. This article explores if a parallel contrast is observable in production, and if, despite acquiring typical linguistic representations, their early words are inconsistent. The participants were eight Spanish-learning deaf children implanted before their second birthday. Two studies examined the transition from babbling to words, and the one-word period. Study 1 found that the participants used the same sound types in babbling and in words, indicating that production is guided by stored motor patterns. No clear evidence of inconsistent production was observed. Study 2 found that in the one-word period CI users develop typical prosodic representations, but that their productions are highly unstable. Results are discussed in terms of the role of auditory feedback for the development of productive language skills.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by a grant from the Regional Government of Andalucia (Proyecto de Excelencia P07-SEJ-03119). I would like to thank Santiago Torres, Rafael Santana, and María del Mar Cid for their help in collecting and analyzing the data. I am most grateful to the children, the families, and the therapists who participated in the two studies.

References

REFERENCES

Adi-Bensaid, L. & Ben-David, A. (2010). Typical acquisition by a-typical children: initial consonant cluster acquisition by Hebrew-acquiring children with cochlear implants. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 24, 771–94.Google Scholar
Adi-Bensaid, L. & Tubul-Lavy, G. (2009). Consonant-free words: evidence from Hebrew-speaking children with cochlear implants. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 23, 122–32.Google Scholar
Barry, J., Blamey, P. J. & Fletcher, J. (2006). Factors affecting the acquisition of vowel phonemes by pre-linguistically deafened cochlear implant users learning Cantonese. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 20, 761–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2010). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.1) [Computer program]. Available from: <http://www.praat.org>(last accessed 31 January 2010).(last+accessed+31+January+2010).>Google Scholar
Bouton, S., Serniclaes, W, Bertoncini, J. & Cole, P. (2012). Perception of speech features by French-speaking children with Cochlear Implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 55, 139–53.Google Scholar
Chin, S. B. (2006). Realization of complex onsets by pediatric users of cochlear implants. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 20, 501–08.Google Scholar
Coninx, F., Weichbold, V., Tsiakpini, L., Autrique, E., Bescond, G., Tamas, L. & Brachmaier, J. (2009). Validation of the LittlEARS((R)) Auditory Questionnaire in children with normal hearing. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 73, 1761–68.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (1996). The prosodic structure of early words. In Morgan, J. & Demuth, K. (eds.), Signal to syntax: bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition, 171–84. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A. & Gierut, J. A. (2008). Optimality Theory, phonological acquisition and disorders. London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Dodd, B. (2005). Differential diagnosis and treatment of speech disordered children, 2nd edn.London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Dodd, B. & Bradford, A. (2000). A comparison of three therapy methods for children with different types of developmental phonological disorders. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 35, 189209.Google Scholar
Duchesne, L., Sutton, A. & Bergeron, F. (2009). Language achievement in children who received cochlear implants between 1 and 2 years of age: group trends and individual patterns. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 14, 465–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ertmer, D. J. & Goffman, L. A. (2011). Speech production accuracy and variability in young cochlear implant recipients: comparisons with typically developing age-peers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 54, 177–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ertmer, D. J. & Inniger, K. J. (2009). Transition to spoken words in two young cochlear implant recipients. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 52, 1579–94.Google Scholar
Ertmer, D. J. & Mellon, J. A. (2001). Beginning to talk at 20 months: early vocal development in a young cochlear implant recipient. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44, 192206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrest, K., Elbert, M. & Dinnsen, A. (2000). The effect of substitution patterns on phonological treatment outcomes. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 14, 519–31.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E., Moog, J. S., Biedenstein, J., Brenner, C. & Hayes, H. (2009). Spoken language scores of children using cochlear implants compared to hearing age-mates at school entry. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 14, 371–85.Google Scholar
Gennari, S. & Demuth, K. (1997). Syllable omission in Spanish. In Hughes, E. M. & Green, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 182–93. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Gerrit, E. (2010). The prosodic structure of early words in Dutch toddlers with cochlear implants. Paper presented at the 13th Clinical Linguistics Conference, University of Oslo, Norway.Google Scholar
Giezen, M., Escudero, P. & Baker, A. (2010). Use of acoustic cues by children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 53(6), 1440–57.Google Scholar
Gillis, S., Schauwers, K. & Govaerts, P. J. (2002). Babbling milestones and beyond: early speech development in CI children. Antwerp Papers in Linguistics 102, 2339.Google Scholar
Grunwell, P. (1981). The nature of phonological disability in children. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Grunwell, P. (1991). Developmental phonological disorders from a clinical-linguistic perspective. In Yavas, M. S. (ed.), Phonological disorders in children: theory, research and practice, 3764. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Guenther, F. H. (1994). A neural network model of speech acquisition and motor equivalent speech production. Biological Cybernetics 72(1), 4353.Google Scholar
Guenther, F. H., Ghosh, S. S. & Tourville, J. A. (2006). Neural modeling and imaging of the cortical interactions underlying syllable production. Brain and Language 96(3), 280301.Google Scholar
Guerra, R. (1983). Recuento estadístico de la sílaba en español. Estudios de fonética I, Colectanea fonética. Madrid: CSIC.Google Scholar
Houston, D. M. & Miyamoto, R. T. (2010). Effects of early auditory experience on word learning and speech perception in deaf children with cochlear implants: implications for sensitive periods of language development. Otology and Neurotology 31, 1248–53.Google Scholar
Houston, D. M., Pisoni, D. B., Kirk, K. I., Ying, E. & Miyamoto, R. T. (2003). Speech perception skills of deaf infants following cochlear implantation: a first report. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 67, 479–95.Google Scholar
Hua, Z. & Dodd, B. (eds.) (2006). Phonological development and disorders: a multilingual perspective. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1989). First language acquisition: method, description and exploration. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kim, J. & Chin, S. (2008). Fortition and lenition patterns in the acquisition of obstruents by children with cochlear implants. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 22, 233–51.Google Scholar
Kirchner, R. (2003). Consonant lenition. In Hayes, B., Kirchner, R. & Steriade, D. (eds.), Phonetic bases of markednesss, 313–45. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirk, K. I., Miyamoto, R. T., Lento, C. L., Ying, E., O'Neill, T. & Fears, B. (2002). Effects of age at implantation in young children. Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology 111, 6973.Google Scholar
Kristofferson, K. E. (2008). Speech and language development in cri du chat syndrome: a critical review. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 22(6), 443–57.Google Scholar
Le Normand, M. T. & Chevrie-Muller, C. (1991). A follow-up case study of transitory developmental apraxia of speech: ‘l'enfant à voyelles’. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 57, 99118.Google Scholar
Le Normand, M. T., Parisse, C. & Cohen, H. (2008). Lexical diversity and productivity in French preschoolers: developmental and biosocial aspects by development, gender and sociocultural factors. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 22(1), 4758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, C. C., Schiller, N. O. & Levelt, W. J. M. (1999). A developmental grammar for syllable structure in the production of child language. Brain and Language 68, 291–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loizou, P. (2006). Speech processing in vocoder-centric cochlear implants. In Moller, A. (ed.), Cochlear and brainstem implants (Advances in oto-rhino-laryngology), 109–43. Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S., Gallego, C., Gallo, P., Karousou, A., Mariscal, S. & Martínez, M. (2005). Inventarios de Desarrollo Comunicativo MacArthur: Manual Técnico y Cuadernillos. Madrid: Ediciones TEA.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. (1998). The frame/content theory of evolution of speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, 499511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Martínez-Celdrán, E. (1999). Sobre la naturaleza fonética de los alófonos de b/d/g en español y sus distintas denominaciones. Verba 18, 235–53.Google Scholar
McCathren, R. B., Yoder, S. & Warren, F. (1999). The relationship between prelinguistic vocalization and later expressive vocabulary in young children with developmental delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42, 915–24.Google Scholar
McCune, L. & Vihman, M. M. (2001). Early phonetic and lexical development: a productivity approach. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44, 670–84.Google Scholar
Medina, V. & Serniclaes, W. (2009). Consecuencias de la categorización fonológica sobre la lectura silenciosa de niños sordos con implante coclear [Consequences of phonological categorization on silent reading in children using cochlear implants]. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología 29, 186–94.Google Scholar
Moore, J. A. & Bass-Ringdahl, S. (2002). Role of infant vocal development in candidacy for efficacy of cochlear implantation. Annals of Otology, Rhynololy & Lanringology 111, 5255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, J. A., Scott-Prath, M. A. & Arrieta, A. (2007). Early Spanish speech acquisition following cochlear implantation. Volta Review 106, 321–41.Google Scholar
Morales-Font, A. (2007). Acquisition of syllable structure in Spanish. In Martínez-Gil, Fernando & Colina, Sonia (eds.), Optimality-Theoretic studies in Spanish phonology, 497524. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Moreno-Torres, I. & Torres, S. (2008). From 1 word to 2 words with cochlear implant and Cued Speech: a case study. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 22, 491508.Google Scholar
Nathani, S., Ertmer, D. & Stark, R. E. (2006). Assessing vocal development in infants and toddlers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 20, 351–69.Google Scholar
Nicholas, J. & Geers, A. E. (2006). Effects of early auditory experience on the spoken language of deaf children at 3 years of age. Ear & Hearing 27, 286–98.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (1988). The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Development 59, 441–49.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. & Lynch, M. P. (1992). Infant vocalizations and innovations in infraphonology: toward a broader theory of development and disorder. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (eds.), Phonological development, 509–36. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Peters, R., Moore, B. & Baer, T. (1998). Speech reception thresholds in noise with and without spectral and temporal dips for hearing-impaired and normally hearing people. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 103, 577–87.Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B. & Cleary, M. (2003). Measures of working memory span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after cochlear implantation. Ear & Hearing 24, 106–20.Google Scholar
Pulvermuller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6(7), 576–82.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition 73, 265–92.Google Scholar
Schauwers, K., Gillis, S., Daemers, K., De Beukelaer, C. & Govaerts, P. J. (2004). The onset of babbling and the audiologic outcome. Otology & Neurotology 25, 263–70.Google Scholar
Schramm, B., Bohnert, A. & Keilmann, A. (2009). The prelexical development in children implanted by 16 months compared with normal hearing children. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 73, 1673–81.Google Scholar
Serry, T. A. & Blamey, P. J. (1999). A 4 year investigation into phonetic inventory development in young cochlear implant users. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research 42, 141–54.Google Scholar
Simmonds, A. J., Wise, R. J. & Leech, R. (2011). Two tongues, one brain: imaging bilingual speech production. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 166.Google Scholar
Szagun, G. (2004). Learning by ear: on the acquisition of case and gender marking by German-speaking children with normal hearing and with cochlear implants. Journal of Child Language 31, 130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terband, H. & Maassen, B. (2010). Speech motor development in Childhood Apraxia of Speech (CAS): generating testable hypotheses by neurocomputational modeling. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica 62, 134–42.Google Scholar
Titterington, J., Henry, A., Kramer, M., Toner, J. G. & Stevenson, M. (2006). An investigation of weak syllable processing in deaf children with cochlear implants. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 20, 249–69.Google Scholar
Tubul-Lavy, G. (2012). Intra-word inconsistency in apraxic Hebrew-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 26, 502–17.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. 1996. Phonological development: the origins of language in the child. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. & Croft, W. (2007). Phonological development: toward a ‘radical’ templatic phonology. Linguistics 45, 683725.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Simmons, R. M. H. & Miller, J. (1985) From babbling to speech: a re-assessment of the continuity issue. Language 61, 397445.Google Scholar
Warner-Czyz, A. D. & Davis, B. L. (2008). The emergence of segmental accuracy in young cochlear implant recipients. Cochlear Implants International 9, 143–66.Google Scholar
Warner-Czyz, A. D., Davis, B. L. & MacNeilage, P. F. (2010). Accuracy of consonant–vowel syllables in young cochlear implant recipients and hearing children in the single-word period. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53, 217.Google Scholar
Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Baca, R. L. & Sedey, A. L. (2010). Describing the trajectory of language development in the presence of severe-to-profound hearing loss: a closer look at children with cochlear implants versus hearing aids. Otology & Neurotology 31, 1268–74.Google Scholar