Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T18:50:31.510Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Again and again: reduplication in child phonology*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Richard G. Schwartz
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Laurence B. Leonard
Affiliation:
Purdue University
M. Jeanne Wilcox
Affiliation:
Memphis State University
M. Karen Folger
Affiliation:
Solana County Schools

Abstract

The role of a strategy of reduplication in phonological acquisition and behaviour was examined in terms of: (1) the relationship between adoption of this strategy and children's failure to produce nonreduplicated multisyllabic forms and final consonants when attempting corresponding adult models; and (2) the role of the process of reduplication in production constraints. Comparisons of the phonologies of six children (1; 3–1; 9) classified as reduplicators and six children (1; 5–2; 0) classified as non-reduplicators revealed that the adoption/nonadoption of this strategy was closely related to the extent of production of nonreduplicated multisyllabic forms. Analyses of attempted words produced as reduplications revealed that this process served primarily to constrain multisyllabic productions. To a lesser degree, reduplication appeared to serve in conspiracies to constrain final consonant production.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Address for correspondence: Richard G. Schwartz, Department of Speech and Theatre Arts, 1117 Cathedral of Learning, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, U.S.A.

References

REFERENCES

Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge: Harvard University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, C., Edwards, M., Luckau, J., Stoel, C., Macken, M. & Petersen, J. (1973). On specifying a system for transcribing consonants in child language: a working paper with examples from American English and Mexican Spanish. Unpublished paper, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1976). Learning to pronounce: the earliest stages of phonological development in the child. PRCLD 11. 127.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition: English initial consonants in the first fifty words. Lg 51. 419–39.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A., Peizer, D. P., & Weeks, T. E. (1973). Model-and-replica grammar of a child's first words. Lingua 31. 3565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fudge, E. C. (1969). Syllables. JL 5. 253–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folger, M. K. (1977). Language and sensorimotor development in normal and language-impaired children. Unpublished Master's Thesis, Memphis State University.Google Scholar
Hamp, E. (1974). Reduplication and monosyllables. JChLang 1. 287.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1974). Phonological rules in young children. JChLang 1. 4964.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1976). Phonological disability in children. New York: ElsevierGoogle Scholar
Kisseberth, C. (1970). On the functional unity of phonological rules. LI 1. 291306.Google Scholar
Moskowitz, A. I. (1973). Acquisition of phonology and syntax: a preliminary study. In Hintikka, I. et al. (eds), Approaches to natural language. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: a case study. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Warner, W., Meeker, M. & Eells, K. (1960). Social class in America: The evaluation of status. New York: Harper and RowGoogle Scholar
Waterson, N. (1971). Child phonology: a prosodic view. JL 7. 179211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winer, B. J. (1962). Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-HillCrossRefGoogle Scholar