Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:40:13.487Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Development of verbal skills in first-born and only boys

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 July 2008

A. J Cropley
Affiliation:
University of Regina, Regina, Canada
K. H Ahlers
Affiliation:
University of Regina, Regina, Canada

Summary

Verbal IQs were calculated for 60 boys who had no siblings (only boys) and 64 who were the first in a sibline (first borns) at two age levels—6 and 7 years versus 11 and 12 years. In addition to a pronounced socioeconomic status (SES) effect, three of four subgroups of only boys obtained significantly higher mean IQs than first borns. These data were interpreted as supporting the view that development of verbal abilities is facilitated by contact with parental models of language behaviour, only boys benefiting from closer contact than first borns. However, interactions among IQs, age and SES suggested that although mere increased contact is adequate to foster verbal development in younger children, quality of parental stimulation is important at higher age levels.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1975

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abe, K., Tsuji, K. & Suzuki, H. (1964) The significance of birth order and difference between siblings as observed in the drawings of pre-kindergarten children. Fol. psychiat. neurol. Jap. 17, 315.Google Scholar
Adler, A. (1928) Characteristics of first, second, third child. Children, 3, 14, 52.Google Scholar
Altus, W.D. (1966) Birth order and its sequelae. Science, N.Y. 151, 44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apperly, F.L. (1939) A study of American Rhodes Scholars. J. Hered. 30, 493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blustein, E.S. (1967) The Relationships of Sibling Position in the Family Constellation to School Behaviour Variables in Elementary School Children from Two-Child Families. Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Bossard, J.H.S. & Boll, E.S. (1956) The Large Family System. University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. & Bellugi, U. (1970) Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. In: New Directions in the Study of Language. Edited by Lenneberg, E.. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
Burton, D. (1968) Birth order and intelligence. J. soc. Psychol. 76, 199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cazde, C.B. (1966) Subcultural differences in child language: an inter-disciplinary review. Merrill-Palmer Quart. 12, 185.Google Scholar
Chen, E. & Cobb, S. (1960) Family structure in relation to health and disease: a review of the literature. J. chron. Dis. 12, 544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chittenden, E.A., Foan, M.W., Zweil, D.M. & Smith, J. (1968) School achievement of first and second-born siblings. Child Develpt, 29, 1223.Google Scholar
Cicirelli, V.G. (1967) Sibling constellation, creativity, IQ, and academic achievement. Child Develpt, 38, 481.Google Scholar
Clarke, E.L. (1916) American Men of Letters: Their Nature and Nurture. Columbia University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Clausen, J.A. (1966) Family structure, socialization and personality. In: Review of Child Development Research, Vol. 2. Edited by Hoffman, L.W. and Hoffman, M.L.. Sage Publications, New York.Google Scholar
Cropley, A.J. (1964) Differentiation of abilities, socio-economic status and the WISC. J. consult. Psychol. 28, 512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Day, E.J. (1932) The development of language in twins. I. A comparison of twins and single children. Child Develpt, 3, 179.Google Scholar
Douglas, J.W.B. (1964) The Home and the School: A Study of Ability and Achievement in the Primary School. MacGibbon & Kee, London.Google Scholar
Elder, G.H. (1962) Family structures: the effects of size of family, sex composition and ordinal position on academic motivation and achievement. In: Adolescent Achievement and Mobility Aspirations. Edited by Elder, G.H.. Institute for Research in Social Sciences, Chapel Hill, NC.Google Scholar
Ellis, H. (1904) A Study of British Genius. Hurst & Blackett, London.Google Scholar
Freeberg, N.E. & Payne, D.T. (1967) Parental influence on cognitive development in early childhood: a review. Child Develpt, 38, 65.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1874) English Men of Science: Their Nature and Nurture. Macmillan, London.Google Scholar
Hunt, J.M. (1961) Intelligence and Experience. Ronald Press, New York.Google Scholar
Hunt, J.M. (1973) Heredity, environment, and class or ethnic differences. In: Assessment in a Pluralistic Society. Proceedings of the 1972 Invitational Conference on Testing Problems. ETC,Princeton.Google Scholar
Jencks, C. (1972) Inequality: A Reassessment of the Effect of Family and Schooling in America. Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
Jones, H.E. (1933) Order of birth. In: A Handbook of Child Psychology. Edited by Marchison, C.E.. Clark University Press, Worcester, Mass.Google Scholar
Kahl, J.A. & Davis, J.A. (1955) A comparison of indices of socio-economic status. Am. sociol. Rev. 20, 317.Google Scholar
Koch, H.L. (1954) The relationship of primary mental abilities in five and six year olds to sex of child and characteristics of his siblings. Child Develpt, 25, 208.Google Scholar
Lasko, J.K. (1954) Parent behaviour toward first and second children. Genet. Psychol. Monog. 49, 96.Google ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, D. (1954) Language development in children. In: Manual of Child Psychology. Edited by Carmichael, L.. Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1970) The Acquisition of Language. Harper & Row, New York.Google Scholar
Moore, R.K. (1964) Susceptibility to hypnosis and susceptibility to social influence. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol. 68, 282.Google Scholar
Poole, A. (1974) Bytheway’s statistical trap. J. biosoc. Sci. 6, 73.Google Scholar
Rosen, B.C. (1961) Family structure and achievement motivation. Am. sociol. Rev. 26, 574.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, B.G. & Sutton-Smith, B. (1964) The relationship of ordinal position and sibling sex status to cognitive abilities. Psychonom. Sci. 1, 481.Google Scholar
Sampson, E.E. (1962) Birth order, need achievement, and conformity. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol. 64, 155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoonover, S.M. (1959) The relationship of intelligence to birth order, sex of sibling, and age interval. J. educ. Psychol. 50, 143.Google Scholar
Skinner, B.F. (1957) Verbal Behaviour. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.Google Scholar
Start, A. (1971) Some Relationships between Birth Order and Attainment. MA thesis, University of Saskatchewan, Regina Campus.Google Scholar
Terman, L.M. (1925) Genetic Studies of Genius, Vol. 1. Stanford University Press, Stanford, California.Google Scholar
Vernon, P.E. (1969) Intelligence and Cultural Environment. Methuen, London.Google Scholar
Walker, C.E. & Tahmisian, J. (1967) Birth order and student characteristics. J. consult. Psychol. 31, 219.Google Scholar
Warner, W.L. & Lunt, P.S. (1941) The Social Life of a Modern Community and the Status System of a Modern Community. Yale University Press, New Haven, Conn.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1949) Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Psychological Corporation, New York.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1958) The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence. Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore.Google Scholar
Winer, B.J. (1962) Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. McGraw-Hill, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar