Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T07:11:44.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reading activity prevents long-term decline in cognitive function in older people: evidence from a 14-year longitudinal study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2020

Yu-Hung Chang
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, China Medical University, Taichung, Taiwan
I-Chien Wu
Affiliation:
Institute of Population Health Sciences, National Health Research Institutes, Zhunan, Taiwan
Chao A. Hsiung*
Affiliation:
Institute of Population Health Sciences, National Health Research Institutes, Zhunan, Taiwan
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Chao A. Hsiung, Institute of Population Health Sciences, National Health Research Institutes, Zhunan, Taiwan. Phone +886 37 246 166 ext. 36100; Fax: +886 37 586 467. Email: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

This study examined the effect of daily life reading activity on the risk of cognitive decline and whether the effect differs regarding education levels.

Design:

A longitudinal study with 6-, 10-, and 14-year follow-up.

Setting:

Face-to-face interviews with structured questionnaires at home.

Participants:

A representative sample of 1,962 Taiwanese community-dwelling older persons aged 64 and above, followed up in four waves of surveys over 14 years.

Measurements:

Baseline reading frequencies were measured based on a scale of leisure activity. The Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire was used to measure cognitive performance. We performed logistic regression to assess associations between baseline reading and later cognitive decline. Interaction terms between reading and education were to compare the reading effects on cognitive decline at different education levels.

Results:

After adjusting for covariates, those with higher reading frequencies (≥1 time a week) were less likely to have cognitive decline at 6-year (adjusted odds ratio [AOR]: 0.54; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.34–0.86), 10-year (AOR: 0.58, 95% CI: 0.37–0.92), and 14-year (AOR: 0.54, 95% CI: 0.34–0.86); in a 14-year follow-up, a reduced risk of cognitive decline was observed among older people with higher reading frequencies versus lower ones at all educational levels.

Conclusions:

Reading was protective of cognitive function in later life. Frequent reading activities were associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline for older adults at all levels of education in the long term.

Type
Original Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2020

Introduction

High level of cognitive functional capacity is one of the major components of successful aging (Rowe and Kahn, Reference Rowe and Kahn1997). The World Health Organization (WHO) highlighted the importance of cognitive function in determining health in its policy framework on active aging (WHO, 2002). Cognitive decline is a normal process of aging (Harada et al., Reference Harada, Love and Triebel2013). Mild cognitive impairment is a greater cognitive decline beyond one’s age without interfering with daily life activities; dementia is characterized by more severe and widespread cognitive impairments that substantially influence daily functions (Gauthier et al., Reference Gauthier2006). The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment over the age of 60 ranges from 6.7% to 25.2% and increases with age (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen2018). The number of persons with dementia worldwide was estimated to be 46.8 million in 2015, resulting in 818 billion USD worth of societal costs (Prince, Reference Prince2015). In East Asia, the region of the most populations living with dementia, the prevalence of dementia in older people aged 60 or older was 4.5% in 2015 (Prince, Reference Prince2015). In Taiwan, the prevalence of cognitive impairment among older adults aged 65 and over was 22% (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Lan, Chen, Chiu and Lan2011) and that of dementia was 5.7% (Wu et al., Reference Wu2013), respectively. Dementia affected 210,000 Taiwanese older adults by 2020 and is projected to affect 710,000 by 2060 (Wu et al., Reference Wu2013).

Cognitive impairments and dementia could predict premature death (Sachs et al., Reference Sachs2011; Todd et al., Reference Todd, Barr, Roberts and Passmore2013). Loss of cognitive function was associated with reduced functional abilities of daily living, compromised well-being, and lower life satisfaction (Comijs et al., Reference Comijs, Dik, Aartsen, Deeg and Jonker2005; Mehta et al., Reference Mehta, Yaffe and Covinsky2002; St. John and Montgomery, Reference St. John and Montgomery2010). In addition to normal aging, cognitive decline and impairments may be induced by lack of practice (Mackinnon et al., Reference Mackinnon, Christensen, Hofer, Korten and Jorm2003), anxiety, depression or other neuropsychiatric symptoms (Lara et al., Reference Lara, Koyanagi, Domènech-Abella, Miret, Ayuso-Mateos and Haro2017; Palmer et al., Reference Palmer2011; Potvin et al., Reference Potvin, Forget, Grenier, Préville and Hudon2011), vascular factors (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015; Debette et al., Reference Debette2011), substance use and medication (Anstey et al., Reference Anstey, Mack and Cherbuin2009; Fox et al., Reference Fox2011), loneliness (Boss et al., Reference Boss, Kang and Branson2015), low social support, and isolation (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly2017). The process of cognitive decline could be slowed by involvement in regular physical activity (Ma et al., Reference Ma, Wong, Leung, Fung, Chan and Lam2017; Northey et al., Reference Northey, Cherbuin, Pumpa, Smee and Rattray2018), avoiding smoking (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015), adopting particular dietary patterns and nutrient intake (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Richards, Chan, Chiu, Lee and Lam2017; Solfrizzi et al., Reference Solfrizzi2017), or more engagement in social life and leisure activity (Ghisletta et al., Reference Ghisletta, Bickel and Lovden2006; Glei et al., Reference Glei, Landau, Goldman, Chuang, Rodriguez and Weinstein2005).

Cognitively stimulating activities, or intellectual activities, are recognized as a lifestyle factor in preserving cognitive function in the aged (Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Hertzog, Small and Dixon1999; Mackinnon et al., Reference Mackinnon, Christensen, Hofer, Korten and Jorm2003). Studies have shown that cognitively stimulating or intellectual activities, including reading, watching TV, listening to radio, playing games, puzzling, or gambling was associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline in later life (Gallucci et al., Reference Gallucci2009; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Richards, Chan, Chiu, Lee and Lam2018; Leung et al., Reference Leung2011; Litwin et al., Reference Litwin, Schwartz and Damri2017; Verghese et al., Reference Verghese2006; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson2002). Most studies have adopted a composite measure of cognitive activities and less is known about the effects of a specific activity. To engage actively in daily life, activities may differ in their corresponding cognitive tasks and the amounts of intellectual stimulation required for active engagement (Ghisletta et al., Reference Ghisletta, Bickel and Lovden2006). Besides, different activities might offset each other in their effects on cognitive function (Gallucci et al., Reference Gallucci2009; Lindstrom et al., Reference Lindstrom2005; Lopes et al., Reference Lopes, Ferrioli, Nakano, Litvoc and Bottino2012; Lovden et al., Reference Lovden, Ghisletta and Lindenberger2005). Research evidence for any specific activity is warranted.

Reading is a typical intellectual activity. Compared with other leisure activities, such as physical and social activities, it is more sedentary and isolated. Reading for leisure has proven to have health benefits for older people in prolonging life (Jacobs et al., Reference Jacobs, Hammerman-Rozenberg, Cohen and Stessman2008), and cognition may mediate between reading and survival advantage (Bavishi et al., Reference Bavishi, Slade and Levy2016). Reading ability is strongly related to educational attainment in early life. It is unclear whether reading activity is protective against cognitive decline independent of education, and when reading ability could mediate between early education attainment and later-life cognitive function (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Flicker and Lichtenberg2006). There is little evidence that education could moderate the effect of reading on cognitive function (Lachman et al., Reference Lachman, Agrigoroaei, Murphy and Tun2010). The aim of the study is twofold—first, to investigate the effect of reading on cognitive function in a representative Taiwanese older population who were followed up to 14 years. Second, we seek to investigate the differential effects of reading on preventing cognitive decline for older adults with different educational levels.

Method

Data

To explore the long-term relationship between cognitive activities and cognitive decline, we used data from the Taiwan Longitudinal Study on Aging (TLSA). The nation-wide longitudinal study started in 1989, and six sequential follow-ups were held in a 3- or 4-year interval between 1989 and 2011. In the first wave (1989), a representative sample of 4,049 older adults aged 60 and over was interviewed face-to-face with questionnaires with a 94% response rate. In the second wave (1993), 589 of the original participants were deceased, 301 were lost-to-follow-up, and five were incompletely interviewed, which resulted in 3,154 (78% of original respondents) completely interviewed. The second wave of surveys was the baseline for the study since cognitive functions were assessed for the first time. And, 2,946 out of 3,154 participants completed the assessment at baseline. In the 3rd (1996), 4th (1999), 5th (2003), and the 6th (2007) surveys, 2,669 (66%), 2,310 (59%), 1,743 (43%), and 1,268 (31%) of the first wave respondents completed these follow-ups. The third survey was not used in this study because its cognitive assessment was not comparable to those in other surveys. And, 1,973 participants had both a baseline survey and at least one follow-up survey with complete cognitive assessments. Participants who had severe cognitive impairment at baseline (n = 11) were excluded because the definition of cognitive decline in this study did not apply to them, and a total of 1,962 participants were included in the analysis (1,861 in 6-year, 1,330 in 10-year, and 818 in 14-year follow-up). Details of design and sampling for the surveys can be found elsewhere (Glei et al., Reference Glei, Landau, Goldman, Chuang, Rodriguez and Weinstein2005; Lee et al., Reference Lee2012; Yen et al., Reference Yen2010). The data were officially released by and used abiding by the regulations of the Health and Welfare Data Science Center, Minister of Health and Welfare, Taiwan.

Measurements

In the second wave of TLSA surveys, the Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire (SPMSQ) was first used for assessing cognitive functions of participants. The instrument was also applied to the subsequent four waves. A SPMSQ test is composed of 10 items, which examine a respondent’s orientation to time and location, capacities of recalling personal information and current events, and a counting backward exercise. The SPMSQ was scored as the sum of errors, ranged from 0 to 10, which indicates the extent to which a respondent’s cognitive function is impaired. The Chinese version has been validated in the Taiwanese population (Hsiao et al., Reference Hsiao, Chiu and Liu1994). However, all 10 items were adopted only in the second wave. From the 4th wave to 6th wave, nine items were used except asking a respondent’s birthday and only six items used in the 3rd wave. In the present study, we adopted nine items of SPMSQ from the 2nd, 4th, 5th, and 6th wave as the measurement of cognitive function. The 9-item SPMSQ scores (number of error responses) ranged from 0 to 9. Our preliminary analysis revealed that the 9-item SPMSQ might be less sensitive to the changes in cognitive functions for those who were developing cognitive impairments, although it has an acceptable and similar level of internal consistency as the 10-item version (Cronbach’s α of the 9-item and 10-item SPMSQ are 0.79 and 0.81 based on the data of 1993). Decline in cognitive function was defined as an increase of two or more SPMSQ scores between baseline (the 2nd wave) and subsequent surveys (Black and Rush, Reference Black and Rush2002; Fillenbaum et al., Reference Fillenbaum, Hanlon, Landerman and Schmader2001; Gray et al., Reference Gray, Hanlon, Landerman, Artz, Schmader and Fillenbaum2003; Hanlon et al., Reference Hanlon, Landerman, Artz, Gray, Fillenbaum and Schmader2004). We also conducted a sensitivity analysis that defined cognitive decline as an increase of one error or more in SPMSQ to understand if using a more sensitive criterion would affect the results of the analysis.

Reading was measured with a leisure-time activity scale in the 1993 survey, which asked respondents what leisure-time activities or entertainments they did in daily life when not working, and how often they did these activities. These activities included watching TV or listening to the radio, reading books/magazines/newspapers, worshiping, grand-parenting, playing chess/cards, visiting or hanging out with acquaintances, gardening, playing with pets, meditating, participating in group exercise, involvement in outdoor activities, or gambling. Weekly reading frequency was divided into four categories: “never,” “less than once a week,” “once or twice a week,” and “almost every day.” Due to the relatively smaller case numbers for “less than once a week” and “once or twice a week,” the former was merged with “never” as the “low reading group” and the latter merged with “almost every day” as the “high reading group.”

The baseline demographic and socioeconomic variables, including age, gender, ethnicity, education, marital status, and perceived financial status, were collected for multivariable analysis. Health behavior measures included smoking and alcohol drinking. The frequency of outdoor activities served as a surrogate for physical activity because there was no direct question indicating exercise in the 1993 survey. Activities of daily living (ADL) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) items consisted of a composite index to represent physical function with three levels: independent, IADL impairment, and ADL impairment. Self-reported diabetes mellitus (DM), stroke, and 10 other chronic diseases or health conditions were recorded. Twelve binary variables were created and coded as 1 to indicate the presence of DM, stroke, or health conditions and 0 for the absence of them. We left DM and stroke variables separately but summed the rest to an index indicating the number of comorbidities. Since reading is subject to vision, participants were categorized into with- and without-sight problems in terms of the presence of self-reported cataracts, glaucoma, retinal detachment, or unclear sight even when wearing glasses.

Statistical analysis

Means and standard deviations were reported to describe the distributions of numeric variables, and frequencies and percentages to those of categorical variables. Contingency tables were established in the bivariate analysis and chi-square was used for testing statistical significance. In multivariate analysis, logistic regressions were performed to identify predictors of decline in cognitive function between baseline and follow-ups (i.e. 1999, 2003, and 2007, respectively.) Covariates for adjustments comprised age, sex, education, marital status, ethnicity, perceived financial status, smoking, alcohol drinking, outdoor activities, physical function, number of comorbidities, and sight problems. Diabetes and stroke and their preceding factors such as obesity, hypertension, hyperlipidemia were also the risk factors of cognitive decline (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015). The sequelae of diabetes and stroke such as retinopathy or acquired dyslexia may affect reading abilities. Therefore, diabetes and stroke were separated from other chronic conditions for controlling for potential confounding effects. Reading activities may represent abilities to engage in or be consequences of other intellectual or social leisure activities. We selected three other leisure activities, such as watching TV or listening to the radio, playing games (chess, cards, or mahjong), and visiting or hanging out with acquaintances for controlling for potential confounding effects between reading and cognitive outcomes. Comparing the high reading group to the low reading group in logistic regressions, a dummy variable was created and the low reading group served as reference. Higher frequency of reading is potentially a consequence of some precedent conditions like higher education, better baseline cognitive function, or sight. The high reading group might be over-represented in the sample, while the low reading group might be under-represented. Therefore, we further assessed the models with inverse probability weighting (IPW) to adjust for the potential selection bias (Hernan et al., Reference Hernan, Hernandez-Diaz and Robins2004). To understand the influence of the participants with cognitive impairments at baseline on the results, we conducted two parallel analyses: one for those with SPMSQ errors ≤2 at baseline and one for all eligible participants except SPMSQ errors ≥8 at baseline (because an increase of 2 errors did not apply to them using the 9-item SPMSQ). To investigate effects of reading on cognitive function decline with regard to education levels, two reading groups (high/low) and three education levels (illiterate and literate without formal education as “low,” primary school as “middle,” and junior high school and above as “high”) were re-categorized to form 2 × 3 = 6 groups. Interaction terms between education and reading were created to add in the logistic models where low reading groups and low education groups served as the references in comparison. Since one of the research questions is whether reading affects cognitive decline of older people with different education levels, we conducted planned comparisons without corrections between low and high reading groups at each education level using Wald tests. The significance level of all models was set at 0.05. All statistical analysis was performed using SAS 9.3 for Windows (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina).

This study was approved by the China Medical University & Hospital Research Ethics Committee, Taichung, Taiwan (CMUH103-REC3-057).

Results

Demographics and other study variables are presented in Table 1. More than one-third (37%) of the sample indicated frequently reading at the baseline year. Those who frequently engaged in reading (more than twice a week) were men, junior high school or higher educational levels, mainlanders, living with spouse/partners, perceived sufficient in finances, nonsmokers, involved in more outdoor activities, without impaired physical functions, lower SPMSQ errors, without diabetes and sight problems, and lower numbers of chronic diseases.

Table 1. Characteristics of participants of the Taiwan Longitudinal Study on Aging, 1993–2007

Results of logistic regressions on decline in cognitive function for those with SPMSQ errors ≤2 at baseline are displayed in Table 2. In the crude model, more reading, compared to less, was associated with a lower risk of cognitive function decline in the 6-year span, 10-year span, and 14-year span, respectively. Modeling with adjustment for age and sex increased the odds ratios (ORs) in 6-, 10- and 14-year spans to 0.40 (95% CI: 0.27–0.58), 0.46 (95% CI: 0.32–0.66) and 0.46 (95% CI: 0.31–0.67), respectively (Model 1). When education level was accounted for in Model 2, the ORs for those with a higher reading frequency were again elevated to 0.59 (95% CI: 0.38–0.92) in 6 years, 0.59 (95% CI: 0.39–0.91) in 10 years, and 0.60 (95% CI: 0.39–0.92) in 14 years. Model 3 with full adjustment presents the slightly reduced odds 0.54 (95% CI: 0.34–0.86), 0.58 (95% CI: 0.37–0.92) and 0.54 (95% CI: 0.34–0.86), respectively. In Model 4 with the illiterate excluded, the risks of cognitive decline for those with more reading were reduced to 0.50 (95% CI: 0.29–0.85) at 6 years, to 0.53 (95% CI: 0.32–0.88) at 10 years, and slightly increased to 0.55 (95% CI: 0.33–0.90) at 14 years. After controlling for three intellectual leisure activities in Model 5, the effect of reading was slightly weakened in 6 and 10 years, but slightly stronger in 14 years. In the model with IPW (Model 6), the ORs were changed mildly with reduced confidence intervals; they were 0.61 (95% CI: 0.42–0.88) in 6-year span, 0.54 (95% CI: 0.36–0.79) in 10-year span, and 0.51 (95% CI: 0.35–0.73) in 14-year span. The sensitivity analysis defining cognitive decline as an increase of one error or more revealed slightly attenuated effects of reading with full adjustments at 6 years (aOR: 0.69, 95% CI: 0.50–0.94), 10 years (aOR: 0.64, 95% CI: 0.46–0.90), and 14 years (aOR: 0.59, 95% CI: 0.39–0.88), compared with their counterparts in Model 3, probably caused by some substantial changes in cognitive function mixed up with an increase of one error occurred by chance (Supplementary Table S3).

Table 2. Effects of reading frequencies (higher versus lower) on decline in cognitive function among the TLSA participants with SPMSQ errors ≤ 2 at baseline

Decline is defined by an increase of two or more SPMSQ errors between baseline and end-point years.

Model 1: Adjusted for age and sex.

Model 2: Model 1 plus educational level.

Model 3: Model 2 plus marital status, ethnicity, perceived financial status, smoking, alcohol drinking, outdoor activities, physical function, self-reported diabetes, stroke, number of comorbidities, sight problems, and the number of SPMSQ errors at baseline.

Model 4: Model 3 excluding the illiterate.

Model 5: Model 3 plus watching TV/listening to radio, playing games, and visiting or hanging out with acquaintances.

Model 6: Model 3 using inverse probability weighting method.

Including those with SPMSQ errors >2 at baseline in analysis, Table 3 shows the effect of reading on cognitive decline regardless of baseline cognitive function. In general, compared with the models corresponding to each other in Table 2, the effects of reading on cognitive decline were similar with slightly higher ORs. These results imply that the long-term effects of reading on cognitive decline in 10 years and 14 years with full adjustments were robust and mildly affected by baseline cognitive performance.

Table 3. Effects of reading frequencies on decline in cognitive function among the TLSA participants with inclusion of those with SPMSQ errors >2 at baseline

Decline is defined by an increase of two or more SPMSQ errors between baseline and end-point years; participants having baseline SPMSQ errors ≥8 were not included since the definition of cognitive decline in terms of an increase of two or more errors did not apply to them.

Model 6: Adjusted for age and sex.

Model 7: Model 6 plus educational level.

Model 8: Model 7 plus marital status, ethnicity, perceived financial status, smoking, alcohol drinking, outdoor activities, physical function, self-reported diabetes, stroke, number of comorbidities, sight problems, and the number of SPMSQ errors at baseline.

Model 9: Model 8 excluding the illiterate.

Model 10: Model 8 plus watching TV/listening to radio, playing games, and visiting or hanging out with acquaintances.

Model 11: Model 8 using inverse probability weighting method.

The combined effects of reading and education on decline in cognitive function are presented in Figure 1. In general, those with a higher educational level were less likely to suffer decline in cognitive function regardless of the frequency of reading. In the 6-year follow-up, a person with more reading activities had an apparent lowered risk of cognitive decline among formally educated participants but did not among lower education participants. However, those with lower education could benefit most from reading activities in 10-year follow-up (aOR: 0.38, 95% CI: 0.21–0.66, p < 0.001), followed by those with middle education (high reading versus low reading, aOR: 0.17 versus 0.35; Wald test for comparison of aORs, p = 0.032), while the higher education group could benefit less (aOR: 0.12 versus 0.18; Wald test, p = 0.421). When participants were followed up 14 years after baseline, high frequencies versus lower frequencies of reading activities lead to significant decreases in odds of cognitive decline for those with lower education (aOR: 0.50 versus 1.00; Wald test, p = 0.024), middle education (0.26 versus 0.49; Wald test, p = 0.035), and high education (0.23 versus 0.51; Wald test, p = 0.034).

Figure 1. The combination effects of reading and education on the risk of cognitive decline at 6 years, 10 years, and 14 years of follow-ups in the TLSA participants. Results from logistic regression with interaction terms between education and reading based on Model 3 with adjustments for age, sex, ethnicity, marital status, financial status, smoking, alcohol drinking, outdoor activities, number of comorbidities, diabetes, stroke, sight, and the number of SPMSQ errors at baseline using the inverse probability weighting method. Participants with baseline SPMSQ ≤2 were included. Adjusted odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals are presented; R(L) denotes the groups with lower reading frequencies. R(H) denotes the groups with higher reading frequencies; Lower education refers to those who were not formally educated; middle education refers to 1–6 years of formal education; higher education refers to those who have had formal education for 7 years or more. Planned comparisons between low and high reading groups at the same education level are performed and p values of Wald tests for planned comparisons are reported.

Discussion

This study aims to appreciate the benefits of reading that deferred decline in cognitive function in later life in the Taiwanese older adults. Higher frequency of reading, i.e. twice or more a week, was associated with a reduced risk of decline in cognitive function over the long term—up to 14 years. With adjustment for relevant covariates and selection bias, our study also determines that frequent reading activities predict better cognitive outcomes at all educational levels, and the least formally educated might benefit most in the long term.

The finding that reading in daily life prevents cognitive decline for older people was in accordance with previous studies. Previous intervention studies assessing the effect of learning therapy, a training program consisting of reading aloud and arithmetic calculation which activates the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, showed these mental activities could improve cognitive performance for patients with dementia in a randomized control trial (Kawashima et al., Reference Kawashima2005) and also for normal aged people in community settings (Uchida and Kawashima, Reference Uchida and Kawashima2008). However, the sample size was small and the follow-up time was short in their studies. A cross-sectional study on 668 Italian older adults aged over 70 years shows that regular reading was strongly associated with better cognitive performance when physical and social activities, but not educational level, were controlled for (Gallucci et al., Reference Gallucci2009). A US prospective study on 801 older clergypersons followed up to 7 years showed that a one-point increase in baseline cognitive activity score was associated with one-third less risk of Alzheimer’s disease and reduced decline in global cognitive function (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson2002). The current study using samples from community dwellers in a general population repeatedly over a long time strengthens the evidence of reading benefit on reducing the risk of cognitive decline in later life.

Frequency of reading might be related to early education. The relationship between reading and reduced risk of cognitive decline might, therefore, reflect the education effect. Previous studies on early education achievement and cognitive function in later life were controversial. Some studies supported a positive relationship between education and cognitive performance of older adults (Black and Rush, Reference Black and Rush2002; Glei et al., Reference Glei, Landau, Goldman, Chuang, Rodriguez and Weinstein2005; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Lan, Chen, Chiu and Lan2011), while others did not (Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Hofer, Mackinnon, Korten, Jorm and Henderson2001; Muniz-Terrera et al., Reference Muniz-Terrera, Matthews, Dening, Huppert and Brayne2009; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson2002, Reference Wilson, Yu, Lamar, Schneider, Boyle and Bennett2019). These discrepancies between studies might result from different study populations, measurements, or modeling. Our study shows that, compared with illiterate participants, the risk of cognitive decline was significantly lower in older people who had attended school (Table 4); the higher the level of education, the lower the risk of cognitive decline. It echoes the findings of the study conducted by Liao et al. (Reference Liao2005) that the effect of education on cognitive reserve is cumulative. However, the gradient effect of education on cognitive decline gradually decreased with increasing time of follow-up. This may reflect the detrimental effect of age on the cognitive reserve produced by education in early life. The findings suggest that education can prevent cognitive decline in older people, and its effects may last for a long time.

Table 4. Effects of education on decline in cognitive function among the TLSA participants with SPMSQ errors ≤2 at baseline

Adjusted for age, sex, marital status, ethnicity, reading frequencies, perceived financial status, smoking, alcohol drinking, outdoor activities, physical function, diabetes, stroke, number of comorbidities, sight and number of SPMSQ errors at baseline.

On the other hand, when models are adjusted for education, reading can significantly predict cognitive decline independent of education. In the IPW model, which amends selection bias due to a positive association between education and reading, those who read more had a reduced risk of cognitive decline with even greater statistical significance. The evidence from the current study supports that involvement in more reading is independently predictive of subsequent better cognitive function in later life.

Reading might benefit all education levels. When the follow-up time was shorter and the study participants were younger in the 6-year follow up, reading affected middle and higher education levels but not for the lower one. In the 10-year follow up, the effect of reading at the high education level disappeared, probably because some of them had developed preclinical dementia but showed no clinical symptoms at baseline and they still were able to read. In addition, higher levels of education may imply a later onset of some chronic diseases not captured at baseline. In the 14-year follow up, however, less educated participants were protected by reading as well as those with middle and higher education (Figure 1). Investment in education in childhood may help to increase cognitive reserve (Meng and D’Arcy, Reference Meng and D’Arcy2012; Stern, Reference Stern2009), or cognitive capital as an economic metaphor (Rossor and Knapp, Reference Rossor and Knapp2015), and prevent cognitive decline in later life. Like financial capital, cognitive capital can be depreciated (Richards and Deary, Reference Richards and Deary2010). Engagement in reading might increase cognitive capital to resist aging-related cognitive function loss. Less-educated people may not have the intellectual skills of more-educated people for reading, and their prior cognitive ability before cognitive decline was lower; their capacity to benefit from reading activity was equivalent to, even higher than those of more-educated people in the long run. Our results echo a previous neuropathological study that found even very few years of education being able to improve cognitive reserve (Farfel et al., Reference Farfel2013). This finding implies that, for policymakers, promoting reading for older adults as an intervention to improve successful aging might not introduce a new source to widen health inequity.

Limitations

Missing data might challenge the representativeness of the sample and induce biased estimates. Most dropouts were due to death. Those who were more likely to drop out in follow-ups were those who were older, with more SPMSQ errors at baseline, less educated, less involved in reading, the Holo people, living without a spouse/partner, financially disadvantaged, smokers, non-alcohol drinkers, physically dependent, and having diabetes, stroke, or eye problems (data not shown). To answer whether the heterogeneity between missing and non-missing would bias the results, we employed the pattern-mixture models in which participants were regrouped in terms of their missing-data patterns (Hedeker and Gibbons, Reference Hedeker and Gibbons2006; Little, Reference Little1995). In our study, dummy variables were created to indicate if observations were missing in 1999, 2003, or 2007. An auxiliary model with full adjustment plus these added terms was performed. It was found that only “missing in 2007” significantly impacts the effect of reading on 1993–2003 cognitive decline, which results in ORs in missing and non-missing subgroups, 0.92 (95% CI: 0.43–1.99) and 0.29 (95% CI: 0.15–0.58), respectively. This implies that reading was not protective of cognitive decline during 1993–2003 for those who would be deceased or dropped out before 2007. This might be because older persons with poor health or severe chronic conditions leading to death might also suffer from losses of physical and/or cognitive functions due to their morbidities. In this case, reading was probably restricted by their impaired functions; it was also unknown if reading could benefit those who have already developed cognitive impairment.

In this study, reading frequency was a self-reported measure. Respondents were asked to recall the frequency of reading activities, which may lead to recall bias, especially for older people who have already experienced cognitive impairment. There is no measurement of reading time in this study, so the relationship between reading time and cognitive function cannot be established. According to the Survey of Social Development: Time Use Survey, conducted by the Taiwanese government in 2004 (DGBAS, 2005), among those 65 years and older who have reading habits, newspaper readers spend 65 minutes a day, magazine readers 72 minutes a day, and casual book readers 99 minutes. We speculate that those with reading habits may read more than an hour a day.

We only measured reading at baseline. How long a participant has been practicing reading habits was not clear. According to the first survey of TLSA, 84% of participants with high reading frequencies in 1993 were also having high reading frequencies in 1989; 88% of participants with low reading frequencies in 1993 were having low reading frequencies in 1989 (see Supplementary Table S4); these figures show that reading habits were stable over long periods.

However, those who read more often at baseline might have read less in a follow-up. This might result in misclassification bias and underestimation of the effect of reading on cognitive function. Our qualitative conclusions would not be changed. On the other hand, those who read less at baseline might have more involvement due to participating in a post-school education program or reading groups—again reducing the differential in cognitive performance among subgroups with differential reading at baseline.

Weekly reading frequencies were categorized into two groups because of smaller sample sizes of “less than once a week” (R1) and “once or twice a week” (R2). We acknowledged that this combination was arbitrary and therefore performed sensitivity analysis. We combined R1 and R2 into “almost every day” (R3) and compared with “never” (R0), as well as combined R1 and R2 with R0 and compared with R3. The results of the former grouping show similar estimates to Model 3 in Table 2 (Supplementary Table S5). It implies that a minimum reading even less than once a week might also help. The latter grouping results in a reduced effect with a marginal significance in 6 years, partially a result from misclassification, although the effect remains significant in 10 and 14 years. From the results of the regrouping analysis, the grouping method has little effect on the estimates and does not affect the subsequent qualitative conclusions.

In conclusion, the current study presents the evidence that more engagement in reading independently predicted a reduced risk of cognitive decline in later life. Furthermore, the long-term favorable effect could be observed at all education levels; low education might not impede the beneficial effect of reading activities on cognitive function for older people. Promoting reading activity is a promising approach to improve cognitive health in community settings.

Conflict of interest

None.

Source of funding

This work was supported by the China Medical University (Taichung, Taiwan) (CMU102-N-07) and the National Health Research Institute (Zhunan, Taiwan) (PH-101-SP-05).

Description of authors’ roles

Y. H. Chang was responsible for collection of data, statistical design, statistical analysis and writing the article. I. C. Wu assisted with conception and design of the study and critically reviewed the article. C. A. Hsiung designed the study, supervised the data collection and analysis and assisted with writing the article.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610220000812.

References

Anstey, K. J., Mack, H. A. and Cherbuin, N. (2009). Alcohol consumption as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: meta-analysis of prospective studies. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 542555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumgart, M., Snyder, H. M., Carrillo, M. C., Fazio, S., Kim, H. and Johns, H. (2015). Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 11, 718726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavishi, A., Slade, M. D. and Levy, B. R. (2016). A chapter a day: association of book reading with longevity. Social Science & Medicine, 164, 4448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, S. A. and Rush, R. D. (2002). Cognitive and functional decline in adults aged 75 and older. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50, 19781986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boss, L., Kang, D.-H. and Branson, S. (2015). Loneliness and cognitive function in the older adult: a systematic review. International Psychogeriatrics, 27, 541553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, H., Hofer, S. M., Mackinnon, A. J., Korten, A. E., Jorm, A. F. and Henderson, A. S. (2001). Age is no kinder to the better educated: absence of an association investigated using latent growth techniques in a community sample. Psychological Medicine, 31, 1528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comijs, H. C., Dik, M. G., Aartsen, M. J., Deeg, D. J. and Jonker, C. (2005). The impact of change in cognitive functioning and cognitive decline on disability, well-being, and the use of healthcare services in older persons. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 19, 316323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Debette, S. et al. (2011). Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology, 77, 461468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Directorate-General of Budget, Accounting and Statistics, Executive Yuan (2005). Survey on Social Development Trends, 2004 (AA200007) [data file]. Available from Survey Research Data Archive, Academia Sinica. doi: 10.6141/TW-SRDA-AA200007-1 Google Scholar
Farfel, J. M. et al. (2013). Very low levels of education and cognitive reserve A clinicopathologic study. Neurology, 81, 650657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fillenbaum, G. G., Hanlon, J. T., Landerman, L. R. and Schmader, K. E. (2001). Impact of estrogen use on decline in cognitive function in a representative sample of older community-resident women. American Journal of Epidemiology, 153, 137144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, C. et al. (2011). Anticholinergic medication use and cognitive impairment in the older population: the medical research council cognitive function and ageing study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 59, 14771483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallucci, M. et al. (2009). Physical activity, socialization and reading in the elderly over the age of seventy: what is the relation with cognitive decline? Evidence from “The Treviso Longeva (TRELONG) study”. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 48, 284286.10.1016/j.archger.2008.02.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gauthier, S. et al. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment. The Lancet, 367, 12621270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghisletta, P., Bickel, J. F. and Lovden, M. (2006). Does activity engagement protect against cognitive decline in old age? Methodological and analytical considerations. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61, P253P261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glei, D. A., Landau, D. A., Goldman, N., Chuang, Y. L., Rodriguez, G. and Weinstein, M. (2005). Participating in social activities helps preserve cognitive function: an analysis of a longitudinal, population-based study of the elderly. International Journal of Epidemiology, 34, 864871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, S. L., Hanlon, J. T., Landerman, L. R., Artz, M., Schmader, K. E. and Fillenbaum, G. G. (2003). Is antioxidant use protective of cognitive function in the community-dwelling elderly? American Journal of Geriatric Pharmacotherapy, 1, 310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanlon, J. T., Landerman, L. R., Artz, M. B., Gray, S. L., Fillenbaum, G. G. and Schmader, K. E. (2004). Histamine2 receptor antagonist use and decline in cognitive function among community dwelling elderly. Pharmacoepidemiology and Drug Safety, 13, 781787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harada, C. N., Love, M. C. N. and Triebel, K. L. (2013). Normal cognitive aging. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, 29, 737752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedeker, D. and Gibbons, R. D. (2006). Longitudinal data analysis: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Hernan, M. A., Hernandez-Diaz, S. and Robins, J. M. (2004). A structural approach to selection bias. Epidemiology, 15, 615625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsiao, S., Chiu, H. and Liu, H. (1994). A replication of multidimensionality of Activities of Daily Living (ADL): on the elderly in Southern Taiwan. Gaoxiong yi xue ke xue za zhi= The Kaohsiung Journal of Medical Sciences, 10, 449457.Google ScholarPubMed
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J. and Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging, 14, 245263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, J. M., Hammerman-Rozenberg, R., Cohen, A. and Stessman, J. (2008). Reading daily predicts reduced mortality among men from a cohort of community-dwelling 70-year-olds. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63, S73S80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, A. S., Flicker, L. J. and Lichtenberg, P. A. (2006). Reading ability mediates the relationship between education and executive function tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 6471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawashima, R. et al. (2005). Reading aloud and arithmetic calculation improve frontal function of people with dementia. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 60, 380384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, M. E. et al. (2017). The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review. Systematic Reviews, 6, 259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lachman, M. E., Agrigoroaei, S., Murphy, C. and Tun, P. A. (2010). Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lara, E., Koyanagi, A., Domènech-Abella, J., Miret, M., Ayuso-Mateos, J. L. and Haro, J. M. (2017). The impact of depression on the development of mild cognitive impairment over 3 years of follow-up: a population-based study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 43, 155169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, A. T., Richards, M., Chan, W. C., Chiu, H. F., Lee, R. S. and Lam, L. C. (2017). Lower risk of incident dementia among Chinese older adults having three servings of vegetables and two servings of fruits a day. Age and Ageing, 46, 773779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, A. T., Richards, M., Chan, W. C., Chiu, H. F., Lee, R. S. and Lam, L. C. (2018). Association of daily intellectual activities with lower risk of incident dementia among older Chinese adults. JAMA Psychiatry, 75, 697703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C. T. et al. (2012). Leisure activity, mobility limitation and stress as modifiable risk factors for depressive symptoms in the elderly: results of a national longitudinal study. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 54, e221229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, G. T. et al. (2011). Examining the association between late-life leisure activity participation and global cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly Chinese in Hong Kong. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 26, 3947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, Y.-C. et al. (2005). Cognitive reserve: a SPECT study of 132 Alzheimer’s disease patients with an education range of 0–19 years. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 20, 814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindstrom, H. A. et al. (2005). The relationships between television viewing in midlife and the development of Alzheimer’s disease in a case-control study. Brain and Cognition, 58, 157165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, R. J. (1995). Modeling the drop-out mechanism in repeated-measures studies. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 90, 11121121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litwin, H., Schwartz, E. and Damri, N. (2017). Cognitively stimulating leisure activity and subsequent cognitive function: a SHARE-based analysis. The Gerontologist, 57, 940948.Google ScholarPubMed
Lopes, M. A., Ferrioli, E., Nakano, E. Y., Litvoc, J. and Bottino, C. M. (2012). High prevalence of dementia in a community-based survey of older people from Brazil: association with intellectual activity rather than education. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 32, 307316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovden, M., Ghisletta, P. and Lindenberger, U. (2005). Social participation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old age. Psychology and Aging, 20, 423434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, D. Y., Wong, C. H., Leung, G. T., Fung, A. W., Chan, W. C. and Lam, L. C. (2017). Physical exercise helped to maintain and restore functioning in Chinese older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a 5-year prospective study of the Hong Kong Memory and Ageing Prospective Study (HK-MAPS). Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 18, 306311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackinnon, A., Christensen, H., Hofer, S., Korten, A. and Jorm, A. (2003). Use it and still lose it? The association between activity and cognitive performance established using latent growth techniques in a community sample. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 10, 215229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehta, K. M., Yaffe, K. and Covinsky, K. E. (2002). Cognitive impairment, depressive symptoms, and functional decline in older people. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50, 10451050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meng, X. and D’Arcy, C. (2012). Education and dementia in the context of the cognitive reserve hypothesis: a systematic review with meta-analyses and qualitative analyses. PLoS One, 7, e38268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muniz-Terrera, G., Matthews, F., Dening, T., Huppert, F. A. and Brayne, C. (2009). Education and trajectories of cognitive decline over 9 years in very old people: methods and risk analysis. Age Ageing, 38, 277282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northey, J. M., Cherbuin, N., Pumpa, K. L., Smee, D. J. and Rattray, B. (2018). Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 52, 154160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, K. et al. (2011). Predicting disease progression in Alzheimer’s disease: the role of neuropsychiatric syndromes on functional and cognitive decline. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 24, 3545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R. C. et al. (2018). Practice guideline update summary: mild cognitive impairment. Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 90, 126135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potvin, O., Forget, H., Grenier, S., Préville, M. and Hudon, C. (2011). Anxiety, depression, and 1-year incident cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 59, 14211428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prince, M. J. (2015). World Alzheimer Report 2015: the global impact of dementia: an analysis of prevalence, incidence, cost and trends: Alzheimer’s Disease International.Google Scholar
Richards, M. and Deary, I. (2010). Cognitive capital in the British birth cohorts: an introduction. Longitudinal and Life Course Studies, 1, 197200.Google Scholar
Rossor, M. and Knapp, M. (2015). Can we model a cognitive footprint of interventions and policies to help to meet the global challenge of dementia? The Lancet, 386, 10081010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, J. W. and Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. The Gerontologist, 37, 433440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachs, G. A. et al. (2011). Cognitive impairment: an independent predictor of excess mortality: a cohort study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 155, 300308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solfrizzi, V. et al. (2017). Relationships of dietary patterns, foods, and micro-and macronutrients with Alzheimer’s disease and late-life cognitive disorders: a systematic review. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 59, 815849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St. John, P. D. and Montgomery, P. R. (2010). Cognitive impairment and life satisfaction in older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 814821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47, 20152028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todd, S., Barr, S., Roberts, M. and Passmore, A. P. (2013). Survival in dementia and predictors of mortality: a review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28, 11091124.Google ScholarPubMed
Uchida, S. and Kawashima, R. (2008). Reading and solving arithmetic problems improves cognitive functions of normal aged people: a randomized controlled study. Age (Dordr), 30, 2129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verghese, J. et al. (2006). Leisure activities and the risk of amnestic mild cognitive impairment in the elderly. Neurology, 66, 821827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. S. et al. (2002). Cognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology, 59, 19101914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. S., Yu, L., Lamar, M., Schneider, J. A., Boyle, P. A. and Bennett, D. A. (2019). Education and cognitive reserve in old age. Neurology, 92, e1041e1050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2002). Active ageing: A policy framework (No. WHO/NMH/NPH/02.8). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Wu, M.-S., Lan, T.-H., Chen, C.-M., Chiu, H.-C. and Lan, T.-Y. (2011). Socio-demographic and health-related factors associated with cognitive impairment in the elderly in Taiwan. BMC Public Health, 11, 22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Y.-T. et al. (2013). Prevalence studies of dementia in mainland China, Hong Kong and Taiwan: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One, 8, e66252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yen, C. H. et al. (2010). Determinants of cognitive impairment over time among the elderly in Taiwan: results of the national longitudinal study. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 50(Suppl. 1), S5357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of participants of the Taiwan Longitudinal Study on Aging, 1993–2007

Figure 1

Table 2. Effects of reading frequencies (higher versus lower) on decline in cognitive function among the TLSA participants with SPMSQ errors ≤ 2 at baseline

Figure 2

Table 3. Effects of reading frequencies on decline in cognitive function among the TLSA participants with inclusion of those with SPMSQ errors >2 at baseline

Figure 3

Figure 1. The combination effects of reading and education on the risk of cognitive decline at 6 years, 10 years, and 14 years of follow-ups in the TLSA participants. Results from logistic regression with interaction terms between education and reading based on Model 3 with adjustments for age, sex, ethnicity, marital status, financial status, smoking, alcohol drinking, outdoor activities, number of comorbidities, diabetes, stroke, sight, and the number of SPMSQ errors at baseline using the inverse probability weighting method. Participants with baseline SPMSQ ≤2 were included. Adjusted odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals are presented; R(L) denotes the groups with lower reading frequencies. R(H) denotes the groups with higher reading frequencies; Lower education refers to those who were not formally educated; middle education refers to 1–6 years of formal education; higher education refers to those who have had formal education for 7 years or more. Planned comparisons between low and high reading groups at the same education level are performed and p values of Wald tests for planned comparisons are reported.

Figure 4

Table 4. Effects of education on decline in cognitive function among the TLSA participants with SPMSQ errors ≤2 at baseline

Supplementary material: File

Chang et al. Supplementary Materials

Chang et al. Supplementary Materials

Download Chang et al. Supplementary Materials(File)
File 34.9 KB