Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T04:32:25.209Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are superagers super rare?

Commentary on “Frequency of cognitive ‘super-ageing’ in three Australian samples using different diagnostic criteria” by Powell et al.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2024

Yuta Katsumi*
Affiliation:
Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, USA
Alexandra Touroutoglou
Affiliation:
Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, USA Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA

Abstract

Type
Commentary
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Psychogeriatric Association

Cognitive abilities typically decline as people age, yet there is substantial individual variation. Previous research on remarkable older adults sometimes called “superagers” has demonstrated that age-related cognitive decline is not inevitable and that some individuals exhibit memory function that is comparable to younger adults. The term superaging was coined by Mesulam, Rogalski, and colleagues, who defined superagers as individuals over 80 years old whose delayed recall score of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test was at least as good as normative values for 50–65 year olds (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Weintraub, Mesulam and Rogalski2012). Based on a similar measure of memory function (the long delay free recall score of the California Verbal Learning Test [CVLT]), our group was the first to identify superagers in a younger cohort of older individuals (60–80 years old). These superagers exhibited memory performance comparable to 18–32 year olds (Sun et al., Reference Sun, Stepanovic, Andreano, Barrett, Touroutoglou and Dickerson2016).

Subsequent studies of superaging have employed differing age ranges, neuropsychological tests, and in some cases, longitudinal repeated measures. This has resulted in a number of criteria for classifying an older adult as a superager. In addition to the minimum age of superagers (e.g. 60 or above vs. 80 or above), studies also vary in the age of the reference group; for example, some studies of superagers over 60 years old have compared their performance on the CVLT with 18–32 year olds (Katsumi et al., Reference Katsumi, Andreano, Barrett, Dickerson and Touroutoglou2021; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Stepanovic, Andreano, Barrett, Touroutoglou and Dickerson2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Andreano, Dickerson, Touroutoglou and Barrett2019), whereas others compared performance with 30–44 year olds (Dang et al., Reference Dang, Harrington, Lim, Ames, Hassenstab, Laws, Yassi, Hickey, Rainey-Smith, Robertson, Rowe, Sohrabi, Salvado, Weinborn, Villemagne, Masters and Maruff2019). While superagers are most defined by a combination of measures of episodic memory and executive function, some studies have begun utilizing measures of global cognition as well as performance in other cognitive domains (Maccora et al., Reference Maccora, Peters and Anstey2021; Pezzoli et al., Reference Pezzoli, Giorgio, Martersteck, Dobyns, Harrison and Jagust2023). Finally, some studies have defined superagers longitudinally by requiring them to maintain youthful performance in memory and other cognitive domains over 12 years (Maccora et al., Reference Maccora, Peters and Anstey2021). Despite the considerable heterogeneity in these definitions of superagers, no studies to date have compared them within the same sample to examine how these differences might lead to differing estimates of the prevalence of superagers.

The results presented by Powell et al. (Reference Powell, Lam, Foxe, Close, Sachdev and Brodaty2023) published in International Psychogeriatrics are significant because they demonstrate how the frequency of superagers in a given sample is affected by the precise demographic and neuropsychological criteria used to define them. In this study, different superaging criteria were variably associated with functional impairment, neuroimaging features, and dementia incidence. Specifically, in examining three independent cohorts of older adults using nine different definitionsFootnote 1 of superagers, the authors found that the prevalence of superagers varied from 2.9%, by the most stringent criteria, to as high as 43.7%. This finding suggests that, although superagers have sometimes been conceptualized as a rare subgroup of older adults (e.g. Maher et al., Reference Maher, Makowski-Woidan, Kuang, Zhang, Weintraub, Mesulam and Rogalski2022), exactly how rare they are might depend on the specific definition. Not surprisingly, agreement between superaging definitions was also variable; higher agreement was observed when those with similar neuropsychological criteria were compared. It is noteworthy that the highest agreement (κ = 0.83) was found between the definitions of superagers above the age of 60 years that varied in the age of the reference group (i.e. 18–32 vs. 30–44 year olds). This finding suggests that the frequency of superagers in a given sample is not affected fundamentally by the age of younger adults with whom superagers are compared.

In addition to prevalence estimates, the Powell et al., study adds to the literature of resilience to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) in superaging by showing that superagers have lower incidence of dementia. This finding is in line with another study published in International Psychogeriatrics in which the authors found that older adults who had maintained episodic memory function over 10–15 years showed a decreased risk of developing dementia compared with those who had exhibited memory decline (Josefsson et al., Reference Josefsson, Sundström, Pudas, Adolfsson, Nyberg and Adolfsson2023). The low incidence of dementia found in superagers is consistent with prior evidence demonstrating that while superagers and their peers did not differ in polygenic risk for AD (Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, Banks, Dale, Brewer, Makowski-Woidan, Weintraub, Mesulam, Geula and Rogalski2022) or levels of amyloid plaques in their brains (Borelli et al., Reference Borelli, Leal-Conceição, Andrade, Esper, Feltes, Soder, Matushita, Hartmann, Radaelli, Schilling, Moriguchi-Jeckel, Marques da Silva, Portuguez, Franco and da Costa2021; Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Maass, Baker and Jagust2018), they nonetheless showed no memory decline, suggesting that they may be resilient to negative effects of the disease on cognition. Superagers seem to also have reduced tau pathology accumulation compared with their peers (Nassif et al., Reference Nassif, Kawles, Ayala, Minogue, Gill, Shepard, Zouridakis, Keszycki, Zhang, Mao, Flanagan, Bigio, Mesulam, Rogalski, Geula and Gefen2022; Pezzoli et al., Reference Pezzoli, Giorgio, Martersteck, Dobyns, Harrison and Jagust2023), suggesting that their brains may be more resistant to this aspect of AD-related neuropathologic changes. These findings have important implications for research on preventing cognitive impairment associated with AD.

While the study by Powell et al. (Reference Powell, Lam, Foxe, Close, Sachdev and Brodaty2023) makes a novel contribution to the literature on superaging, it is important to acknowledge its limitations. Powell et al., found that superagers and non-superager participants did not consistently differ in regional brain volumes across definitions, although increased size in specific brain regions including the mid-cingulate cortex (MCC, also called caudal anterior cingulate cortex [ACC]) and hippocampus have been commonly reported in neuroimaging studies of superaging (Borelli et al., Reference Borelli, Schilling, Radaelli, Ferreira, Pisani, Portuguez and da Costa2018; Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Maass, Baker and Jagust2018; Katsumi et al., Reference Katsumi, Wong, Cavallari, Fong, Alsop, Andreano, Carvalho, Brickhouse, Jones, Libermann, Marcantonio, Schmitt, Shafi, Pascual-Leone, Travison, Barrett, Inouye, Dickerson and Touroutoglou2022; Pezzoli et al., Reference Pezzoli, Giorgio, Martersteck, Dobyns, Harrison and Jagust2023; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Stepanovic, Andreano, Barrett, Touroutoglou and Dickerson2016). Additionally, the thickness and degree of intrinsic functional connectivity of these regions are associated with better memory abilities in both superagers and typical older adults (Sun et al., Reference Sun, Stepanovic, Andreano, Barrett, Touroutoglou and Dickerson2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Andreano, Dickerson, Touroutoglou and Barrett2019). These findings are further supported by recent evidence showing high metabolic activity in both the hippocampus and the broader cingulate cortex in superagers (Borelli et al., Reference Borelli, Leal-Conceição, Andrade, Esper, Feltes, Soder, Matushita, Hartmann, Radaelli, Schilling, Moriguchi-Jeckel, Marques da Silva, Portuguez, Franco and da Costa2021). The anterior MCC in particular has been previously described as a key region of a neural signature of superaging (Sun et al., Reference Sun, Stepanovic, Andreano, Barrett, Touroutoglou and Dickerson2016) and an “important region involved in the neurocircuitry of underlying successful aging” (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Maass, Baker and Jagust2018), making it a potential imaging biomarker for resilience to age-related cognitive decline. Notably, however, Powell et al., found no evidence of this “neural signature” of superaging.

The null imaging results reported by Powell et al., may be in part due to the parameters of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data acquisition, processing, and/or analysis. Specifically, MRI data analyzed in the Powell et al., study were collected using four different scanners that varied in field strengths (1.5 and 3 T) and manufacturers. Higher field strength MRI generally yields an enhanced signal contrast between tissue compartments, which could result in larger regional gray matter morphometric estimates (by as much as ∼30%; Buchanan et al., Reference Buchanan, Muñoz Maniega, Valdés Hernández, Ballerini, Barclay, Taylor, Russ, Tucker-Drob, Wardlaw, Deary, Bastin and Cox2021). In group-level analyses of regional brain volumes, it is also a standard practice to control for total intracranial volume (i.e. head size), although this adjustment was not reported in Powell et al. Prior work employing vertex-wise analyses of surface-based MRI data have consistently identified between-group differences in MCC thickness; however, the precise anatomical location and spatial extent of such differences was somewhat variable from one study to another (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Weintraub, Mesulam and Rogalski2012, Reference Harrison, Maass, Baker and Jagust2018; Katsumi et al., Reference Katsumi, Wong, Cavallari, Fong, Alsop, Andreano, Carvalho, Brickhouse, Jones, Libermann, Marcantonio, Schmitt, Shafi, Pascual-Leone, Travison, Barrett, Inouye, Dickerson and Touroutoglou2022; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Stepanovic, Andreano, Barrett, Touroutoglou and Dickerson2016). It is therefore possible that Powell et al.’s analytical approach based on anatomically defined regions of interest might not have been sufficiently sensitive to characteristic neural differences between superagers and typical older adults. Altogether, these technical considerations are important to address so that comparisons across studies would be more meaningful.

There are several outstanding issues that warrant clarifications in future studies of superaging. First, the vast majority of prior studies on superaging are cross-sectional in nature; more longitudinal studies are needed to investigate how aspects of cognition and brain integrity in superagers change over time relative to non-superagers. One study reported that superagers did not show performance decline on measures of episodic memory, attention, language, and executive function over a 18-month period (Gefen et al., Reference Gefen, Shaw, Whitney, Martersteck, Stratton, Rademaker, Weintraub, Mesulam and Rogalski2014); another study showed that superagers had slower decline in episodic memory function over a 5-year period compared with typically aging older adults (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Maass, Baker and Jagust2018). Superagers were also ∼70% less likely to receive a clinical diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment or dementia over an 8-year period than their cognitively normal counterparts, despite the similar proportion of amyloid-positive participants and APOE ɛ4 carriers in both samples (Dang et al., Reference Dang, Harrington, Lim, Ames, Hassenstab, Laws, Yassi, Hickey, Rainey-Smith, Robertson, Rowe, Sohrabi, Salvado, Weinborn, Villemagne, Masters and Maruff2019). Interestingly, superagers and typical older adults show age- and AD-related cortical atrophy at comparable rates over 8 years, suggesting that the former group may show resilience to these changes. More work is needed to better understand the trajectory of cognitive and brain aging in superagers versus typical older adults and examine the role of other factors, including lifestyle, fitness, genetic, and social influences. In social domains, loneliness may be particularly important to consider because of its association with increased dementia risk, as highlighted by recent work in International Psychogeriatrics (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Aschwanden, Zhu, Stephan and Terracciano2023).

Second, current evidence on superaging is largely based on samples of Western and highly educated individuals, limiting the generalizability of findings to the rest of the population. There is evidence suggesting that culture and age interact to influence episodic memory and its mechanisms, including the trajectory of age-related decline (Lipnicki et al., Reference Lipnicki, Crawford, Dutta, Thalamuthu, Kochan, Andrews, Lima-Costa, Castro-Costa, Brayne, Matthews, Stephan, Lipton, Katz, Ritchie, Scali, Ancelin, Scarmeas, Yannakoulia and Dardiotis2017). As such, future work should characterize and compare superagers across cultures to better understand how cultural and societal values might modulate youthful memory function and brain integrity in late adulthood.

Third, neuroimaging studies of superaging have thus far primarily focused on examining anatomical features (e.g. gray matter volume/thickness and white matter integrity) and little is known about their brain function. Prior work on intrinsic functional connectivity estimated from resting-state functional MRI has revealed the involvement of the large-scale default mode network (including regions in the isocortico-hippocampal circuit) and the salience network (including frontoparietal and cingulate cortical regions) in differentiating superagers from typical older adults (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Andreano, Dickerson, Touroutoglou and Barrett2019). Superagers also exhibited youthful brain activation patterns during episodic memory encoding and retrieval, which were associated with their memory performance (Katsumi et al., Reference Katsumi, Andreano, Barrett, Dickerson and Touroutoglou2021). As more neuroimaging evidence becomes available, future work should integrate high-dimensional, multimodal imaging data to comprehensively characterize the unique properties of superagers’ brains.

Taken together, Powell et al.’s findings highlight the importance of considering the definitional variability in interpreting the results of superaging studies. The study marks an important first step toward much needed harmonization of definitions of superaging in multisite studies employing large and demographically diverse cohorts with the goal to better understand the phenomenon of superaging.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Description of authors’ roles

Yuta Katsumi and Alexandra Touroutoglou both wrote and approved the submitted version.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grants K01 AG084820, K23 DC016912, and R21 AG080588.

Footnotes

1 It is important to acknowledge that four of these definitions came from studies that did not explicitly use the term “superagers.” These studies identified older individuals with superior cognition relative to their own age group and not to younger adults. For simplicity, we consider in all nine definitions variants of superaging in this commentary.

References

Borelli, W. V., Leal-Conceição, E., Andrade, M. A., Esper, N. B., Feltes, P. K., Soder, R. B., Matushita, C. S., Hartmann, L. M., Radaelli, G., Schilling, L. P., Moriguchi-Jeckel, C., Marques da Silva, A. M., Portuguez, M. W., Franco, A. R., & da Costa, J. C. (2021). Increased glucose activity in subgenual anterior cingulate and hippocampus of high performing older adults, despite amyloid burden. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 81(4), 14191428. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-210063 Google Scholar
Borelli, W. V., Schilling, L. P., Radaelli, G., Ferreira, L. B., Pisani, L., Portuguez, M. W., & da Costa, J. C. (2018). Neurobiological findings associated with high cognitive performance in older adults: A systematic review. International Psychogeriatrics, 30(12), 18131825. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610218000431 Google Scholar
Buchanan, C. R., Muñoz Maniega, S., Valdés Hernández, M. C., Ballerini, L., Barclay, G., Taylor, A. M., Russ, T. C., Tucker-Drob, E. M., Wardlaw, J. M., Deary, I. J., Bastin, M. E., & Cox, S. R. (2021). Comparison of structural MRI brain measures between 1.5 and 3 T: Data from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Human Brain Mapping, 42(12), 39053921. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25473 Google Scholar
Dang, C., Harrington, K. D., Lim, Y. Y., Ames, D., Hassenstab, J., Laws, S. M., Yassi, N., Hickey, M., Rainey-Smith, S. R., Robertson, J., Rowe, C. C., Sohrabi, H. R., Salvado, O., Weinborn, M., Villemagne, V. L., Masters, C. L., Maruff, P., & AIBL Research Group (2019). Superior memory reduces 8-year risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia but not amyloid β-associated cognitive decline in older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 34(5), 585598. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acy078 Google Scholar
Gefen, T., Shaw, E., Whitney, K., Martersteck, A., Stratton, J., Rademaker, A., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M.-M., & Rogalski, E. (2014). Longitudinal neuropsychological performance of cognitive superagers. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 62(8), 15981600. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.12967 Google Scholar
Harrison, T. M., Maass, A., Baker, S. L., & Jagust, W. J. (2018). Brain morphology, cognition, and β-amyloid in older adults with superior memory performance. Neurobiology of Aging, 67, 162170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.03.024 Google Scholar
Harrison, T. M., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M.-M., & Rogalski, E. (2012). Superior memory and higher cortical volumes in unusually successful cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18(6), 10811085. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617712000847 Google Scholar
Josefsson, M., Sundström, A., Pudas, S., Adolfsson, A. N., Nyberg, L., & Adolfsson, R. (2023). Memory profiles predict dementia over 23-28 years in normal but not successful aging. International Psychogeriatrics, 35(7), 351359. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610219001844 Google Scholar
Katsumi, Y., Andreano, J. M., Barrett, L. F., Dickerson, B. C., & Touroutoglou, A. (2021). Greater neural differentiation in the ventral visual cortex is associated with youthful memory in superaging. Cerebral Cortex, 31(11), 52755287. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab157 Google Scholar
Katsumi, Y., Wong, B., Cavallari, M., Fong, T. G., Alsop, D. C., Andreano, J. M., Carvalho, N., Brickhouse, M., Jones, R., Libermann, T. A., Marcantonio, E. R., Schmitt, E., Shafi, M. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Travison, T., Barrett, L. F., Inouye, S. K., Dickerson, B. C., & Touroutoglou, A. (2022). Structural integrity of the anterior mid-cingulate cortex contributes to resilience to delirium in Superaging. Brain Communications, 4(4), fcac163. https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcac163 Google Scholar
Lipnicki, D. M., Crawford, J. D., Dutta, R., Thalamuthu, A., Kochan, N. A., Andrews, G., Lima-Costa, M. F., Castro-Costa, E., Brayne, C., Matthews, F. E., Stephan, B. C. M., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Ritchie, K., Scali, J., Ancelin, M.-L., Scarmeas, N., Yannakoulia, M., Dardiotis, E., … Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium (COSMIC) (2017). Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: A collaborative cohort study. PLOS Medicine, 14(3), e1002261. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261 Google Scholar
Maccora, J., Peters, R., & Anstey, K. J. (2021). Gender differences in superior-memory superagers and associated factors in an Australian cohort. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 40(4), 433442. https://doi.org/10.1177/0733464820902943 Google Scholar
Maher, A. C., Makowski-Woidan, B., Kuang, A., Zhang, H., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M. M., & Rogalski, E. (2022). Neuropsychological profiles of older adults with superior versus average episodic memory: The Northwestern “SuperAger” cohort. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 28(6), 563573. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617721000837 Google Scholar
Nassif, C., Kawles, A., Ayala, I., Minogue, G., Gill, N. P., Shepard, R. A., Zouridakis, A., Keszycki, R., Zhang, H., Mao, Q., Flanagan, M. E., Bigio, E. H., Mesulam, M.-M., Rogalski, E., Geula, C., & Gefen, T. (2022). Integrity of neuronal size in the entorhinal cortex is a biological substrate of exceptional cognitive aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 42(45), 85878594. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0679-22.2022 Google Scholar
Pezzoli, S., Giorgio, J., Martersteck, A., Dobyns, L., Harrison, T. M., & Jagust, W. J. (2023). Successful cognitive aging is associated with thicker anterior cingulate cortex and lower tau deposition compared to typical aging. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.13438 Google Scholar
Powell, A., Lam, B. C. P., Foxe, D., Close, J. C. T., Sachdev, P. S., & Brodaty, H. (2023). Frequency of cognitive “super-aging” in three Australian samples using different diagnostic criteria. International Psychogeriatrics, 117. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610223000935 Google Scholar
Spencer, B. E., Banks, S. J., Dale, A. M., Brewer, J. B., Makowski-Woidan, B., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M.-M., Geula, C., & Rogalski, E. (2022). Alzheimer’s polygenic hazard score in Superagers: SuperGenes or SuperResilience? Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Translational Research & Clinical Interventions, 8(1), e12321. https://doi.org/10.1002/trc2.12321 Google Scholar
Sun, F. W., Stepanovic, M. R., Andreano, J., Barrett, L. F., Touroutoglou, A., & Dickerson, B. C. (2016). Youthful brains in older adults: Preserved neuroanatomy in the default mode and salience networks contributes to youthful memory in superaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(37), 96599668. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1492-16.2016 Google Scholar
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., Aschwanden, D., Zhu, X., Stephan, Y., & Terracciano, A. (2023). Loneliness and risk of all-cause, Alzheimer’s, vascular, and frontotemporal dementia: A prospective study of 492,322 individuals over 15 years. International Psychogeriatrics, 35(6), 283292. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610222001028 Google Scholar
Zhang, J., Andreano, J. M., Dickerson, B. C., Touroutoglou, A., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). Stronger functional connectivity in the default mode and salience networks is associated with youthful memory in superaging. Cerebral Cortex, 30(1), 7284. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz071 Google Scholar