Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T01:52:40.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Accounting for differences in cognitive health between older adults in New Zealand and the USA

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 December 2014

Christine Stephens*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand
John Spicer
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand
Claire Budge
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand
Brendan Stevenson
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand
Fiona Alpass
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Christine Stephens, School of Psychology, Massey University, Private Bag 11 222, Palmerston North, New Zealand. Phone: +64 63505799; Fax: +64 63505679. Email: [email protected].
Get access

Abstract

Background:

National differences in cognitive health of older adults provide an opportunity to shed light on etiological factors. We compared the cognitive health of older adults in New Zealand and the USA, and examined differences in known risk factors.

Methods:

Two nationally representative samples were derived from the 2010 waves of the New Zealand Longitudinal Study of Ageing (n = 953) and the US Health and Retirement Study (HRS) (n = 3,746). Data from comparable measures of cognitive function, gender, age, income, education, prevalence of cancer, diabetes, heart disease, hypertension and stroke, exercise, alcohol consumption, smoker status, depression, and self-reported health were subjected to hierarchical regression analysis to examine how national differences in cognitive function might be explained by differences in these risk factors.

Results:

The New Zealand sample scored 4.4 points higher on average than the US sample on the 43 point cognitive scale. Regression analyses of the combined samples showed that poorer cognitive health is more likely in those who are male, older, less educated, have suffered a stroke, consume alcohol less frequently, are more depressed, and report worse overall health. Controlling for age and sex reduced the mean difference to 2.6 and controlling for risk factors further reduced it to 2.3.

Conclusions:

Older New Zealand adults displayed better cognitive function than those in a US sample. This advantage can be partially explained by age and sex differences and, to some extent, by differences in known risk factors. However, the national advantage remained even when all measured risk factors are statistically controlled.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albert, M. S. et al. (1995). Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 10, 578589.Google Scholar
Alpass, F. et al. (2007). Independence, well-being, and social participation in an aging population. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1114, 241250. doi: 10.1196/annals.1396.009.Google Scholar
Anstey, K. and Christensen, H. (2000). Education, activity, health, blood pressure and Apolipoprotein E as predictors of cognitive change in old age: a review. Gerontology, 46, 163177. doi: 10.1159/000022153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anstey, K. J., Mack, H. A. and Cherbuin, N. (2009). Alcohol consumption as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline. Meta-analysis of prospective studies. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 542555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anstey, K. J., von Sanden, C., Salim, A. and O’Kearney, R. (2007). Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. American Journal of Epidemiology, 166, 367378.Google Scholar
Brandt, J., Spencer, M. and Folstein, M. (1988). The telephone interview for cognitive status. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 1, 111117.Google Scholar
Bunce, D., Batterham, P., Mackinnon, A. and Christensen, H. (2012). Depression, anxiety and cognition in community-dwelling adults aged 70 years and over. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46, 16621666. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.08.023.Google Scholar
Callow, L. and Alpass, F. (2014). Cognitive functioning. Summary Report for the New Zealand Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Palmerston North: Massey University. Available at: http://www.massey.ac.nz/?pabaa4609a.Google Scholar
Campbell, A., McCosh, L. and Reinken, J. (1983). Dementia in old age and the need for services. Age and Ageing, 12, 1116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elwood, P. et al. (2013). Healthy lifestyles reduce the incidence of chronic diseases and dementia: evidence from the caerphilly cohort study. PloS one, 8, e81877.Google Scholar
Freitas, S., Simoes, M. R., Alves, L. and Santana, I. (2012). Montreal cognitive assessment: influence of sociodemographic and health variables. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 27, 165175. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acr116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzog, A. R. and Wallace, R. B. (1997). Measures of cognitive functioning in the AHEAD Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 52, 3748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kivipelto, M. et al. (2001). Midlife vascular risk factors and Alzheimer's disease in later life: longitudinal, population based study. British Medical Journal, 322, 14471451.Google Scholar
Kumari, M. and Marmot, M. (2005). Diabetes and cognitive function in a middle-aged cohort: findings from the whitehall II study. Neurology, 65, 15971603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langa, K. M. et al. (2009). Cognitive health among older adults in the United States and in England. BMC Geriatrics, 9, 23. doi:10.1186/1471-2318-9-23.Google Scholar
Melzer, D., Ely, M. and Brayne, C. (1997). Cognitive impairment in elderly people: population based estimate of the future in England, Scotland, and Wales. British Medical Journal, 315, 462. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7106.462.Google Scholar
Moreno-John, G. et al. (2004). Ethnic minority older adults participating in clinical research: developing trust. Journal of Ageing and Health, 16 (Suppl. 1), 93S123S.Google Scholar
Ofstedal, M., Fisher, G. and Herzog, R. (2005). HRS/AHEAD Documentation Report: Documentation of Cognitive Functioning Measures in the Health and Retirement Study. Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Research Center, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Ofstedal, M., Weir, D. R., Chen, K-T. and Wagner, J. (2011). Updates to HRS Sample Weights. Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Research Center, University of Michigan. Retrieved (12/05/2014) from: http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-013.pdf.Google Scholar
Perry, R. and Hodges, J. (2000). Relationship between functional and neuropsychological performance in early Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Association Disorders, 14, 110.Google Scholar
Plassman, B. L., Williams, J. J. W., Burke, J. R., Holsinger, T. and Benjamin, S. (2010). Systematic review: factors associated with risk for and possible prevention of cognitive decline in later life. Annals of Internal Medicine, 153, 182193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. and Locke, B. (2000). Center for epidemiologic studies depression scale (CES-D). In Rush, J. (ed.), Psychiatric Measures. Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, P. B., Mielke, M. M., Xue, Q. and Carlson, M. C. (2010). Depressive symptoms predict incident cognitive impairment in cognitive healthy older women. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 204211. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181c53487.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 507514. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023.Google Scholar
Singh-Manoux, A. et al. (2012). Timing and onset of cognitive decline: results from whitehall II prospective cohort study. British Medical Journal, 344, 28. doi:10.1136/bmj.d7622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuck, A. E. et al. (1999). Risk factors for functional status decline in community-living elderly people: a systematic literature review. Social Science & Medicine, 48, 445469.Google Scholar
Suthers, K., Jung, K. and Crimmins, E. (2003). Life Expectancy with cognitive impairment in the older population of the United States. The Journals of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 58, 179186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Towers, A. and Stevenson, B. (2014). NZLSA cohort profile. Technical Report for the New Zealand Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Palmerston North: Massey University.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. USA: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Winship, C. and Radbill, L. (1994). Sampling weights and regression analysis. Sociological Methods & Research, 23, 230257.Google Scholar