Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T03:01:39.108Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Absence of a relationship between subjective memory complaint and objective memory impairment in mild cognitive impairment (MCI): is it time to abandon subjective memory complaint as an MCI diagnostic criterion?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 May 2012

Megan Elizabeth Lenehan
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Australia
Shannon Zofia Klekociuk
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Australia
Mathew James Summers*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Australia Wicking Dementia Research and Education Centre, Menzies Research Institute Tasmania, Hobart, Australia
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Dr. Mathew J. Summers, School of Psychology, Locked Bag 1342, Launceston, Tasmania 7250, Australia. Phone: +61 3 6324 3266; Fax: +61 3 6324 3168. Email: [email protected].

Abstract

Background: Subjective memory complaints are a requirement in the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) as they are thought to indicate a decline in objective memory performance. However, recent research suggests that the relationship between subjective memory complaint and objective memory impairment is less clear. Thus, it is possible that many people without subjective memory complaints who develop Alzheimer's disease are precluded from a diagnosis of MCI.

Methods: The present study examined the relationship between subjective memory complaint assessed using the Multifactorial Memory Questionnaire (MMQ) and objective memory impairment assessed using standard neuropsychological measures in cases of amnestic MCI (n = 48), non-amnestic MCI (n = 27), and unimpaired healthy participants (n = 64).

Results: Correlational and regression analyses indicated that subjective memory complaints displayed a poor relationship with objective memory performance. A subsequent discriminant function analysis indicated that subjective memory complaints failed to improve the diagnostic accuracy of MCI and resulted in increased rates of false negative and false positive diagnoses.

Conclusion: The results of the present study suggest that a diagnostic criterion of subjective memory complaint reduces the accuracy of MCI diagnosis, resulting in an elevated rate of false positive and false negative diagnoses. The results of this study in conjunction with recent research indicate that a criterion of subjective memory complaint should be discarded from emerging diagnostic criteria for MCI.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albert, M. S. et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 270279.Google Scholar
Bassett, S. S. and Folstein, M. F. (1993). Memory complaint, memory performance, and psychiatric diagnosis: a community study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 6, 105111.Google Scholar
Busse, A., Bischkopf, J., Riedel-Heller, S. G. and Angermeyer, M. C. (2003). Subclassifications for mild cognitive impairment: prevalence and predictive validity. Psychological Medicine, 33, 10291038.Google Scholar
Chung, J. C. C. and Man, D. W. K. (2009). Self-appraised, informant-reported, and objective memory and cognitive function in mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 27, 187193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, S. and Marsiske, M. (2006). Subjective memory beliefs and cognitive performance in normal and mildly impaired older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 10, 413423.Google Scholar
Grober, E., Hall, C. B., Lipton, R. B., Zonderman, A. B., Resnick, S. M. and Kawas, C. (2008). Memory impairment, executive dysfunction, and intellectual decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 266278.Google Scholar
Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L. and Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate Data Analysis, 5th edn.Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Hohman, T. J., Beason-Held, L. L., Lamar, M. and Resnick, S. M. (2011). Subjective cognitive complaints and longitudinal changes in memory and brain function. Neuropsychology, 25, 125130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessen, F. et al. (2007). Patterns of subjective memory impairment in the elderly: association with memory performance. Psychological Medicine, 37, 17531762.Google Scholar
Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A. and Henderson, A. S. (2001). Memory complaints as a precursor of memory impairment in older people: a longitudinal analysis over 7–8 years. Psychological Medicine, 31, 441449.Google Scholar
Jungwirth, S., Fischer, P., Weissgram, S., Kirchmeyr, W., Bauer, P. and Tragl, K.-H. (2004). Subjective memory complaints and objective memory impairment in the Vienna-TransDanube aging community. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52, 263268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, T. et al. (2010). Risk factors for incident mild cognitive impairment: results from the German Study on Ageing, Cognition and Dementia in Primary Care Patients (AgeCoDe). Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 121, 260272.Google Scholar
Mattis, S. (2001). Dementia Rating Scale – 2 (DRS-2). Odessa, RL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Mendes, T. et al. (2008). Memory complaints in healthy young and elderly adults: reliability of memory reporting. Aging & Mental Health, 12, 177182.Google Scholar
Morris, J. C. et al. (2001). Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 58, 397405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearman, A. and Storandt, M. (2004). Predictors of subjective memory in older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59, P4P6.Google Scholar
Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 183194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Kokmen, E. and Tangelos, E. G. (1997). Aging, memory, and mild cognitive impairment. International Psychogeriatrics, 9, 6569.Google Scholar
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G. and Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56, 303308.Google Scholar
Petersen, R. C. et al. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 58, 19851992.Google Scholar
Psychological Corporation (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Purser, J. L., Fillenbaum, G. G., Pieper, C. F. and Wallace, R. B. (2005). Mild cognitive impairment and 10-year trajectories of disability in the Iowa established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderly cohort. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 19661972.Google Scholar
Reisberg, B. and Gauthier, S. (2008). Current evidence for subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) as the pre-mild cognitive impairment (MCI) stage of subsequently manifest Alzheimer's disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 20, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, N. L. J. and Summers, M. J. (2010). Attention and working memory deficits in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 350357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, N. L. J. and Summers, M. J. (2011). Longitudinal deficits to attention, executive and working memory in subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 25, 237248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snaith, R. P. and Zigmond, A. S. (1994). The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS): Manual. London: GL Assessment Ltd.Google Scholar
Solfrizzi, V. et al. (2004). Vascular risk factors, incidence of MCI, and rates of progression to dementia. Neurology, 63, 18821891.Google Scholar
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S. and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administrations, Norms, and Commentary, 3rd edn.New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Troyer, A. K. and Rich, J. B. (2002). Psychometric properties of a new metamemory questionnaire for older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, P19P27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhaeghen, P. and Salthouse, T. A. (1997). Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wahlund, L.-O., Pihlstrand, E. and Jonhagen, M. E. (2003). Mild cognitive impairment: experience from a memory clinic. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 107, 2124.Google Scholar
Ward, A., Arrighi, H. M., Michels, S. and Cedarbaum, J. M. (2012). Mild cognitive impairment: disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 8, 1421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1997a). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition (WAIS-III): Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1997b). Wechsler Memory Scale – Third Edition (WMS-III): Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Winblad, B. et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 240246.Google Scholar