Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:40:11.508Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Becoming and acting as an ally against weight-based discrimination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 March 2024

Christopher J. Waterbury*
Affiliation:
Portland State University, Portland, OR, USA
Larry R. Martinez
Affiliation:
The University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, TX, USA
Liana Bernard
Affiliation:
Portland State University, Portland, OR, USA
Nicholas A. Smith
Affiliation:
The University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, TX, USA
*
Corresponding author: Christopher J. Waterbury; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology

We appreciate and agree with the importance of the Best Practices for Weight at Work Research outlined by Lemmon et al. (Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2023). To help further contribute to this body of literature, we connect the scholarship related to weight-basedFootnote 1 discrimination to contemporary allyship scholarship. Allyship support and advocacy behaviors improve employee experiences on day-to-day and long-term bases, and are therefore critical to research about weight at work. It is critically important to examine the development of allies against weight-stigma for two reasons. First, misguided assumptions about the controllability of weight (and resultant expectations that the victims of weight-stigma are solely responsible for reducing the negativity they experience by changing their body size; Lemmon et al., Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2023) can be a mitigable but particularly pernicious barrier to people becoming allies against size based discrimination. Second, the direly misguided notion that certain discriminatory behaviors (e.g., commenting on others’ sizes, recommending size management strategies) are deserved, helpful, and welcome for people in larger or smaller bodies represents an example of inappropriate expression of mistreatment that allyship behaviors can address (Sniezek, Reference Sniezek2021). Importantly, such perspectives and behaviors are ultimately harmful for both the targets of weight-based discrimination and others around them (Major et al., Reference Major, Tomiyama, Hunger, Major, Dovidio and Link2018). As such, to support the development of allyship against weight-stigma in research, we draw from past research to (a) outline how individuals, including researchers, may develop into allies against weight-stigma, and (b) describe specific behaviors that allies can engage in to combat weight discrimination.

Development of allyship for weight-based discrimination

Ally identity development represents an individual-level, socio-cognitive, and behavioral transformation of the individual to curb the perpetuation of weight-stigma by oneself and others. Lemmon et al. (Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2023) assert that researchers should acknowledge uncomfortable feelings about weight and consider that disinterest not only impedes organizational research but also enables mistreatment. Answering this call to catalyze research through personal reflection, we draw inspiration from a recent synthesis of the ally identity development literature (Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Sabat, Ruggs, Hamilton, Bergman and Dray2023) to outline how individuals may progress from an inability or unwillingness to address weight-based discrimination to being successful allies for people in larger or smaller bodies.

The first of five stages of ally identity development is Apathy, which includes unawareness, disinterest, or tolerance of prejudice and discrimination. Apathy justification is likely to manifest around weight-stigma due to perceptions that the people with larger or smaller bodies are responsible for the discrimination that they experience due to perceived body size controllability. Indeed, perceived controllability and deservedness predict one’s likelihood to espouse anti-fat prejudice and/or fail to consider anti-fat prejudice to be harmful (Lemmon et al., Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2023). Furthermore, people in the apathetic stage are also more likely to actively justify (rather than suppress) their prejudicial beliefs due to controllability perceptions (Crandall & Eshleman, Reference Crandall and Eshleman2003). For researchers, apathy is reflected in research designs that conflate group characteristics and use downward and upward (e.g., inferior and superior) group comparisons (e.g., equivocating body size with health), thereby reifying mechanisms of workplace mistreatment (e.g., on the basis of body size; Nutter et al., Reference Nutter, Russell-Mayhew and Saunders2021). Apathy can also manifest as not considering or downplaying the importance of weight in research designs in which it would be appropriate to do so.

Progressing from Apathy typically requires experiencing Dissonance, the second stage of ally identity development (Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Sabat, Ruggs, Hamilton, Bergman and Dray2023). Dissonance is typically caused by hypocrisies including (a) a newfound awareness of prejudice and discrimination, and an appreciation of the resultant harm they can cause, and (b) a stark realization that one may have contributed to such oppression as a function of one’s contextually privileged identities. Thus, developing an ally identity against weight-stigma involves acknowledging that weight is often not controllable (Gordon, Reference Gordon2023; Lin & Stutts, Reference Lin and Stutts2020), that anti-fat bias exists (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Cash and Bubb-Lewis1997), that it is highly prevalent in workplaces (Puhl et al., Reference Puhl, Andreyeva and Brownell2008), and that it is imminently harmful to employees (Hunt & Rhodes, Reference Hunt and Rhodes2018; Major et al., Reference Major, Tomiyama, Hunger, Major, Dovidio and Link2018). As individuals become more aware of interpersonal mistreatment and denigrating media representations of people in larger bodies (Ravary et al., Reference Ravary, Baldwin and Bartz2019), along with global-scale prejudice, they are more likely to progress from the apathy stage.

Moving from the apathy stage to the dissonance stage can be challenging for many as it requires reflection on one’s own worldviews and lived experiences in society, including widely-held misinformation. The dissonance stage for allies against weight-stigma in research may include acknowledging the hypocrisy that health promotion programs that emphasize individual responsibility may actually increase the prevalence of weight-based discrimination (Täuber et al., Reference Täuber, Mulder and Flint2018). With regards to ally development, we highlight two hypocrisies that we have found beneficial to explore in anti weight-stigma organizational training ([redacted for naive review]). First, prospective allies should critically consider who benefits from diet culture in the US—the weight loss industry, currently worth more than $224bil (Facts & Factors, 2023). Second, people often believe that they are justified in apathy toward weight-stigma because they want others to be healthier. On the contrary, one’s health is not owed to others, and weight-stigma does not improve but harms health. Indeed, anti-health behaviors are associated with weight-stigma (Puhl & Suh, Reference Puhl and Suh2015), including eating disorders, which are the most deadly mental health condition, regardless of body size (NEDA, 2022). Not only does “dieting” not promote health, with 95% of intentional weight loss attempts failing (Bacon & Aphramor, Reference Bacon and Aphramor2011; Tylka et al., Reference Tylka, Annunziato, Burgard, Daníelsdóttir, Shuman, Davis and Calogero2014)—worse, one in four intentional weight loss attempts develop into eating disorders (NEDA, 2022). Thus, we prompt researchers to study behaviors that would be consistent with believing that weight-stigma causes injustice.

As Lemmon et al. (Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2023) suggested: “Read up!” For allies, resolving dissonance requires Learning, the third stage of ally identity development (Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Sabat, Ruggs, Hamilton, Bergman and Dray2023). Learning involves seeking out information, forming relationships, and finding local resources. Specifically, researchers in the learning stage of allyship against weight-stigma can read personal accounts and scientific literature related to weight-stigma, view media and media critiques focused on weight-stigma, follow body size and body positivity activists on social media, and contribute time and money to organizations that fight against weight-stigma. In particular, we recommend that researchers reflect inwardly to examine their relationships with their own bodies, as weight-stigma is often motivated by individuals’ relationships with their own bodies (Major et al., Reference Major, Tomiyama, Hunger, Major, Dovidio and Link2018). Through conscientious reflection, allies may reduce their projected weight-stigma toward others.Footnote 2

Fledgling allies tend to become more emboldened to actually engage in allyship behaviors as their knowledge increases. However, allies frequently make mistakes; thus the fourth stage of ally identity development is Stumbling. Researchers developing as allies against weight-stigma might stumble in designing research by implicitly or explicitly (a) drawing attention to people’s size, (b) commenting on people’s eating behaviors with unsolicited advice, (c) bringing up body positivity while assuming that another party wishes to discuss it merely because they are in a larger body, (d) speaking over people in larger or smaller bodies and thus removing their agency to empower themselves, or (e) highlighting unsolicited positive stereotypes (i.e., tokenizing body positivity, sassiness, or knowledge about cooking). For example, a qualitative study identified a common microaggression involving complimenting people in larger bodies for exercising: participants described unsolicited “praise” from strangers as evoking shame, reinforcing anti-fat stereotypes, and easily being dismissed by bystanders as words of “encouragement” (Sniezek, Reference Sniezek2021). All of these examples illustrate that allyship against weight-based discrimination is nuanced and tied to the controllability and deservedness characteristics of weight-stigma. Even well-intentioned behaviors can perpetuate weight-stigma.

The ideal balance of allyship beliefs and behaviors is conceptualized as Integration, the fifth stage of ally identity development. A theoretical endpoint on the allyship journey, Integration entails never stumbling, so it is best understood as an aspirational yet unrealistic goal (Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Sabat, Ruggs, Hamilton, Bergman and Dray2023). During integration, allies recognize that stumbling will occur and appreciate individuals who flag weight-stigma rather than apologizing or defending prejudiced statements—both of which displace responsibility from the ally to perform strategic ally behaviors, which we outline next. As such, researchers should be open to constructive criticism from others and actively seek to incorporate the perspectives of those in larger and smaller bodies in the design, implementation, and dissemination of their work, in line with participatory action research principles (see Bernard et al., Reference Bernard, Fox, Kulason, Phanphackdy, Kahle, Martinez, Praslova and Smith2023).

Strategies to combat weight-based discrimination

In this section, we draw from prior research that conceptualizes allyship behavior in two broad categories: supportive behaviors and advocacy behaviors (Martinez & Hebl, Reference Martinez and Hebl2010; Ready et al., Reference Ready, Snoevink, Martinez, Hamilton, Carsey, Eggler and Nittrouer (Chairs)2023; Sabat et al., Reference Sabat, Martinez and Wessel2013; Snoeyink et al., Reference Snoeyink, Martinez, Hamilton, Smith, Gardner, Ryan (Chairs) and llyship2020). Both support and advocacy vary in level of intervention (i.e., interpersonal to structural; Rappaport, Reference Rappaport1977), so researchers should assess the characteristics of the contexts in which they conduct research to determine how to effectively apply their interpersonal skills or structural power as allies against weight-based discrimination.

Supportive behaviors are usually individual-level actions that improve a specific person’s experience in a short-term timeframe. An important phenomenon that allies can attend to with support is responding to microaggressions, which have large impacts; contribute to discriminatory cultures; and produce negative emotional, physical, and formal effects (Major et al., Reference Major, Tomiyama, Hunger, Major, Dovidio and Link2018; Rosette et al., Reference Rosette, Akinola, Ma, Colella and King2018; Valian, Reference Valian1999). Supportive responses to microaggressions often include actively listening to and affirming coworkers in larger or smaller bodies who speak up about experiencing mistreatment. Allies providing support in these instances need to avoid excusing microaggressions, which by definition are “easy to excuse,” or often perceived as minor despite reinforcing harmful stereotypes (Williams, Reference Williams2019). When coworkers experience weight-based discrimination, a supportive allyship behavior would be to follow up with them to make sure they are okay; validate their experience; and give them an opportunity to advocate for themselves. These behaviors are incorporable to industrial and organizational psychology research design and practice. When things are not okay, supportive allies within organizations will lend a shoulder to cry on. Researchers may gather qualitative data from said employees corroborating weight-based discrimination in the workplace or describing coping mechanisms to buffer the associated negative impacts (Gerend et al., Reference Gerend, Patel, Ott, Wetzel, Sutin, Terracciano and Maner2021). In such contexts, researchers should be prepared to provide support and validation in the moment in addition to external resources as desired. Contextually, researchers can study peer groups (important for confronting weight-based discrimination among college students; Stevens, Reference Stevens2018), and workplace employee resource groups (Welbourne et al., Reference Welbourne, Rolf and Schlachter2015) to understand and bolster community support. Researchers can also study coworkers practicing effective coping mechanisms, such as seeking support from friends, coworkers, or family; changing perspectives on mistreatment to emphasize self-compassion; or engaging in other activities to get one’s mind off of difficult memories of mistreatment (Gerend et al., Reference Gerend, Patel, Ott, Wetzel, Sutin, Terracciano and Maner2021). Finally, conducting research that highlights experiences of discrimination, harassment, and ostracism can elicit negative affect among those conducting the research, particularly if the researchers are especially empathetic or are members of the targeted groups themselves (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Moran, French, Cheung, Hebl, Martinez, McGonagle and Rudolph2023). As such, researchers can lean on each other for support to grapple with the negativity they are helping illuminate.

Although many allyship behaviors manifest as support, other behaviors are better classified as advocacy, which entails behaviors that contribute to macro-level or long-term campaigns to promote inclusion, advance opportunities, and combat discrimination; that is, advocacy does not necessarily involve a specific, individual recipient of support (Evans & Wall, Reference Evans and Wall1991). Allyship advocacy behaviors present a tall order for weight-based discrimination researchers. Indeed, weight-based discrimination has been classified on the high end of susceptibility to mistreatment at macro-levels, according to the employment protections and stigmatization classification model (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Roberto, Black and Ahamad2021), which considers the high level of stigmatization (via morality beliefs and perceived controllability of weight), the increasing prevalence of anti-fat bias (Andreyeva et al., Reference Andreyeva, Puhl and Brownell2008), and the absence of nondiscrimination corporate policies and federal legislation. Thus, researchers should seek out local opportunities for advocacy, such as speaking up in one’s research for nondiscrimination policies on the basis of body size, campaigning for nondiscrimination legislation with empirically-based information, contributing to advocacy organizations addressing weight-stigma, or (perhaps most importantly) confronting weight-stigma when it occurs—on paper or in-person. For example, healthcare providers and healthcare personnel management researchers have a unique opportunity to advocate for policy to prevent the neglect of pain among patients in larger bodies (Azevedo et al., Reference Azevedo, Macaluso, Viola, Sani and Aglioti2014). In addition, the confronting prejudiced responses model (Ashburn-Nardo et al., Reference Ashburn-Nardo, Morris and Goodwin2008) can be readily adapted to weight-stigma for researchers to identify the cognitive hurdles that stand to be overcome through research encouraging engagement in confrontation behaviors. Specifically, researchers should consider improving bystanders’ abilities to (a) recognize that weight-based discrimination is occurring, (b) determine that the discrimination is harmful and warrants intervention, (c) decide that they are responsible for intervening, and (d) have confidence in their intended response to address weight discrimination successfully. As discussed previously, controllability myths and justification processes can contribute to these hurdles preventing confrontation behaviors, thus stifling allyship in the form of advocacy. We note that the efforts of the authors of the focal article (Lemmon et al., Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2023) to draw more attention to the influence of weight in organizational research represents an excellent form of advocacy in its own right.

Conclusion

It is our hope that bridging the weight-stigma literature and the contemporary allyship literature will provide actionable strategies for everyone interested in creating change, including research scholars and practitioners. Knowledge about weight-stigma (and the lack thereof) influences how allies develop. Thus, educating others about harmful stereotypes can help them become better allies against weight-stigma, and can increase the likelihood of engaging in effective support and advocacy behaviors for people in larger or smaller bodies.

Footnotes

1 We discuss weight-stigma toward people in smaller and larger bodies given we provide several examples that are relevant for people in both groups (e.g., disordered eating). Furthermore, in line with Lemmon et al. (Reference Lemmon, Jensen and Kuljanin2023)’s recommendations to be aware of our use of language, we use “person in larger body” or “person in smaller body” throughout. We recognize the oppressive histories of the terms “fat,” “obese,” and “overweight,” and based on our reflexivity, we are not in a marginalized position to reclaim such terms ourselves.

2 Recommended media for developing allies: a. (Bacon, Reference Bacon2010). Health at every size: The surprising truth about your weight. BenBella Books, Inc. b. (Taylor, Reference Taylor2021). The body is not an apology: The power of radical self-love. Berrett-Koehler Publishers. c. (Strings, Reference Taylor2019). Fearing the black body: The racial origins of fat phobia. New York University Press.

References

Andreyeva, T., Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2008). Changes in perceived weight discrimination among Americans, 1995-1996 through 2004-2006. Obesity, 16 (5), 11291134. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.35.Google Scholar
Ashburn-Nardo, L., Morris, K. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2008). The Confronting Prejudiced Responses (CPR) model: Applying CPR in organizations. Academy of Management Learning & Education, 7 (3), 332342. https://doi.org/10.5465/amle.2008.34251671.Google Scholar
Azevedo, R. T., Macaluso, E., Viola, V., Sani, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2014). Weighing the stigma of weight: An fMRI study of neural reactivity to the pain of obese individuals. Neuroimage, 91, 109119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.041 Google Scholar
Bacon, L. (2010). Health at every size: The surprising truth about your weight (Rev. and upd.). BenBella Books.Google Scholar
Bacon, L., & Aphramor, L. (2011). Weight science: Evaluating the evidence for a paradigm shift. Nutrition Journal, 10, 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-2891-10-9.Google Scholar
Bernard, L., Fox, S., Kulason, K., Phanphackdy, A., Kahle, X., Martinez, L. R., Praslova, L., & Smith, N. A. (2023). Not your “typical” research: Inclusion ethics in neurodiversity scholarship. Industrial & Organizational Psychology, 16(1), 5054. https://doi.org/10.1017/iop.2022.100.Google Scholar
Crandall, C. S., & Eshleman, A. (2003). A justification-suppression of the expression and experience of prejudice. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 414446. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.414.Google Scholar
Evans, N. J., & Wall, V. A. (1991). Beyond tolerance: Gays, lesbians, and bisexuals on campus. American College Personnel Association.Google Scholar
Facts & Factors (2023). Gobal weight loss and weight management market size/share worth usd 405.4 billion by 2030 at a 6.84% cagr: Growing obesity rate to propel market growth. GlobeNewswire News Room. https://www.globenewswire.com/en/news-release/2023/02/09/2604662/0/en/Latest-Global-Weight-Loss-and-Weight-Management-Market-Size-Share-Worth-USD-405-4-Billion-by-2030-at-a-6-84-CAGR-Growing-obesity-rate-to-propel-market-growth-Facts-Factors-Industry.html Google Scholar
Garcia, S. C., Moran, L. H., French, K. A., Cheung, H. K., Hebl, M. R., Martinez, L. R., McGonagle, A., & Rudolph, C. Conducting organizational psychology research among EEOC protected peoples. In: Expert panel presented at the 38th Annual Conference for the Society of Industrial & Organizational Psychology, Boston, MA, 2023.Google Scholar
Gerend, M. A., Patel, S., Ott, N., Wetzel, K., Sutin, A. R., Terracciano, A., & Maner, J. K. (2021). Coping with weight discrimination: Findings from a qualitative study. Stigma and Health, 6(4), 440449. https://doi.org/10.1037/sah0000335.Google Scholar
Gordon, A. (2023). You just need to lose weight”: And 19 other myths about fat people. Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Hunt, A. N., & Rhodes, T. (2018). Fat pedagogy and microaggressions: Experiences of professionals working in higher education settings. Fat Studies, 7(1), 2132. https://doi.org/10.1080/21604851.2017.1360671.Google Scholar
Johnson, A. F., Roberto, K. J., Black, S. L., & Ahamad, F. (2021). Neglected under the law: A typology of stigmatization and effective employment protections. Human Resource Management Review, 32(4), 100873. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2021.100873.Google Scholar
Lemmon, G., Jensen, J. M., & Kuljanin, G. (2023). Best practices for weight at work research. Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives in Science and Practice, 17(1), 85105. https://doi.org/10.1017/iop.2023.50.Google Scholar
Lewis, R. J., Cash, T. F., & Bubb-Lewis, C. (1997). Prejudice toward fat people: The development and validation of the antifat attitudes test. Obesity Research, 5(4), 297307. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1550-8528.1997.tb00555.x.Google Scholar
Lin, S., & Stutts, L. A. (2020). Impact of exposure to counterstereotypic causality of obesity on beliefs about weight controllability and obesity bias. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 25(6), 730741. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2019.1653484.Google Scholar
Major, B., Tomiyama, J., & Hunger, J. M. (2018). The negative and bidirectional effects of weight stigma on health. In Major, B., Dovidio, J. F., & Link, B. G. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Stigma, Discrimination, and Health (pp. 499520). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190243470.013.27.Google Scholar
Martinez, L., & Hebl, M. R. (2010). Additional agents of change in promoting lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgendered inclusiveness in organizations. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 3(1), 8285. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1754-9434.2009.01203.x.Google Scholar
Martinez, L., Sabat, I., Ruggs, E., Hamilton, K., Bergman, M., & Dray, K. (2023). Development-ally focused: A review and reconceptualization of ally identity development. Equality, Diversity and Inclusion: An International Journal. ahead-of-print. https://doi.org/10.1108/EDI-10-2022-0284.Google Scholar
NEDA 2022). Statistics & research on eating disorders. National Eating Disorders Association. Available at https://www.nationaleatingdisorders.org/statistics-research-eating-disorders Google Scholar
Nutter, S., Russell-Mayhew, S., & Saunders, J. F. (2021).Towards a sociocultural model of weight stigma. Eating and Weight Disorders – Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 26(3), 9991005. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-020-00931-6 Google Scholar
Puhl, R., Andreyeva, T., & Brownell, K. D. (2008). Perceptions of weight discrimination: Prevalence and comparison to race and gender discrimination in America. International Journal of Obesity, 32 (6), 9921000. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2008.22.Google Scholar
Puhl, R., & Suh, Y. (2015). Health consequences of weight stigma: Implications for obesity prevention and treatment. Current Obesity Reports, 4(2), 182190. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13679-015-0153-z.Google Scholar
Rappaport, J. (1977). Community psychology: Values, research, and action. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Google Scholar
Ravary, A., Baldwin, M. W., & Bartz, J. A. (2019). Shaping the body politic: Mass media fat-shaming affects implicit anti-fat attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45 (11), 15801589. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219838550.Google Scholar
Ready, E. J., Snoevink, M., Martinez, L. R., Hamilton, K., & Carsey, T. (2023). The predictive validity of two allyship scales. In Eggler, K. D., & Nittrouer (Chairs), C. (Eds.), Advancing allyship research and practice: Theory, methodology, and training. Symposium presented at the 38th annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Rosette, A. S., Akinola, M., & Ma, A. (2018). Subtle discrimination in the workplace: Individual-level factors and processes. In Colella, A. J., & King, E. B. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of workplace discrimination (pp. 724). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199363643.013.2.Google Scholar
Sabat, I. E., Martinez, L. R., & Wessel, J. L. (2013). Neo-activism: Engaging allies in modern workplace discrimination reduction. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 6(4), 480485. https://doi.org/10.1111/iops.12089.Google Scholar
Sniezek, T. (2021). Running while fat: How women runners experience and respond to size discrimination. Fat Studies, 10(1), 6477. https://doi.org/10.1080/21604851.2019.1671135.Google Scholar
Snoeyink, M. J., Martinez, L. R., Hamilton, K. M., & Smith, N. A. (2020, April). Support and advocacy as ally behaviors: The creation and validation of a scale. In Gardner, D. M., Ryan (Chairs), A. M., & llyship, A. (Eds.), What works and what doesn’t. Symposium presented at the 35th Annual Conference for the Society of Industrial & Organizational Psychology. Austin, TX.Google Scholar
Stevens, C. (2018). Fat on campus: Fat college students and hyper(in)visible stigma. Sociological Focus, 51(2), 130149. https://doi.org/10.1080/00380237.2017.1368839.Google Scholar
Täuber, S., Mulder, L. B., & Flint, S. W. (2018). The impact of workplace health promotion programs emphasizing individual responsibility on weight stigma and discrimination. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2206. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02206 Google Scholar
Taylor, S. R. (2021). The body is not an apology: The power of radical self-love. Berrett-Koehler Publishers.Google Scholar
Tylka, T. L., Annunziato, R. A., Burgard, D., Daníelsdóttir, S., Shuman, E., Davis, C., & Calogero, R. M. (2014). The weight-inclusive versus weight-normative approach to health: Evaluating the evidence for prioritizing well-being over weight loss. Journal of Obesity, 2014, 118. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/983495.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (1999). Why so slow?: The advancement of women. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Welbourne, T. M., Rolf, S., & Schlachter, S. (2015). Employee resource groups: An introduction, review and research agenda. Academy of Management Proceedings, 2015(1), 15661. https://doi.org/10.5465/ambpp.2015.15661abstract.Google Scholar
Williams, M. T. (2019). Psychology cannot afford to ignore the many harms caused by microaggressions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(1), 3843. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619893362.Google Scholar