Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T17:43:24.287Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Virtual reality evidence on the impact of physicians' open versus defensive communication on patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 December 2023

Lotte Daniels*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Business Economics, Hasselt University, 3500 Hasselt, Belgium
Wim Marneffe
Affiliation:
Faculty of Business Economics, Hasselt University, 3500 Hasselt, Belgium
Samantha Bielen
Affiliation:
Faculty of Business Economics, Hasselt University, 3500 Hasselt, Belgium
*
Corresponding author: Lotte Daniels; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Using virtual reality (VR) in an experimental setting, we analyse how communicating more openly about a medical incident influences patients' feelings and behavioural intentions. Using VR headsets, participants were immersed in an actual hospital room where they were told by a physician that a medical incident had occurred. In a given scenario, half of the participants were confronted by a physician who communicated openly about the medical incident, while the other half were confronted with the exact same scenario except that the physician employed a very defensive communication strategy. The employed technology allowed us to keep everything else in the environment constant. Participants exposed to open disclosure were significantly more likely to take further steps (such as contacting a lawyer to discuss options and filing a complaint against the hospital) and express more feelings of blame against the physician. At the same time, these participants rated the physician's communication skills and general impression more highly than those who were confronted with a defensive physician. Nevertheless, communicating openly about the medical incident does not affect trust in the physician and his competence, perceived incident severity and likelihood of changing physician and filing suit.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Each year, approximately 400,000 patients die worldwide from unintended harm caused by care, or so-called medical incidents (James, Reference James2013). When confronted with medical incidents, physicians' fear of complex (legal) unfolding procedures (Bielen et al., Reference Bielen, Grajzl and Marneffe2019, Reference Bielen, Grajzl and Marneffe2020) may drive them to a rather defensive or non-open communication towards patients and their relatives (i.e. hiding information, avoiding responsibility, showing no empathy and lacking openness for further questions) (Blendon et al., Reference Blendon, DesRoches, Brodie, Benson, Rosen, Schneider, Altman, Zapert, Herrmann and Steffenson2002; Gallagher et al., Reference Gallagher, Garbutt, Waterman, Flum, Larson, Waterman, Dunagan, Fraser and Levinson2006). However, multiple empirical studies suggest that open communication about incidents (being empathic, honest and sincere, taking responsibility, providing information about the incident, possible compensation and corrective actions) would lead to fewer lawsuits and better patient perceptions (Mazor et al., Reference Mazor, Reed, Yood, Fischer, Baril and Gurwitz2006; Nazione and Pace, Reference Nazione and Pace2015). Unfortunately, many studies suffer from endogeneity issues. Moreover, many studies focus on guidelines about, for example, expressiveness or eye contact, which are open to interpretation and are difficult to implement in practice. Studies also commonly address clear physician mistakes, while physicians' responsibility for the incident is mostly unclear during initial incident conversations.

In the present paper, we overcame these shortcomings by using virtual reality (VR) techniques in an experimental setting. We showed participants 360° videos of physicians saying that a medical incident occurred and manipulate the verbal (i.e. what is said in the conversation) aspects of physicians' communication (hereafter called treatment), rather than body language. We focus on cases in which responsibility for the incident is unclear in the open videos and attributed to the patient in the defensive videos. After each video, we ask the participants how they perceived the physician and the incident and how likely they were to engage in further steps, such as filing suit or changing physician.

The set-up allows us to hold everything except the treatment (body language, hospital room, clinical indications, etc.) constant. In other words, while one participant watches the conversations where the physicians communicate openly about the medical incidents, another participant is asked to watch the exact same consultations, with the same physicians, the same non-verbal actions, the same medical incidents, except that the physicians communicate defensively in one specific part of the conversations. By creating arguably perfect counterfactuals, we can answer the question of whether physicians' verbal communication about a medical incident impacts patients' perceptions and willingness to (legally) hold the physician accountable for the caused harm.

While the external validity of experiments is generally low, using VR technology mitigates the problem. We made use of 360° cameras to shoot hypothetical medical incident conversations with real physicians in a real hospital setting. These videos are implemented in VR headsets (Oculus Rift headsets) so that, by putting on the headsets, participants are ‘immersed’ in a consultation where they lie in a hospital bed and are told by the physician that an incident has occurred. In this manner, unlike in video- or paper-based experiments, patients are not distracted with sensorial stimuli in the room and can easily imagine an environment when being in the PC lab. Because the camera was positioned from the patients' perspective (in the hospital bed), the participants can look around in the hospital room as real patients would do, feeling that they are really ‘present’ in the hospital room and feeling part of the consultation. As studies have shown that perceived reality with VR is better than 2D videos (Slater and Wilbur, Reference Slater and Wilbur1997) and written scenarios (Van Gelder et al., Reference Van Gelder, De Vries, Demetriou, Van Sintemaartensdijk and Donker2019), the VR technology in an experimental setting adequately addresses the issue of internal validity, at the same time as mitigating the external validity concerns relative to the alternative approaches used in the literature (Blascovich et al., Reference Blascovich, Loomis, Beall, Swinth, Hoyt and Bailenson2002). Furthermore, research has indicated that using VR enhances the focus (Patterson et al., Reference Patterson, Darbani, Rezaei, Zacharias and Yazdizadeh2017) and decision-making of participants in choice experiments (Mokas et al., Reference Mokas, Lizin, Brijs, Witters and Malina2021). To the best of our knowledge, VR has never been used before in this research area.

Our results reveal that, in contrast to existing literature, open disclosure leads to higher intentions to contact a lawyer to discuss their options and complain to the hospital and to higher reported feelings of blame against the physician. Nevertheless, there are no differences in participants' intentions to file suit or change physician, the trust they have in the physician and his competence, and in the perceived incident severity. The participants' general impression of the physician and his communication skills is even better in the open scenarios than in the defensive counterparts.

The rest of the paper is organised as follows. Section 2 discusses the related literature. Section 3 describes in detail the experiment set-up, production and validation process of the VR videos and sample selection methods. Our empirical results are presented in sections 4 and 5 and discussed in section 6. Section 7 concludes.

2. Related literature

Using physicians' audiotapes and their malpractice history, Levinson et al. (Reference Levinson, Roter, John and Dull1997) and Ambady et al. (Reference Ambady, LaPlante, Nguyen, Rosenthal, Chaumeton and Levinson2002) showed that sued physicians communicate differently with patients than non-sued physicians, raising the question of whether physicians' communication style drives patients' intention to sue. Since then, several scholars have examined potential relationships. First, some researchers have exploited the implementation of open disclosure guidelines to perform before-and-after analyses. Open disclosure is defined as ‘a process in order to acknowledge and redress emotional, physical, and financial harm, express an ethic of continuing care for the patient, and restore trust after a medical incident’ (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Bismark and Mello2017). Kachalia et al. (Reference Kachalia, Kaufman, Boothman, Anderson, Welch, Saint and Rogers2010, Reference Kachalia, Sands, Niel, Dodson, Roche, Novack, Yitshak-Sade, Folcarelli, Benjamin, Woodward and Mello2018) found that open-disclosure programmes correlate negatively with the number of new claims.

Second, many researchers have organised focus groups or surveys to question physicians and/or patients about their perception on incident communication, describing the needs of patients, physicians and institutions in case of a medical incident (Gallagher et al., Reference Gallagher, Waterman, Ebers, Fraser and Levinson2003; Iedema et al., Reference Iedema, Allen, Britton, Piper, Baker, Grbich, Allan, Jones, Tuckett and Williams2011; Hannawa et al., Reference Hannawa, Beckman, Mazor, Paul and Ramsey2013).

A third strand of literature employed an experimental approach in which people are shown videos or scripts of hypothetical physician–patient dialogues after a medical incident and are asked about their thoughts, feelings and behavioural intentions. For example, Mazor et al. (Reference Mazor, Reed, Yood, Fischer, Baril and Gurwitz2006) used this method to show that open disclosure after a medical incident lowers liability risk. However, they did not present a sufficiently large sample size to ensure high power of the study results regarding four design variables (existence of a positive prior relationship, severity of clinical outcome, level of disclosure and an offer to waive costs). Similarly, Nazione and Pace (Reference Nazione and Pace2015) attempted to address the role of apology, empathy, corrective action and compensation in patients' intentions after error disclosure. That study found that apologies do not result in greater feelings of responsiveness against physicians and that empathy, in contrast to corrective action and compensation, leads to substantially less anger and negative behavioural intentions. However, the authors were not able to report successful manipulation checks and the sample size was relatively small to ensure clear causal effects for the multiple manipulations in the study design. The same sample issues are prevalent in the study of Wu et al. (Reference Wu, Huang, Stokes and Pronovost2009), who suggested that full apology and responsibility result in significantly higher feelings of trust and referral intentions, but found no differences in intention to sue. Similarly, Allan et al. (Reference Allan, McKillop, Dooley, Allan and Preece2015) showed that participants watching videos of a surgeon apologising for an adverse event focusing on patients' needs evaluated the apology as more sincere and as denoting greater regret than in case of a self-focused apology. In addition to the fact that this study solely focused on one aspect of open disclosure (i.e. apology formulation), the authors could not guarantee that duration differences confounded the results.

Apart from empirical research on the impact of verbal communication, scholars have focused on communication style (i.e. kindness, humour and eye contact). Using a randomised controlled trial, Hannawa (Reference Hannawa2011, Reference Hannawa2014) proved the importance of non-verbal involvement in communicating incidents. Using video-taped and paper-based transcripts of hypothetical consultations, Lester and Smith (Reference Lester and Smith1993) and Moore et al. (Reference Moore, Adler and Robertson2000), respectively, found that a negative communication style during treatment increases the probability of a malpractice lawsuit. However, the studies' scopes are limited to communication during medical treatments instead of during disclosing incidents.

The present paper contributes to the existing literature in three ways. First, we ensure high power of the study results by using an experimental design with only one manipulation (a generally open vs. defensive verbal communication). By keeping all other factors constant, we are able to isolate the effect of verbal from non-verbal aspects of incident communication. Second, we use VR techniques to improve the external validity of the experiment. Third, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to focus on scenarios regarding initial incident disclosure in which uncertainty exists in physicians' responsibility in the open versions and the patient is blamed for the incident in the defensive versions.

3. Design and methods

3.1 Scenarios

Each participant watched three VR videos regarding a follow-up consultation after a harmful medical incident; the videos featured the treating physician, the patient and a friend or family member in a hospital room. In each video, the physician explained that there was a medical incident and answered a question from the patient's visitor about the medical procedures that were necessary to resolve the harm the incident caused. To avoid participants having difficulty empathising with the patient in the videos, for example, regarding to voice or gender, the patient does not say anything during the conversation. Also, we did not use names, gender, age and other demographics for the patients in the scenarios and chose general cases (e.g. we did not choose any gynaecological cases because they are only applicable to women). The videos are related to anaesthetics (two videos) and general surgery (one video), as these are high-risk specialities and hence harmful medical incidents are common (Lawthers et al., Reference Lawthers, Localio, Laird, Lipsitz, Hebert and Brennan1992; Studdert et al., Reference Studdert, Mello, Sage, Desroches, Peugh, Zapert and Brennan2005; Jena et al., Reference Jena, Seabury, Lakdawalla and Chandra2011). We shot only the initial conversation after the incident, as Moore et al. (Reference Moore, Bismark and Mello2017) showed that this determines the further course of the unfolding of the incident. Based on initial discussions with physicians, we know that such conversations normally last between 5 and 15 min. Our videos are no longer than 5 min for practical reasons.

We manipulated the physician's message. We have one version of each case in which the physician communicates openly (observed by the treatment group) and another in which he communicates defensively (observed by the control group). We based our scripts and manipulations on a qualitative study of prior scripted studies in open disclosure/communication literature (Mazor et al., Reference Mazor, Simon, Yood, Martinson, Gunter, Reed and Gurwitz2004; Gallagher et al., Reference Gallagher, Studdert and Levinson2007; Levinson, Reference Levinson2009; Allan and McKillop, Reference Allan and McKillop2010; Iedema et al., Reference Iedema, Allen, Britton, Piper, Baker, Grbich, Allan, Jones, Tuckett and Williams2011; Hannawa, Reference Hannawa2012; Hannawa et al., Reference Hannawa, Beckman, Mazor, Paul and Ramsey2013; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Boyle, Wallace and Mazor2013; Mello et al., Reference Mello, Boothman, Mcdonald, Driver, Lembitz, Bouwmeester, Dunlap and Gallagher2014; Allan et al., Reference Allan, McKillop, Dooley, Allan and Preece2015; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Bismark and Mello2017), and the expertise of 33 field experts (hereafter, expert panel).Footnote 1 More specifically, we conducted interviews with the field experts to review existing literature and discuss the major and most common distinctions between open and defensive communication. Differences between open and defensive communication are defined in the following aspects of physicians' verbal communication: (1) responsibility (patient's responsibility vs. summary of objective potential causes of the incident), (2) empathy (little vs. more empathy), (3) information (limited vs. extensive information about what happened), (4) honesty/sincerity (self-assured vs. expression of uncertainty), (5) corrective action (no intention vs. intention for correction action mentioned), (6) communication skills (limited vs. elaborated), (7) compensation (no offer vs. offer for compensation mentioned) and (8) openness to further questions (no openness vs. openness to further questions showed). These elements are manipulated simultaneously, given that they are together considered as the most important differences between open and defensive communication. The aim of this paper is not to disentangle the effect of these different verbal aspects of communication, but rather to analyse the impact of a more open communication more generally (in terms of all eight communication characteristics) versus a more defensive communication. Table 1 summarises the manipulations for a scenario about an accidental dural puncture. Full transcripts of the videos can be requested from the author.

Table 1. Manipulations scenario, accidental dural puncture

Two important features of our design ensure that the scenarios with defensive communication are suitable counterfactuals for the ones demonstrating open disclosure. First, by design, everything unrelated to the physician's communication is held constant. That is, the physician, the patient's health status and care path, the adverse outcome, the environment, the friend/family member (and what he says and does) and everything else that is observed by the participant are exactly the same in the two versions of a given scenario. This is because we shot one version of each scenario, and subsequently only replaced the manipulated dialogue of the physician. Second, one might argue that physicians who engage in more defensive communication feature other characteristics in communication style (unrelated to what is said) that could explain possible differences in patient reactions. For example, more defensive physicians might be more likely to use a different tone of voice (e.g. less vocal animation and relaxation) or exhibit specific non-verbal behaviour (such as less smiling or less immediacy, with a forward lean and appropriate touch).Footnote 2 However, the aim of this paper is to analyse the impact of a message related to a medical incident that is characterised as non-defensive in substance (such as summarising objective potential causes of the incident, expressing openness for further questions and the intention for corrective action) and holding constant the aforementioned non-verbal behaviour. Therefore, the defensive and open communication videos of a specific case were roleplayed by the same physician, who was asked to act out the defensive and open parts of the conversations in order so that their position was the same in both versions of the videos. To mitigate further concerns of changing non-verbal behaviour, we followed Pingitore et al. (Reference Pingitore, Dugoni, Tindale and Spring1994) and (1) thoroughly trained the participating physicians to keep vocal intonation, body movements, gestures and posture the same in all conditions, (2) closely monitored and directed the participating physicians to make sure non-verbal behaviour was very similar in the two versions of the video, (3) shot each scene multiple times, allowing us to select and edit the shots that were most comparable with respect to non-verbal behaviour.

3.2 Production of the videos

A professional VR production firm was hired to shoot the scripted scenarios with high-quality sound and vision. The videos were shot with a static 360° camera. The technicians were not allowed in the hospital room; they followed the footage and the accompanying sound on iPads with headsets in the corridor. The camera perspective was the patients' point of view. To achieve this, the camera was positioned on top of a mannequin (i.e. where the head would be), lying in a hospital bed. Because the majority of all specialists in Belgium are male (Roberfroid et al., Reference Roberfroid, Stordeur, Camberlin, Van de Voorde, Vrijens and Leonard2008), we asked three male physicians to roleplay the physicians in the videos.Footnote 3 The patient's visitor was impersonated by a single male actor in the three scenarios. The videos were recorded in a real hospital room in Lanaken, Belgium.

3.3 Validation of the videos

To ensure that physicians' verbal behaviour was distinguishable in the open and defensive version of each scenario, we first asked 90 people to rate the verbal aspects of the written scenarios and then asked another 30 to rate the VR videos. We followed the recommendations of Van Vliet et al. (Reference van Vliet, Hillen, van der Wall, Plum and Bensing2013) to pilot-test both media (text and videos), as the medium may alter participants' perceptions. A double pilot test also allowed us to check whether preliminary changes after the first pilot round had been successful. After a validated translation,Footnote 4 survey items from the literature were used to check all manipulations on seven-point Likert scales. Specifically, we asked respondents about the extent to which they agree (1 = completely disagree to 7 = completely agree) with various statements about (1) feelings of blame and fault (12 items) (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Adler and Robertson2000; Coombs and Holladay, Reference Coombs and Holladay2002; Nazione and Pace, Reference Nazione and Pace2015), (2) empathy (five items) (Coke et al., Reference Coke, Batson and McDavis1978; Hannawa et al., Reference Hannawa, Shigemoto and Little2016), (3) information (six items) (Schoenfeld et al., Reference Schoenfeld, Mader, Houghton, Wenger, Probst, Schoenfeld, Lindenauer and Mazor2019), (4) honesty/sincerity (four items) (Brugel et al., Reference Brugel, Postma-Nilsenová and Tates2015), (5) communication skills (12 items) (Jonas et al., Reference Jonas, Etzel and Barzansky1992; Schulman et al., Reference Schulman, Jesse, Harless, Kerner, Sistrunk, Gersch, Phil, Dubé, Taleghani, Burke, Williams, Eisenberg and Escarce1999; Rollnick et al., Reference Rollnick, Mason and Butler2001; Gerbert et al., Reference Gerbert, Berg-Smith, Mancuso, Caspers, Danley, Herzig and Brand2003; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Huang, Stokes and Pronovost2009), (6) compensation (two items) (Nazione and Pace, Reference Nazione and Pace2015), (7) corrective action (two items) (Hannawa, Reference Hannawa2011; Nazione and Pace, Reference Nazione and Pace2015) and (8) openness for further questions (two items). All manipulations were perceived as intended at the 5 per cent significance level. That is, participants who saw the open disclosure videos placed less blame on the patient for the incident, experienced more empathy, more information, more honesty/sincerity, better communication skills of the physician, a belief in compensation, corrective actions and openness to further questions. Interestingly, and as hypothesised, perceived differences between the open and defensive conversations were more pronounced in the VR videos than in the written scenarios because of the immersiveness related to VR.

We also used seven-point Likert scales to assess the realism of (1) the physician, (2) the patient's visitor, (3) the conversation, (4) the medical consultation, (5) the medical incident, (6) the hospital room and (7) the length of the conversation (Willson and McNamara, Reference Willson and McNamara1982; Shapiro et al., Reference Shapiro, Boggs, Melamed and Graham-Pole1992; Aruguete and Roberts, Reference Aruguete and Roberts2000, Reference Aruguete and Roberts2002; Roberts and Aruguete, Reference Roberts and Aruguete2000; Bradley et al., Reference Bradley, Sparks and Nesdale2001; Strasser et al., Reference Strasser, Palmer, Willey, Shen, Shin, Sivesind, Beale and Bruera2005; Verheul et al., Reference Verheul, Sanders and Bensing2010; Hillen et al., Reference Hillen, van Vliet, de Haes and Smets2013; Schoenfeld et al., Reference Schoenfeld, Mader, Houghton, Wenger, Probst, Schoenfeld, Lindenauer and Mazor2019). Participants rated the realism of all elements, except for the patient's visitor, significantly higher than the scale centre of four (5 per cent significance level).Footnote 5

The same seven-point Likert scale was used to measure participants' ability to empathise with the patient (Green and Brock, Reference Green and Brock2000; Hillen et al., Reference Hillen, van Vliet, de Haes and Smets2013) and the understandability and clarity of the scripts and the videos. All of these items were found to be higher than the scale centre of four (5 per cent significance level). A content check with three open questions was also successful. Based on additional open-ended remarks on the written scenarios, we included more information about the recovery process and the reporting of the incident to the insurance company in the final open VR videos.

3.4 Sample and procedures

Randomly selected economics, medicine and physiotherapy students at Hasselt University participated in the final experiment. The participants were not aware of the goal of the study and did not need to have experience with the conditions in the scenarios. Several studies have indicated that answers of analogue patients are representative for those of real patients and that they are equally engaged in watching video vignettes (Van Vliet et al., Reference Van Vliet, Van Der Wall, Albada, Spreeuwenberg, Verheul and Bensing2012; Visser et al., Reference Visser, Tollenaar, Bosch, van Doornen, de Haes and Smets2016). Following the power analysis for multivariate regression analysis of Dupont and Plummer (Reference Dupont and Plummer1998) with a desired statistical power level of 90 per cent, the number of included variables and a probability level of 0.05, we aimed for a sample size of at least 120 participants. In order to obtain a sufficiently large sample, we spread an announcement via mail at Hasselt University, and participants were entitled to a €10 voucher or a movie ticket.

Each participant was randomly assigned to a sequence of three videos with the same communication style: open or defensive. That is, randomisation happened at the participant level. We follow Charness et al. (Reference Charness, Gneezy and Kuhn2012) in using a between-design above a within-design to ensure a high number of observations while avoiding confounds and spurious effects. We varied the order of the videos to avoid bias due to order effects. Oculus Rift headsets were used to watch the videos in a PC lab. There was no interaction between the participants during the experiment. After watching each video, participants were asked to fill out a short online questionnaire about their feelings and behavioural intentions. At the end of the experiment, questions were asked about socio-demographics and background. A webpage guided participants through the experiment. It took an average of 35 min for the participants to complete the experiment.

3.5 Key variables

Table 2 provides definitions of the key variables. As the table shows, we combine multiple items into five dummy outcomes. The Cronbach's alphas for the constructs are all at least 0.8. All the items were originally measured on seven-point Likert scales. We have five outcome variables. Our first outcome is the variable further steps, which is a dummy equal to one if the respondent gave a mean score higher than 4 on a seven-point Likert scale to how likely they are to take five behavioural intentions (i.e. intentions to file a lawsuit, to contact a lawyer to discuss options, to complain to the hospital, to discuss the situation with a general practitioner and to change physician) (1 = very unlikely, 7 = very likely) (Mazor et al., Reference Mazor, Simon, Yood, Martinson, Gunter, Reed and Gurwitz2004, Reference Mazor, Reed, Yood, Fischer, Baril and Gurwitz2006; Grégoire et al., Reference Grégoire, Tripp and Legoux2009; Nazione and Pace, Reference Nazione and Pace2015; Schoenfeld et al., Reference Schoenfeld, Mader, Houghton, Wenger, Probst, Schoenfeld, Lindenauer and Mazor2019). Second, we measured feelings of blame against the physician, following the study of Coombs and Holladay (Reference Coombs and Holladay2002). Third, we combined three constructs into the variable physician ratings, namely: (1) the general impression of how the physician handles the incident (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Huang, Stokes and Pronovost2009), (2) physician's competence (two items) (Saha and Beach, Reference Saha and Beach2011) and (3) trust in physician (seven items) (Anderson and Dedrick, Reference Anderson and Dedrick1990). Our fourth outcome is communication skills, constructed of physician's empathy (four items) and sincerity (two items) (Coke et al., Reference Coke, Batson and McDavis1978; Brugel et al., Reference Brugel, Postma-Nilsenová and Tates2015). Fifth, we used two items to assess perceived incident severity (Grégoire et al., Reference Grégoire, Tripp and Legoux2009; Joireman et al., Reference Joireman, Grégoire, Devezer and Tripp2013; Nazione and Pace, Reference Nazione and Pace2015). We also included questions to control for participants' socio demographics and legal and health background.

Table 2. Variable definitions

a How likely are you to contact a lawyer to file a lawsuit regarding your hospital visit? (1 = very unlikely, 7 = very likely).

b How likely are you to contact a lawyer to discuss your complaint and your options regarding your hospital visit? (1 = very unlikely, 7 = very likely).

c How likely are you to send a complaint to the hospital about your hospital visit (such as an email, letter or phone call to patient relations)? (1 = very unlikely, 7 = very likely).

d How likely are you to talk to your primary care physician about the care you received at your hospital visit? (1 = very unlikely, 7 = very likely).

e How likely are you to change physicians? (1 = very unlikely, 7 = very likely).

f To what extent do you agree with the following statement about the medical incident discussed in the video? (1 = completely disagree, 7 = completely agree).

  • The blame for the incident lies with the physician.

g To what extent would you agree with the following statements if you were the patient in the video? (1 = completely disagree, 7 = completely agree).

  • I would trust the physician so much that I would always try to follow his advice.

  • I would distrust the physician's opinion and would like a second one

  • I would feel the physician is not doing everything he could for my medical care

  • I doubt that the physician really cares about me as a person.

  • I would trust the physician to tell me if a mistake was made about my treatment.

h What is your general impression of how the physician handles the incident? (1 = very bad, 7 = very good).

i To what extent would you agree with the following statements if you were the patient in the video? (1 = completely disagree, 7 = completely agree).

  • I would let the physician treat me further.

  • The physician didn't seem very competent to me.

j To what extent do you agree with the following statements about the physician in the video? (1 = completely disagree, 7 = completely agree).

  • The physician is gentle.

  • The physician is warm.

  • The physician is concerned.

  • The physician is compassionate.

k To what extent do you agree with the following statements about the physician in the video? (1 = completely disagree, 7 = completely agree).

  • The physician seems to withhold information from me.

  • The physician seems to mean what he says.

l To what extent do you agree with the following statements about the medical incident discussed in the video? (1 = completely disagree, 7 = completely agree).

  • The incident was severe.

  • Much harm was done by this incident.

4. Descriptive statistics

One hundred and forty students participated in the experiment, generating a sample of 420 observations. Table 3 shows descriptive statistics of the outcomes and student characteristics. Most of the participants (63 per cent) were female and had relatively good general health (mean of 6.06 on a seven-point Likert scale). Almost half of the sample (46 per cent) followed a medical or physiotherapist education and almost one-third (31 per cent) had a parent who currently works or has worked in health or law (31 per cent). Consistent with existing research (Mazor et al., Reference Mazor, Simon, Yood, Martinson, Gunter, Reed and Gurwitz2004; Hannawa, Reference Hannawa2014), almost one-third of respondents (31 per cent) indicated that they have been confronted with a medical incident as a patient or as a friend/family member.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics

Thirty-seven per cent of the sample intends to take further steps after the conversations, which is our primary outcome. More than a quarter (27 per cent) of the respondents reported high feelings of blame against the physician, but good communication skills (27 per cent). Thirty-eight per cent of the sample rated the physician relatively high on his competence, trust and general impression (38 per cent) and experiences the incident as severe (37 per cent).

Because participants were randomly assigned, there should be no systematic differences between participants in the control and treatment group. Nevertheless, to check for balance, we performed a series of ordinary-least-squared (OLS) regressions, one per participant characteristic, with open disclosure as the explanatory variable. Table 4 indicates that these balance checks reveal no statistically significant differences between the treatment and control group.

Table 4. Results on tests of covariate balance

Notes: The table reports the results based on tests of covariate balance. Columns (1) and (2) display means for the control and the treatment group, respectively. Column (3) reports the coefficients from an OLS regression with open disclosure as the explanatory variable, with corresponding p-values shown in (parentheses).

Table 5 presents the descriptive statistics of the outcomes by communication style in the videos (defensive vs. open). For example, row (1) indicates that intentions to take further steps are 10 percentage points higher in case of open disclosure (0.42 vs. 0.32) and that the difference is statistically significant from zero. There are also significant greater feelings of blame against the physician for the treatment group in row (2) (0.35 vs. 0.20). Interestingly, there is no significant difference in physician ratings [row (3)] or perceived incident severity [row (5)]. The treatment group rates physician communication skills significantly higher (0.31 vs. 0.23), as can be seen in row (4).

Table 5. Descriptive statistics outcomes

Notes: Cells contain means and (standard errors). ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1.

5. Econometric analysis and results

5.1 Empirical strategy

To examine the impact of open disclosure on patients' feelings and behavioural intentions, we estimated the following equation.

(1)$$Y_{ic} = \alpha + \beta Open\;Disclosure_i + X_i\Theta + \varphi _c + \varepsilon _{ic}$$

where Y ic is one of the various indicators of the feelings and intentions (e.g. intentions to take further steps, feelings of blame, physician ratings) of participant i in case c (each participant encountered three videos). We controlled for a set of participant characteristics (vector X i) that would be crucial when using observational data since participants may embody attributes that confound the results. This should not be a concern in our research because, by design, physicians' communication type is orthogonal to participant characteristics. Nevertheless, we control for gender, health status, whether the participant is a medicine or physiotherapist student, whether the participant has a parent (has been) working in health or law and incident experience in order to reduce residual variance and improve the precision of the estimates. We also included dummies to control for the order in which the videos are watched to avoid bias resulting from order effects. φc are case fixed effects. The variable of interest, Open Disclosure i, is a dummy that equals one if participant i has seen the videos with open disclosure. Hence, β measures the impact of open disclosure on feelings and behavioural intentions of participant i, all else being equal. Standard errors are clustered at the participant level in order to control for potential correlations between unobservable characteristics within individuals.

5.2 Main results

Table 6 provides the results obtained from estimating equation (1). The results show that being open results in a statistically significant higher intention for patients to engage in further (legal) steps and greater feelings of blame against the physician. More specifically, participants exposed to open disclosure are more than 10 per cent more likely to pursue the physician for the harm suffered and 16 per cent more likely to blame the physician. At the same time, open disclosure does not lead to different physician ratings or different perceptions of the severity of the incident. Interestingly, physician communication skills are rated almost 8 per cent higher than the defensive counterpart.

Table 6. Main results

Notes: The table reports OLS results. Heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in (parentheses) are clustered at the participant level. In each regression, we controlled for case and order fixed effects. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1.

Results also show that men have significantly better perceptions of physicians' communication skills and participants with parents who have working experience in health or law express less feelings of blame against the physician.

5.3 Interacting effects of open disclosure

In this section, we examine the interaction of open disclosure with three participant characteristics: incident experience (previous experiences may create expectations and alter the treatment effect), medicine or physiotherapist students (who are educated about patient communication) and gender (women are especially sensitive to communication style). However, as Table 7 shows, none of these interaction effects are statistically significant.

Table 7. Results with interaction terms

Notes: The table reports OLS results. Heteroscedasticity robust standard errors (in parentheses) are clustered at the participant level. In each regression, we added controls and case and order fixed effects as in Table 6. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1.

5.4 Further results

In Table 8, we run separate OLS regressions for each dummy outcome instead of combined constructs. These results help us understand which behavioural intentions participants pursue following open disclosure, and which patients' feelings are influenced. Participants exposed to open disclosure are more than 15 per cent more likely to contact a lawyer to discuss their options regarding the medical incident and more than 13 per cent more inclined to complain to the hospital (panel A). Although the coefficients for other behavioural intentions such as filing a lawsuit, contacting general practitioner and changing physician are positive, they are not significant. Panel B indicates that open disclosure leads to a significantly better general impression of the physician, while no significant differences for the trust in physician, physician competence, sincerity and empathy are found.

Table 8. Results on separate dummy outcomes

Notes: The table reports OLS results. Heteroscedasticity robust standard errors (in parentheses) are clustered at the participant level. In each regression, we added controls and case and order fixed effects as in Table 6. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1.

5.5 Robustness checks

Table 9 shows the results of a series of robustness checks. Panel A shows the main results for the original Likert outcomes instead of dummy variables. These results are consistent with the results presented in Table 6; that is, a significant positive impact of open disclosure on intention to take further steps, feelings of blame against physician and physician communication skills, although there was no effect on physician ratings and incident severity.

Table 9. Robustness checks

Notes: The table reports OLS results (except for panel F, panel F shows average marginal effects of Probit regressions). Heteroscedasticity robust (delta-method in panel F) standard errors (in parentheses) are clustered at the participant level. In each regression, we added controls and case and order fixed effects as in Table 6. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1.

To check the robustness of the cut-offs we used for the main analyses, we performed OLS regressions on dummies with cut-offs of one point lower and higher on a seven-point Likert scale in panels B and C, respectively. Compared with Table 6, the results are consistent, apart from the feelings of blame against physicians in panel B. A potential explanation is that the feelings of blame are rated relatively high (mean of 4.65 on a seven-point Likert scale), and a lower cut-off leads to less variation for the dummy variable.

To investigate the sensitivity of our results to decision time (i.e. the total time the respondent spent on the experiment), we dropped the slowest 25 per cent and the fastest 25 per cent of respondents of the sample, in panels D and E, respectively. These results are comparable to those of the whole sample, except for the non-significant coefficient of open disclosure on intentions to take further steps.

We also repeated our main analysis with probit regression models in panel F. The results in this panel are completely consistent with those of Table 6.

6. Discussion and limitations

This aim of the present study was to analyse the impact of physician's open disclosure in case of a possible medical neglect on how patients feel and behave. Our first result is that when physicians openly disclose a medical incident, patients are significantly more engaged to take initiatives such as contacting a lawyer to discuss options and complaining to the hospital, although patients are not more intended to file suit directly.

This finding is contrary to previous studies, which have suggested that open disclosure correlates negatively with the number of claims. A possible explanation for this result is the way we manipulated physicians' communication about the responsibility for the incident differently than related studies in the field. Particularly, in the open videos, the physician enumerated possible objective causes for the incident, leaving open the discussion of whether the physician made a mistake or not. This contrasts to the defensive counterpart, in which the physician says that the patient's medical condition or behaviour caused the incident. In most studies, physicians' responsibility (and not patients' responsibility) is clear in both the open and defensive communication videos. As proving physicians' responsibility is a decisive factor for getting indemnified, we expect that this manipulation leads participants in the treatment group (where responsibility for the incident is unclear) to report significantly greater feelings of blame against the physicians and be more convinced that they are entitled to receive an indemnification and that further steps are worth the effort than participants in the control group (where responsibility for the incident is attributed to the patient's situation).

This finding raises intriguing questions regarding the specific elements of open disclosure driving patient behaviour. Examining these elements (e.g. showing responsibility, offering compensation, saying sorry) separately is an important issue for future research. In addition, more insights are needed into the impact of open disclosure in the further steps of the unfolding of a medical incident (i.e. not only focussing on the first step in the open disclosure process like in our research). Seeking answers to questions such as ‘could open disclosure eventually lead to more indemnity payments?’ would be important for assessing the general impact of open disclosure and finding solutions for the legal battlefield between physicians and various stakeholders involved, such as insurance companies.

At the same time, our results follow existing literature in that open disclosure leads to a better general impression of a physician and his communication skills. We also found that open disclosure does not impact patients' trust in a physician and his or her competence, the perceived incident severity and intentions to change physician. Showing physicians that physician ratings by patients are not affected by open disclosure might be an important first step in boosting open communication, given that physicians have mentioned potential reputational damage as a major issue in the unfolding of a medical incident (Vandersteegen et al., Reference Vandersteegen, Vandijck and Marneffe2015), and is therefore considered a main driver of physicians' defensive communication in practice.

There are several reasons why it is important to find explanations and resolutions for physicians' fear for malpractice procedures. First, besides defensive communication, physicians' medical liability risk may drive physicians to perform more tests and procedures than is strictly medically necessary, or to what is known in the literature as defensive medicine (OTA, 1994; Kessler and McClellan, Reference Kessler and McClellan1996; Klingman et al., Reference Klingman, Localio, Sugarman, Wagner, Polishuk, Wolfe and Corrigan1996; Garattini and Padula, Reference Garattini and Padula2020). Multiple studies have examined various drivers of physician's defensive medicine in an attempt to approach efficient spending of limited health care budgets (Bradford, Reference Bradford1995; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Sage and Becker2005; Roberts and Hoch, Reference Roberts and Hoch2007; Sloan and Shadle, Reference Sloan and Shadle2009; Shurtz, Reference Shurtz2013; Amaral-Garcia et al., Reference Amaral-Garcia, Bertoli and Grembi2015; Avraham and Schanzenbach, Reference Avraham and Schanzenbach2015; Osti and Steyrer, Reference Osti and Steyrer2017). However, more attention needs to be given to the interplay with incident disclosure, and how this may create a vicious cycle.

Second, defensive communication about medical incidents may enlarge the suffering of physicians as second victims, as being open towards colleagues, family, friends, patients and their relatives may be a strategy for physicians to restore trust and self-confidence (Hannawa et al., Reference Hannawa, Beckman, Mazor, Paul and Ramsey2013; Seys et al., Reference Seys, Scott, Wu, Van Gerven, Vleugels, Euwema, Panella, Conway, Sermeus and Vanhaecht2013; Ullström et al., Reference Ullström, Sachs, Hansson, Øvretveit and Brommels2014; Coughlan et al., Reference Coughlan, Powell and Higgins2017). At least as importantly, in the absence of open disclosure, there will be no learning opportunities for institutions and physicians to improve their processes and avoid future recurrences (Wu and Steckelberg, Reference Wu and Steckelberg2012; Seys et al., Reference Seys, Scott, Wu, Van Gerven, Vleugels, Euwema, Panella, Conway, Sermeus and Vanhaecht2013).

Third, medical incidents and their unfolding are associated with large financial and emotional costs (Carey and Stefos, Reference Carey and Stefos2011; Bielen et al., Reference Bielen, Grajzl and Marneffe2020, Reference Bielen, Grajzl and Marneffe2019), and increasing health care costs are a concern in developed countries (OECD, 2015). With the establishment of the Funds for Medical Incidents in 2012, Belgium introduced no-fault liability in its malpractice system to tackle compensation issues and lower the number of complex malpractice procedures. However, the system suffers from backlogs and a lack of experts, and thus currently lacks efficiency and effectivity in practice (FMO, 2020), which may have increased uncertainty among physicians about their true malpractice risk. Therefore, optimising malpractice laws and reducing related physician defensive behaviour should be a primary focus of policy makers.

The present study has certain limitations. First, the study only addresses intentions, instead of real patient behaviour. Therefore, additional research is necessary in order to examine whether intentions also match real behaviour for the research question discussed in this paper. Second, these findings cannot be extrapolated to all patients. After all, only students participated, and the health and law context of Belgium is quite different from other countries.

7. Conclusion

This study is the first to use VR techniques in combination with an experimental design to examine the impact of physician verbal open incident disclosure on patients' feelings and behavioural intentions. More specifically, we shot 360° videos of hypothetical physician–patient conversations after the occurrence of a medical incident. For each of the three cases, two versions are made: one where the physician openly communicates verbally about the incident and a defensive counterpart. It is important to note that everything else is kept constant, such as physician non-verbal behaviour and patient symptoms, ensuring that physicians' verbal communication is driving the effect. With our design and the use of VR techniques, we overcome the omitted variable bias and external validity problems that are common in existing literature in this domain.

One hundred and forty economics, medicine and physiotherapy students each randomly evaluated three videos with the same communication style (open vs. defensive). The results show that open disclosure results in higher intentions to take further (legal) steps, but does not directly damage a physician's reputation. We found that participants in the open videos are more likely to blame the physician, while we found no difference in physician ratings and the perceived incident severity between the groups. Participants' general impression of how the physician handles the incident and his communication skills are better than in the defensive counterpart.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the 33 experts for their input for the scenarios and the discussion of the study results and to multiple research assistants for help with data collection.

Financial support

None.

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standards

This study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of Hasselt University (REC/SMEC/VRAI/190/120) and therefore meets all the requirements.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Footnotes

1 We collaborated closely with ten physicians, three nurses, two health care managers, seven scholars, three lawyers, one patient, one member of the Flemish Patient Platform, one health insurance expert, three ombudsmen and two quality coordinators.

2 Hannawa (Reference Hannawa2014) showed that non-verbally uninvolved error disclosure affects perceived incident severity and patient understanding.

3 We did not include female physicians because the sample sizes would have been too small to disentangle possible gender effects.

4 Following the method of Cha et al. (Reference Cha, Kim and Erlen2007).

5 In general, scores were higher for the VR videos than for the written scenarios, except for the patient's visitor and the length of the vignettes.

References

Allan, A and McKillop, D (2010) The health implications of apologizing after an adverse event. International Journal for Quality in Health Care 22, 126131. https://doi.org/10.1093/intqhc/mzq001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allan, A, McKillop, D, Dooley, J, Allan, MM and Preece, DA (2015) Apologies following an adverse medical event: the importance of focusing on the consumer's needs. Patient Education and Counseling 98, 10581062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2015.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amaral-Garcia, S, Bertoli, P and Grembi, V (2015) Does experience rating improve obstetric practices? Evidence from Italy. Health Economics 24, 10501064. https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.3210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambady, N, LaPlante, D, Nguyen, T, Rosenthal, R, Chaumeton, N and Levinson, W (2002) Surgeons’ tone of voice: a clue to malpractice history. Surgery 132, 59. https://doi.org/10.1067/msy.2002.124733CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, LA and Dedrick, RF (1990) Development of the trust in physician scale: a measure to assess interpersonal trust in patient-physician relationships. Psychological Reports 67(3_suppl), 10911100.Google Scholar
Aruguete, MS and Roberts, CA (2000) Gender, affiliation, and control in physician–patient encounters. Sex Roles 42, 107118. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1007036412480CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aruguete, MS and Roberts, CA (2002) Participants’ ratings of male physicians who vary in race and communication style. Psychological Reports 91, 793806. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2002.91.3.793CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avraham, R and Schanzenbach, M (2015) The impact of tort reform on intensity of treatment: evidence from heart patients. Journal of Health Economics 39, 273288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2014.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bielen, S, Grajzl, P and Marneffe, W (2019) Investigating medical malpractice victim compensation: micro-level evidence from a professional liability insurer's files. The European Journal of Health Economics 20, 12491260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bielen, S, Grajzl, P and Marneffe, W (2020) The resolution process and the timing of settlement of medical malpractice claims. Health Economics, Policy and Law 15, 509529. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1744133119000185CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blascovich, J, Loomis, J, Beall, AC, Swinth, KR, Hoyt, CL and Bailenson, JN (2002) Immersive virtual environment technology: just another methodological tool for social psychology? Psychological Inquiry 13, 146149. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1302_03CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blendon, RJ, DesRoches, CM, Brodie, M, Benson, JM, Rosen, AB, Schneider, E, Altman, DE, Zapert, K, Herrmann, MJ and Steffenson, AE (2002) Views of practicing physicians and the public on medical errors. The New England Journal of Medicine 347, 19331940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradford, WD (1995) Solo versus group practice in the medical profession: the influence of malpractice risk. Health Economics 4, 95112. https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.4730040203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, G, Sparks, B and Nesdale, D (2001) Doctor communication style and patient outcomes: gender and age as moderators. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 31, 17491773. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb02749.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brugel, S, Postma-Nilsenová, M and Tates, K (2015) The link between perception of clinical empathy and nonverbal behavior: the effect of a doctor's gaze and body orientation. Patient Education and Counseling 98, 12601265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2015.08.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, K and Stefos, T (2011) Measuring the cost of hospital adverse patient safety events. Health Economics 20, 14171430. https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.1680CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cha, ES, Kim, KH and Erlen, JA (2007) Translation of scales in cross-cultural research: issues and techniques. Journal of Advanced Nursing 58, 386395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charness, G, Gneezy, U and Kuhn, MA (2012) Experimental methods: between-subject and within-subject design. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 81, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coke, JS, Batson, CD and McDavis, K (1978) Empathic mediation of helping: a two-stage model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36, 752766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coombs, WT and Holladay, SJ (2002) Helping crisis managers protect reputational assets: initial tests of the situational crisis communication theory. Management Communication Quarterly 16, 165186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coughlan, B, Powell, D and Higgins, M (2017) The second victim: a review. European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology 213, 1116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dupont, WD and Plummer, WD Jr (1998) Power and sample size calculations for studies involving linear regression. Controlled Clinical Trials 19, 589601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FMO (2020) Activiteitenverslag van het Fonds Voor de Medische Ongevallen – 2020. Belgium: RIZIV.Google Scholar
Gallagher, TH, Waterman, AD, Ebers, AG, Fraser, VJ and Levinson, W (2003) Patients’ and physicians’ attitudes regarding the disclosure of medical errors. JAMA 289, 10011007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, TH, Garbutt, JM, Waterman, AD, Flum, DR, Larson, EB, Waterman, BM, Dunagan, WC, Fraser, VJ and Levinson, W (2006) Choosing your words carefully: how physicians would disclose harmful medical errors to patients. JAMA Internal Medicine 166, 15851593. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.166.15.1585Google ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, TH, Studdert, D and Levinson, W (2007) Disclosing harmful medical errors to patients. The New England Journal of Medicine 356, 27132719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garattini, L and Padula, A (2020) Defensive medicine in Europe: a ‘full circle’? European Journal of Health Economics 21, 165170. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10198-019-01144-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerbert, B, Berg-Smith, S, Mancuso, M, Caspers, N, Danley, D, Herzig, K and Brand, R (2003) Video study of physician selection: preferences in the face of diversity. Journal of Family Practice 52, 552559.Google ScholarPubMed
Green, MC and Brock, TC (2000) The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79, 701721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grégoire, Y, Tripp, TM and Legoux, R (2009) When customer love turns into lasting hate: the effects of relationship strength and time on customer revenge and avoidance. Journal of Marketing 73, 1832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hannawa, AF (2011) Shedding light on the dark side of doctor–patient interactions: verbal and nonverbal messages physicians communicate during error disclosures. Patient Education and Counseling 84, 344351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2011.04.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannawa, AF (2012) ‘ Explicitly implicit’: examining the importance of physician nonverbal involvement during error disclosures. Swiss Medical Weekly 142, w13576.Google ScholarPubMed
Hannawa, AF (2014) Disclosing medical errors to patients: effects of nonverbal involvement. Patient Education and Counseling 94, 310313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2013.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannawa, AF, Beckman, H, Mazor, KM, Paul, N and Ramsey, JV (2013) Building bridges: future directions for medical error disclosure research. Patient Education and Counseling 92, 319327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2013.05.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannawa, AF, Shigemoto, Y and Little, TD (2016) Medical errors: disclosure styles, interpersonal forgiveness, and outcomes. Social Science & Medicine 156, 2938. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.03.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillen, MA, van Vliet, LM, de Haes, HC and Smets, EM (2013) Developing and administering scripted video vignettes for experimental research of patient-provider communication. Patient Education and Counseling 91, 295309. 10.1016/j.pec.2013.01.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iedema, R, Allen, S, Britton, K, Piper, D, Baker, A, Grbich, C, Allan, A, Jones, L, Tuckett, A and Williams, A (2011) Patients’ and family members’ views on how clinicians enact and how they should enact incident disclosure: the ‘100 patient stories’ qualitative study. BMJ 343, d4423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, JT (2013) A new, evidence-based estimate of patient harms associated with hospital care. Journal of Patient Safety 9, 122128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jena, AB, Seabury, SA, Lakdawalla, DN and Chandra, A (2011) Malpractice risk according to physician specialty. New England Journal of Medicine 365, 629636. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMsa1012370CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joireman, J, Grégoire, Y, Devezer, B and Tripp, TM (2013) When do customers offer firms a ‘second chance’ following a double deviation? The impact of inferred firm motives on customer revenge and reconciliation. Journal of Retailing 89, 315337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonas, HS, Etzel, SI and Barzansky, B (1992) Educational programs in us medical schools. JAMA 268, 10831090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kachalia, A, Kaufman, SR, Boothman, RC, Anderson, S, Welch, K, Saint, S and Rogers, MAM (2010) Liability claims and costs before and after implementation of a medical error disclosure program. Annals of Internal Medicine 153, 213222. https://doi.org/10.1059/0003-4819-153-4-201008170-00002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kachalia, A, Sands, K, Niel, MV, Dodson, S, Roche, S, Novack, V, Yitshak-Sade, M, Folcarelli, P, Benjamin, EM, Woodward, AC and Mello, MM (2018) Effects of a communication-and-resolution program on hospitals’ malpractice claims and costs. Health Affairs 37, 18361844. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2018.0720CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, D and McClellan, M (1996) Do doctors practice defensive medicine? The Quarterly Journal of Economics 111, 353390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, D, Sage, W and Becker, D (2005) Impact of malpractice reforms on the supply of physician services. JAMA 293, 26182625. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.293.21.2618CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klingman, D, Localio, AR, Sugarman, J, Wagner, JL, Polishuk, PT, Wolfe, L and Corrigan, JA (1996) Measuring defensive medicine using clinical scenario surveys. Journal of Health Politics, Policy and Law 21, 185220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawthers, AG, Localio, AR, Laird, NM, Lipsitz, S, Hebert, L and Brennan, TA (1992) Physicians’ perceptions of the risk of being sued. Journal of Health Politics Policy and Law 17, 463482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lester, GW and Smith, SG (1993) Listening and talking to patients: a remedy for malpractice suits? Western Journal of Medicine 158, 268272. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006254-199310000-00012Google ScholarPubMed
Levinson, W (2009) Disclosing medical errors to patients: a challenge for health care professionals and institutions. Patient Education and Counseling 76, 296299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, W, Roter, DL, John, P and Dull, VT (1997) Physician-patient communication relationship with malpractice claim. JAMA 277, 553559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazor, KM, Simon, SR, Yood, RA, Martinson, BC, Gunter, MJ, Reed, GW and Gurwitz, JH (2004) Health plan members’ views about disclosure of medical errors. Annals of Internal Medicine 140, 409418. https://doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-140-6-200403160-00006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazor, KM, Reed, GW, Yood, RA, Fischer, MA, Baril, J and Gurwitz, JH (2006) Disclosure of medical errors: what factors influence how patients respond? Journal of General Internal Medicine 21, 704710. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00465.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mello, BMM, Boothman, RC, Mcdonald, T, Driver, J, Lembitz, A, Bouwmeester, D, Dunlap, B and Gallagher, T (2014) Communication-and-resolution programs: the challenges and lessons learned from six early adopters. Health Affairs 33, 2029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mokas, I, Lizin, S, Brijs, T, Witters, N and Malina, R (2021) Can immersive virtual reality increase respondents’ certainty in discrete choice experiments? A comparison with traditional presentation formats. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 109, 102509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeem.2021.102509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, PJ, Adler, NE and Robertson, PA (2000) Medical malpractice: the effect of doctor-patient relations on medical patient perceptions and malpractice intentions. Western Journal of Medicine 173, 244250. https://doi.org/10.1136/ewjm.173.4.244CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, J, Bismark, M and Mello, MM (2017) Patients’ experiences with communication-and-resolution programs after medical injury. JAMA Internal Medicine 177, 15951603. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2017.4002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazione, S and Pace, K (2015) An experimental study of medical error explanations: do apology, empathy, corrective action, and compensation alter intentions and attitudes? Journal of Health Communication 20, 14221432. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2015.1018646CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OECD (2015) Fiscal Sustainability of Health Systems: Bridging Health and Finance Perspectives. Paris: OECD Publishing.Google Scholar
Osti, M and Steyrer, J (2017) A perspective on the health care expenditures for defensive medicine. The European Journal of Health Economics 18, 399404. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10198-016-0848-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OTA (1994) Defensive Medicine and Medical Malpractice. Washington, WA: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
Patterson, Z, Darbani, JM, Rezaei, A, Zacharias, J and Yazdizadeh, A (2017) Comparing text-only and virtual reality discrete choice experiments of neighbourhood choice. Landscape and Urban Planning 157, 6374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pingitore, R, Dugoni, BL, Tindale, RS and Spring, B (1994) Bias against overweight job applicants in a simulated employment interview. Journal of Applied Psychology 79, 909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberfroid, D, Stordeur, S, Camberlin, C, Van de Voorde, C, Vrijens, F and Leonard, C (2008) Physician Workforce Supply in Belgium: Current Situation and Challenges. Brussels: Belgian Health Care Knowledge Centre (KCE).Google Scholar
Roberts, CA and Aruguete, MS (2000) Task and socioemotional behaviors of physicians: a test of reciprocity and social interaction theories in analogue physician–patient encounters. Social Science & Medicine 50, 309315. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00245-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, B and Hoch, I (2007) Malpractice litigation and medical costs in Mississippi. Health Economics 16, 841859. https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.1195CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rollnick, S, Mason, P and Butler, C (2001) Health Behavior Change: A Guide for Practitioners. Edinburgh, UK: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Saha, S and Beach, MC (2011) The impact of patient-centered communication on patients’ decision making and evaluations of physicians: a randomized study using video vignettes. Patient Education and Counseling 84, 386392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2011.04.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoenfeld, EM, Mader, S, Houghton, C, Wenger, R, Probst, MA, Schoenfeld, DA, Lindenauer, PK and Mazor, KM (2019) The effect of shared decisionmaking on patients ’ likelihood of filing a complaint or lawsuit: a simulation study. Annals of Emergency Medicine 74, 126136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annemergmed.2018.11.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulman, KA, Jesse, A, Harless, W, Kerner, JF, Sistrunk, S, Gersch, BJ, Phil, D, Dubé, R, Taleghani, CK, Burke, JE, Williams, S, Eisenberg, JM and Escarce, JJ (1999) Misunderstandings about the effects of race and sex on physicians’ referrals for cardiac catheterization. New England Journal of Medicine 340, 618626. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2005-869823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seys, D, Scott, S, Wu, A, Van Gerven, E, Vleugels, A, Euwema, M, Panella, M, Conway, J, Sermeus, W and Vanhaecht, K (2013) Supporting involved health care professionals (second victims) following an adverse health event: a literature review. International Journal of Nursing Studies 50, 678687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, DE, Boggs, SR, Melamed, BG and Graham-Pole, J (1992) The effect of varied physician affect on recall, anxiety, and perceptions in women at risk for breast cancer: an analogue study. Health Psychology 11, 6166. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.11.1.61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shurtz, I (2013) The impact of medical errors on physician behavior: evidence from malpractice litigation. Journal of Health Economics 32, 331340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2012.11.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slater, M and Wilbur, S (1997) A framework for immersive virtual environments (five): speculations on the role of presence in virtual environments. Presence 6, 603616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloan, FA and Shadle, JH (2009) Is there empirical evidence for ‘defensive medicine’? A reassessment. Journal of Health Economics 28, 481491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhealeco.2008.12.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strasser, F, Palmer, JL, Willey, J, Shen, L, Shin, K, Sivesind, D, Beale, E and Bruera, E (2005) Impact of physician sitting versus standing during inpatient oncology consultations: patients’ preference and perception of compassion and duration. A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 29, 489497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Studdert, DM, Mello, MM, Sage, WM, Desroches, CM, Peugh, J, Zapert, K and Brennan, TA (2005) Defensive medicine among high-risk specialist physicians in a volatile malpractice environment. JAMA 293, 26092617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullström, S, Sachs, MA, Hansson, J, Øvretveit, J and Brommels, M (2014) Suffering in silence: a qualitative study of second victims of adverse events. BMJ Quality & Safety 23, 325331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Gelder, J-L, De Vries, RE, Demetriou, A, Van Sintemaartensdijk, I and Donker, T (2019) The virtual reality scenario method: moving from imagination to immersion in criminal decision-making research. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 56, 451480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Vliet, LM, Van Der Wall, E, Albada, A, Spreeuwenberg, PM, Verheul, W and Bensing, JM (2012) The validity of using analogue patients in practitioner–patient communication research: systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of General Internal Medicine 27, 15281543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Vliet, LM, Hillen, MA, van der Wall, E, Plum, N and Bensing, JM (2013) How to create and administer scripted video-vignettes in an experimental study on disclosure of a palliative breast cancer diagnosis. Patient Education and Counseling 91, 5664. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2012.10.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandersteegen, T, Vandijck, D and Marneffe, W (2015) Physician specialists’ perceptions of the medical malpractice system in Belgium. European Journal of Health Law 22, 481491. https://doi.org/10.1163/15718093-12341373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verheul, W, Sanders, A and Bensing, J (2010) The effects of physicians’ affect-oriented communication style and raising expectations on analogue patients’ anxiety, affect and expectancies. Patient Education and Counseling 80, 300306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2010.06.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visser, LN, Tollenaar, MS, Bosch, JA, van Doornen, LJ, de Haes, HC and Smets, EM (2016) Analogue patients’ self-reported engagement and psychophysiological arousal in a video-vignettes design: patients versus disease-naive individuals. Patient Education and Counseling 99, 17241732. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2016.04.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willson, P and McNamara, JR (1982) How perceptions of a simulated physician-patient interaction influence intended satisfaction and compliance. Social Science & Medicine 16, 16991704. https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(82)90095-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, AW and Steckelberg, RC (2012) Medical error, incident investigation and the second victim: doing better but feeling worse? BMJ 21, 267270.Google ScholarPubMed
Wu, AW, Huang, I-C, Stokes, S and Pronovost, PJ (2009) Disclosing medical errors to patients: it's not what you say, it's what they hear. Journal of General Internal Medicine 24, 10121017. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-009-1044-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, AW, Boyle, DJ, Wallace, G and Mazor, KM (2013) Disclosure of adverse events in the United States and Canada: an update, and a proposed framework for improvement. Journal of Public Health Research 2, 186193. https://doi.org/10.4081/jphr.2013.e32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Manipulations scenario, accidental dural puncture

Figure 1

Table 2. Variable definitions

Figure 2

Table 3. Descriptive statistics

Figure 3

Table 4. Results on tests of covariate balance

Figure 4

Table 5. Descriptive statistics outcomes

Figure 5

Table 6. Main results

Figure 6

Table 7. Results with interaction terms

Figure 7

Table 8. Results on separate dummy outcomes

Figure 8

Table 9. Robustness checks