Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T03:42:51.189Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

National characteristics associated with prevalence of depression and anxiety symptoms: a cross-sectional ecological study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 February 2022

Anthony F. Jorm*
Affiliation:
Melbourne School of Population and Global Health, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia
Roger T. Mulder
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand
*
Author for correspondence: Anthony F. Jorm, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Cross-national comparisons of the prevalence of mental disorders have relied on lay-administered interviews scored using complex diagnostic algorithms. However, this approach has led to some paradoxical findings, with more vulnerable countries showing lower prevalence, and its appropriateness for cross-national comparisons has been questioned. This study used an alternative method involving simple questions from social surveys to assess the prevalence of specific depression and anxiety symptoms, and investigated their association with national indicators of human development, quality of government, mental health resources, and mental health governance.

Methods

The study used data on the prevalence of three symptoms indicating depression or anxiety: sadness, worry, and unhappiness. These data were taken from the Gallup World Poll (142 countries) and the World Values Survey (77 countries). National characteristics examined covered indicators of human development (income, life span, education, gender equality), quality of government (human freedom, perceptions of corruption), mental health resources (per capita numbers of psychiatrists, mental health nurses, psychologists, and social workers), and mental health governance (whether there is a national mental health plan and a mental health law).

Results

All the human development and quality of government indicators, and some of the mental health resource indicators, were strongly associated with a lower prevalence of symptoms.

Conclusion

Populations of nations with higher human development, quality of government, and mental health resources have better mental health when measured by the prevalence of specific symptoms.

Type
Original Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Cross-national comparisons of the prevalence of mental disorders have the potential to increase understanding of the factors that determine a nation's mental health. The ability to carry out these studies received a considerable boost with the development of standardized psychiatric interviews in the 1980s that could be administered by lay interviewers and scored with diagnostic algorithms. The Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI), in particular, was specifically designed for cross-national studies and has underpinned much of the research in the area. However, concerns have been expressed about the validity of cross-national comparisons of prevalence using the CIDI. These concerns have been based on the possibility of measurement error due to national differences in stigma and somatization (Patten, Reference Patten2003), cultural differences in the degree to which symptoms impair functioning (e.g. in some cultures, people may be more prone to ‘soldier on’) (Mulder et al., Reference Mulder, Scott and Jorm2020), and a greater willingness by people from Western countries to endorse screening questions even when they do not have as severe symptoms (Scorza et al., Reference Scorza, Masyn, Salomon and Betancourt2018).

There has also been skepticism about the findings from these studies, given that low-income countries and those affected by greater trauma exposure have been found to have equal or lower prevalence of common mental disorders compared to other countries (Patten, Reference Patten2003; Jorm, Reference Jorm2006; Vermetten et al., Reference Vermetten, Stein and McFarlane2016). In particular, lower prevalence has been found in countries with a higher Vulnerability Index, which is a complex aggregation of 23 indicators, covering Susceptibility (public infrastructure, nutrition, poverty and dependences, economic capacity and income distribution), Coping (government and authorities, medical services, material insurance coverage) and Adaptation (education and research, gender equality, environmental status/ecosystem protection, health investment). Such observations have led to the concept of a ‘vulnerability paradox’, where greater vulnerability is associated with a higher risk of disorders for individuals within a country, but a lower risk at a national level between countries (Dückers et al., Reference Dückers, Alisic and Brewin2016, Reference Dückers, Reifels, De Beurs and Brewin2019; Dückers and Olff, Reference Dückers and Olff2017). This paradox has also been questioned as to its plausibility (Vermetten et al., Reference Vermetten, Stein and McFarlane2016; Mulder et al., Reference Mulder, Scott and Jorm2020).

We have proposed an alternative simpler method of making cross-national comparisons of mental health, which avoids the need for complex branching interviews involving questions about the timing, duration, and impact of symptoms and the use of diagnostic algorithms (Jorm, Reference Jorm2006; Mulder et al., Reference Mulder, Scott and Jorm2020). This method involves examining the prevalence of individual symptoms assessed in social surveys of representative samples. We have recently used this method to investigate cross-national differences in the prevalence of sadness, worry, and unhappiness (Jorm and Mulder, Reference Jorm and Mulder2021). The justification for using these symptoms is that they are frequently included in scales measuring depression, anxiety, or general mental health (see online Supplementary File 1, which lists nine scales which ask about sadness, 11 which ask about worry, and 12 which ask about unhappiness). Using this method, we found a higher prevalence of each of these symptoms in countries with greater vulnerability (as measured by the Vulnerability Index), lower Gross Domestic Product (GDP) per capita, and greater income inequality (as indicated by the Gini Index, which measures inequality on a 0–1 scale). These cross-national associations are consistent with those at the individual level and do not support the existence of a vulnerability paradox.

In the present paper, we aim to use this methodology to investigate other factors that might be associated with national mental health. The characteristics we examined covered human development, quality of government, mental health resources, and mental health governance indicators.

The human development indicators examined covered income, lifespan, education, and gender equality. These were chosen because within countries, the prevalence of mental disorders and symptoms is higher in people who are poor (Ridley et al., Reference Ridley, Rao, Schilbach and Patel2020), less educated (Chlapecka et al., Reference Chlapecka, Kagstrom and Cermakova2020), physically unwell (Moussavi et al., Reference Moussavi, Chatterji, Verdes, Tandon, Patel and Ustun2007), and experiencing gender inequality (Van de Velde et al., Reference Van de Velde, Huijts, Bracke and Bambra2013). We hypothesized that these factors would also be associated with a higher prevalence at the country level.

The quality of government indicators examined covered freedom and corruption. Freedom was chosen because a previous study on seven European countries and the USA found that a freedom index was associated with lower symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress, which the authors attributed to greater autonomy and opportunities to pursue happiness (Scholten et al., Reference Scholten, Velten, Neher and Margraf2017). Previous research on European countries has also found an association of perceived corruption with depressive symptoms, but that association was non-significant once national wealth was controlled (van Deurzen, Reference van Deurzen2017). Van Deurzen (Reference van Deurzen2017) has argued that corruption could affect mental health in various ways, including acting as a stressor, decreasing optimism and locus of control, and increasing powerlessness. We hypothesized that countries with greater freedom and less corruption would have a lower prevalence of symptoms.

The mental health resource indicators examined involved the per capita availability of mental health professionals, while the quality of mental health governance was assessed by whether a country had a stand-alone policy or plan for mental health and whether there was a stand-alone law for mental health. These resource and governance indicators were selected because they are regarded as key indicators of mental health system development by the World Health Organization (WHO) (World Health Organization, 2021). We hypothesized that countries with greater resources and quality of governance would have a lower prevalence of symptoms.

Methods

Data on prevalence of symptoms

Data on the prevalence of sadness and worry were taken from the 2019 Gallup Global Emotions Report (Gallup, 2019), which surveyed representative samples of adults aged 15 and older from 143 countries in 2018, with samples of around 1000 for most countries. The list of countries covered can be found in the datafile given in online Supplementary File 2. Telephone interviews were carried out in countries where telephone coverage represents at least 80% of the population or where this is the customary survey method, and face-to-face interviews were used in other countries. This survey asked questions about 10 positive emotions (in 143 countries) and 10 negative emotions (from 142 countries) experienced in the day before the survey. The two questions analyzed here as indicators of mental ill health are: ‘Did you experience the following feelings during a lot of the day yesterday? How about worry’ and ‘Did you experience the following feelings during a lot of the day yesterday? How about sadness?’. The participants responded ‘yes’ or ‘no’ to these questions. For the Gallup surveys, the translation process started with an English, French, or Spanish version, depending on the region. A translator who was proficient in the original and target languages translated the survey into the target language and then a second translator reviewed the translation against the original version and recommended any improvements.

Data on the prevalence of unhappiness were taken from Wave 7 of the World Values Survey, which covered surveys carried out in 2017–2020. Wave 7 is planned to involve 80 countries, but at the time the data were accessed, it was available in the World Values Survey online database for 77 countries (World Values Survey, 2020). The list of countries covered can be found in the datafile provided (online Supplementary File 2). To be included in the World Values Survey, a country's data had to involve a face-to-face survey of a representative sample of at least 1000 covering all residents of a country aged 18 years and over. The question on happiness (English version) was: ‘Taking all things together, would you say you are; 1. Very happy 2. Rather happy 3. Not very happy 4. Not at all happy’. The prevalence used here was the proportion of the sample responding ‘Not at all happy’. The World Values Survey questions were provided in major languages (English, Arabic, Spanish and Russian) by the World Values Survey Association. National survey teams were responsible for translations into other languages. In any country, the questionnaire was required to be translated into all languages that were the first language for at least 15% of the population.

Data on human development indicators

The human development indicators were taken from the three dimensions used by the 2019 Human Development Report to construct the Human Development Index (United Nations Development Programme, 2019). These are the Life Expectancy Index, the Education Index, and the Income Index, with each index having a range from 0 to 1, with higher scores reflecting greater human development. These indices were available for 139 of the 142 countries in the Gallup Global Emotions Report and for 74 of the 77 countries in the World Values Survey.

In addition, we used the Human Development Report's Gender Inequality Index, which is comprised of gender differences in health, empowerment and labor market (United Nations Development Programme, 2019). This index ranges from 0 to 1 with higher scores indicating greater inequality. This additional index was included because women typically have a higher prevalence of depression and anxiety symptoms, and this may arguably be influenced by gender inequality. This index was available for 133 of the 142 countries in the Gallup Global Emotions Report and for 71 of the 77 countries in the World Values Survey.

Data on quality of government

The quality of government was assessed by the Cato institute's 2019 Human Freedom Index and Transparency International's 2019 Corruption Perceptions Index. The Human Freedom Index is ‘a broad measure that encompasses personal, civil, and economic freedom’ and rates countries on a scale from 0 to 10, with high scores indicating greater freedom (Vàsquez and Porčnik, Reference Vàsquez and Porčnik2019). The Corruption Perception Index ‘aggregates data from a number of different sources that provide perceptions by business people and country experts of the level of corruption in the public sector’ (Transparency International, 2019). The Human Freedom Index was available for 135 countries and the Corruption Perceptions Index for 140 countries out of the 142 countries in the Gallup Global Emotions Report. Both indices were available for 74 of the 77 countries in the World Values Survey.

Data on mental health resources and governance

Mental health resource and governance indicators were taken from the World Health Organization's Global Health Observatory (World Health Organization, 2021) with data updated to April 2019. The resource indicators were number of psychiatrists, nurses, social workers, and psychologists working in the mental health sector per 100 000 population. The mental health governance indicators were whether or not there was a stand-alone policy or plan for mental health and whether or not there was a stand-alone law for mental health. These indicators were taken for the most recent year available for each country up to 2017. For the 142 countries in the Gallup Global Emotions Report, these indicators were available for 113 countries for psychiatrists, 92 for nurses, 71 for social workers, and 92 for psychologists. For the 77 countries in the World Values Survey, they were available for 61 countries for psychiatrists, 53 for nurses, 37 for social workers, and 52 for psychologists.

Statistical analysis

The data were analyzed using Comprehensive Meta-analysis V3 (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2013), with country as the unit of analysis. In using a meta-analysis approach, we are treating each country's survey of symptom prevalence as a separate study that can be combined with other country surveys in a meta-analysis, with countries/studies weighted according to the size of the survey sample. Meta-regression can then be used to investigate the association of country/study characteristics with symptom prevalence. Because the unit of analysis is the country/study, it is possible to use country/study characteristics as covariates in the meta-regression that are taken from data sources other than the symptom prevalence surveys themselves. Thus, in the current analysis, the symptom prevalence data are from countries/studies in the Gallup surveys and the World Values Survey, but the country/study characteristics are taken from other datasets such as the Human Development Report.

The prevalence proportion of unhappiness, sadness, or worry and the sample size were used to produce the logit of the prevalence proportion and standard error for each country. The random-effects model was used. Meta-regressions were run using the following as moderator variables: Life Expectancy Index, Education Index, Income Index, Gender Inequality Index, Human Development Index, Corruption Perceptions Index, number of psychiatrists, nurses, social workers, and psychologists working in the mental health sector per 100 000 population, and the presence of a national mental health policy or plan and the presence of a national mental health law. Scatterplots were used to visualize the associations, with each country survey represented by a circle, with the size of the circle proportionate to the weight of the survey. Associations were regarded as statistically significant if the two-sided p value was <0.05.

In order to give examples of countries with very high or very low mental health according to all three symptoms, a principal components analysis was carried out on the logit transformed prevalence proportions, with scores on the first principal component used as an overall indicator of mental health. This combined mental health variable was only available for the 73 countries that had data on all three symptoms. IBM SPSS Statistics 27 was used for this analysis (IBM Corp, 2020).

Results

The symptom prevalence data on individual countries are shown in the datafile provided as online Supplementary File 2. Descriptive statistics (mean, s.d., skew, and kurtosis) on all variables are given in online Supplementary File 3.

The random-effects meta-analysis found that the prevalence of the three symptoms was: unhappiness 1.5% (95% CI 1.2–1.8), sadness 24.6% (23.2–26.2), and worry 39.3% (95% CI 37.5–41.1).

In the principal components analysis, the first principal component accounted for 63% of the variance and had loadings of 0.89 for sadness, 0.87 for worry, and 0.57 for unhappiness. For countries with data on all three symptoms, the principal component score indicated that the following had the worst mental health (ranked by low score): Iran, Iraq, Nicaragua, Italy, Armenia, Bolivia, Portugal, Bangladesh, Peru, and Philippines. The 10 with the best mental health (ranked by high score) were: Taiwan, Kazakhstan, Poland, South Korea, Sweden, New Zealand, Kyrgyzstan, Czechia, Tajikistan, and Japan.

Table 1 shows the results of the meta-regressions predicting the prevalence of sadness from each of the covariates. All the human development and quality of government indicators were significantly associated with prevalence, as were two of the resources indicators (psychiatrists and nurses) and one of the mental health governance indicators (mental health law).

Table 1 Association of country characteristics with the prevalence of sadness

Figures S1–S3 (online Supplementary File 4) show some illustrative scatterplots. These show the strongest associations in each of the categories of human resources, quality of government and mental health resources, namely associations with Gender Inequality, Corruption Perceptions, and number of psychiatrists per 100 000.

Table 2 shows the results of the meta-regressions predicting the prevalence of worry. The same associations were statistically significant as for sadness. However, for all these variables, the magnitude of the associations was smaller than for sadness.

Table 2 Association of country characteristics with the prevalence of worry

Table 3 shows the results of the meta-regressions predicting the prevalence of unhappiness. Again, all of the human development and quality of government indicators were significantly associated with prevalence. However, the only resources indicator with a significant association was number of psychiatrists, while neither of the mental health governance indicators was associated. As with sadness, the magnitude of the associations was generally higher than for worry.

Table 3 Association of country characteristics with the prevalence of unhappiness

To see whether there were specific associations that accounted for most of the cross-country variation, a series of exploratory multiple meta-regression analyses was carried out. However, in general, once the most highly correlated covariate was entered into the regression, the remaining covariates accounted for no additional variance. An important reason was that there were high correlations among the human development, quality of government, and some of the mental health resources indicators, as shown in Table 4, resulting in multicollinearity in meta-regression analyses. To give an example, the three best individual predictors of the prevalence of sadness were Gender Inequality (R 2 analog = 0.38, p < 0.0001), Income (R 2 analog = 0.35, p < 0.0001), and Education (R 2 analog = 0.34, p < 0.0001). When all three of these predictors were entered simultaneously in a meta-regression, the overall prediction was the same as for Gender Inequality alone (R 2 analog = 0.38, p < 0.0001). The regression coefficient for Gender Inequality was still statistically significant in the simultaneous analysis, but with a higher p value (p = 0.0326), whereas the coefficients for Income and Education became non-significant. The reason for these reduced p values for the individual coefficients is that the three predictors were highly associated, with correlations ranging from 0.86 to 0.89, indicating that it was not possible to isolate the contribution of any one of these predictors independent of the others.

Table 4 Correlations among covariates in the Gallup Survey countries: Pearson correlation (N)

Because symptom prevalence may be affected by the age structure of a population, meta-regressions were also carried out using the percentage of population aged 15–64 and 65+ years (with 0–14 as the reference category) as covariates. However, these variables were found to be strongly related to the Life Expectancy Index, with a multiple R of 0.89, implying that the age structure is essentially redundant after the inclusion of Life Expectancy.

Discussion

As hypothesized, the prevalence of all three symptoms was associated with the indicators of a country's human development. Specifically, there was better mental health in countries with a high level of income and education, long life expectancy, and high gender equality. These cross-country associations are entirely consistent with findings for individuals within countries. There is no evidence of a ‘vulnerability paradox’ in these symptom data. The strength of these associations is large and highly significant, particularly with sadness, accounting for over a third of the variance between countries.

Also as hypothesized, the prevalence of all three symptoms was related to the indicators of quality of government, being lower in countries with more freedom and less corruption. As with the human development indicators, the associations are large and highly statistically significant. For the symptom of sadness, they accounted for over a third of the between-country variance.

The mental health resources indicators showed a more complex pattern of associations that was not always as hypothesized. Number of psychiatrists was consistently associated with a lower prevalence of all three symptoms, whereas number of nurses was associated only with a lower prevalence of sadness and worry, and number of social workers and psychologists was not associated with any symptoms. However, there were considerable missing country data on these professional resources, which reduced the statistical power to detect associations. The data on psychiatrists may be more reliable than for the other professions, as most countries will have medical registers. It is noteworthy that while the number of psychiatrists per 100 000 was a highly significant individual predictor, it was also highly correlated with the other predictors. For example, once the Income Index was controlled, there was no association with the number of psychiatrists, indicating that wealthier countries have better mental health and spend more on psychiatrists.

The mental health governance indicators had the weakest associations, with the existence of a mental health law being significantly associated with a lower prevalence of sadness and worry, but not with unhappiness. Contrary to hypothesis, the existence of a mental health policy or plan was not associated with the prevalence of any of the symptoms.

The weaker associations between mental health resources and symptom prevalence are consistent with concerns that increasing provision of treatment may not be associated with improved mental health (Jorm et al., Reference Jorm, Patten, Brugha and Mojtabai2017). Data from a range of high-income countries show that large increases in pharmacological and psychological treatment have not led to detectable reductions in the prevalence of psychological distress or the suicide rate (Mojtabai and Jorm, Reference Mojtabai and Jorm2015; Mulder et al., Reference Mulder, Rucklidge and Wilkinson2017; Jorm, Reference Jorm2018; Nishi et al., Reference Nishi, Susukida, Usuda, Mojtabai and Yamanouchi2018; Jorm and Kitchener, Reference Jorm and Kitchener2021). It is possible that identifying and addressing social determinants such as the ones we have reported may be more productive than simply increasing mental health treatment resources.

Taken together, these indicators may be associated with the national prevalence of symptoms because they involve stressors that increase distress in individuals, factors that give individuals greater control of their lives, or the resources available to treat mental health problems when these develop. The indicators may be strongly inter-related because improvements in any indicator are conditional on improvements in others, so that they cannot develop in isolation from each other. These indicators may reflect a general latent variable of country development that is associated with a lower prevalence of symptoms. We hypothesize that this general latent variable is a key determinant of a nation's mental health, rather than being a vulnerability factor as implied by the vulnerability paradox. In proposing this causal hypothesis, we acknowledge that the data are correlational, but it seems more plausible that this general latent variable is a driver of national mental health symptoms rather than the reverse. In hypothesizing this causal role, we also acknowledge that there may be other factors that influence a nation's mental health including war, social conflict, natural disasters, parenting practices, the availability of substances, and nutrition.

Previous research on the prevalence of depression and anxiety disorders using the CIDI has found a higher prevalence of Generalized Anxiety Disorder in countries that are wealthier (Ruscio et al., Reference Ruscio, Hallion, Lim, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Andrade, Borges, Bromet, Bunting, de Almeida, Demyttenaere, Florescu, de Girolamo, Gureje, Haro, He, Hinkov, Hu, de Jonge, Karam, Lee, Lepine, Levinson, Mneimneh, Navarro-Mateu, Posada-Villa, Slade, Stein, Torres, Uda, Wojtyniak, Kessler, Chatterji and Scott2017) and that economic development is not an important correlate of depression prevalence (Rai et al., Reference Rai, Zitko, Jones, Lynch and Araya2013). Such findings have led to the proposal of a ‘vulnerability paradox’, which posits that vulnerability is a risk factor for individuals within countries, but is associated with lower risk across countries (Dückers et al., Reference Dückers, Alisic and Brewin2016). By contrast, the present findings, which are based on individual symptoms rather than disorders, align the country data with the individual data, showing that the pattern of risk is the same within and between countries. To reconcile these apparently discrepant findings may require simultaneous investigation of cross-national differences at both the disorder and symptom level.

This study has a number of limitations which must be acknowledged. This is an ecological study and the associations found between countries may not apply within countries. Many of the indices examined involve composites of multiple indicators and it is possible that the associations might differ if more specific indicators were examined as predictors. The emotional states examined here may not be expressed equivalently in various languages, with differences in connotation possibly affecting prevalence. The social acceptability of reporting these emotions in surveys may also differ between cultures. Such effects will add ‘noise’ that reduces the magnitude of the associations examined here. We have attempted to reduce the impact of these factors by looking for consistent patterns of association with all three symptoms and across two international social surveys. Another limitation is that the mental health indicators examined cover only a small number of internalizing symptoms and the findings may not be generalizable to other dimensions of mental health, such as externalizing problems and psychotic disorders, or to the general p-factor of psychopathology (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel, Meier, Ramraka, Shalev, Poulton and Moffitt2014). A final limitation is the unknown quality of the data on mental health resource and governance, which is based on reports from individual countries and involved considerable missing data.

Conclusion

Although it has been suggested that mental disorders are ‘disorders of development’ (Sachs, Reference Sachs, Helliwell, Layard and Sachs2012), using the prevalence of individual symptoms as an indicator of a nation's mental health, it appears that poorer mental health is associated with low development. To reduce symptom prevalence globally requires greater investment in human development and better quality of government, as well as adequate resourcing of mental health services.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2022.9

Financial support

Anthony F. Jorm was supported by National Health and Medical Research Council Investigator Grant 11172889.

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

As this was a secondary analysis of published data, ethical review was not required. The World Values Survey and the Gallup World Poll are voluntary social surveys.

References

Borenstein, M, Hedges, L, Higgins, J and Rothstein, H (2013) Comprehensive Meta-Analysis, Version 3. Englewood, NJ: Biostat.Google Scholar
Caspi, A, Houts, RM, Belsky, DW, Goldman-Mellor, SJ, Harrington, HL, Israel, S, Meier, MH, Ramraka, S, Shalev, I, Poulton, R and Moffitt, TE (2014) The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science 2, 119137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chlapecka, A, Kagstrom, A and Cermakova, P (2020) Educational attainment inequalities in depressive symptoms in more than 100,000 individuals in Europe. European Psychiatry 63, e97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dückers, MLA and Olff, M (2017) Does the vulnerability paradox in PTSD apply to women and men? An exploratory study. Journal of Traumatic Stress 30, 200204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dückers, ML, Alisic, E and Brewin, CR (2016) A vulnerability paradox in the cross-national prevalence of post-traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 209, 300305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dückers, MLA, Reifels, L, De Beurs, DP and Brewin, CR (2019) The vulnerability paradox in global mental health and its applicability to suicide. British Journal of Psychiatry 215, 588593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallup (2019) Gallup Global Emotions. Washington DC: Gallup.Google Scholar
IBM Corp (2020) IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.Google Scholar
Jorm, AF (2006) National surveys of mental disorders: are they researching scientific facts or constructing useful myths? Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 40, 830834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, AF (2018) Australia's ‘Better Access’ scheme: has it had an impact on population mental health? Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 52, 10571062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, AF and Kitchener, BA (2021) Increases in youth mental health services in Australia: have they had an impact on youth population mental health? Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 55, 476484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, AF and Mulder, RT (2021) Cross-national differences in the prevalence of mental illness symptoms: evidence against the vulnerability paradox. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 55, 620626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, AF, Patten, SB, Brugha, TS and Mojtabai, R (2017) Has increased provision of treatment reduced the prevalence of common mental disorders? Review of the evidence from four countries. World Psychiatry 16, 9099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mojtabai, R and Jorm, AF (2015) Trends in psychological distress, depressive episodes and mental health treatment-seeking in the United States: 2001–2012. Journal of Affective Disorders 174, 556561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moussavi, S, Chatterji, S, Verdes, E, Tandon, A, Patel, V and Ustun, B (2007) Depression, chronic diseases, and decrements in health: results from the World Health Surveys. Lancet (London, England) 370, 851858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulder, R, Rucklidge, J and Wilkinson, S (2017) Why has increased provision of psychiatric treatment not reduced the prevalence of mental disorder? Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 51, 11761177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulder, RT, Scott, KM and Jorm, AF (2020) Making sense of the vulnerability paradox in cross-national studies of mental disorders: lessons from research on subjective well-being. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 54, 664666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishi, D, Susukida, R, Usuda, K, Mojtabai, R and Yamanouchi, Y (2018) Trends in the prevalence of psychological distress and the use of mental health services from 2007 to 2016 in Japan. Journal of Affective Disorders 239, 208213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patten, SB (2003) International differences in major depression prevalence: what do they mean? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 56, 711716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rai, D, Zitko, P, Jones, K, Lynch, J and Araya, R (2013) Country- and individual-level socioeconomic determinants of depression: multilevel cross-national comparison. British Journal of Psychiatry 202, 195203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridley, M, Rao, G, Schilbach, F and Patel, V (2020) Poverty, depression, and anxiety: causal evidence and mechanisms. Science (New York, N.Y.) 370, eaay0214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruscio, AM, Hallion, LS, Lim, CCW, Aguilar-Gaxiola, S, Al-Hamzawi, A, Alonso, J, Andrade, LH, Borges, G, Bromet, EJ, Bunting, B, de Almeida, JMC, Demyttenaere, K, Florescu, S, de Girolamo, G, Gureje, O, Haro, JM, He, Y, Hinkov, H, Hu, C, de Jonge, P, Karam, EG, Lee, S, Lepine, JP, Levinson, D, Mneimneh, Z, Navarro-Mateu, F, Posada-Villa, J, Slade, T, Stein, DJ, Torres, Y, Uda, H, Wojtyniak, B, Kessler, RC, Chatterji, S and Scott, KM (2017) Cross-sectional comparison of the epidemiology of DSM-5 generalized anxiety disorder across the globe. JAMA Psychiatry 74, 465475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachs, J (2012) Introduction. In Helliwell, JL, Layard, R and Sachs, J (eds), World Happiness Report. New York: The Earth Institute Columbia University, pp. 310.Google Scholar
Scholten, S, Velten, J, Neher, T and Margraf, J (2017) Wealth, justice and freedom: objective and subjective measures predicting poor mental health in a study across eight countries. SSM Population Health 3, 639648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scorza, P, Masyn, K, Salomon, JA and Betancourt, TS (2018) The impact of measurement differences on cross-country depression prevalence estimates: a latent transition analysis. PLoS ONE 13, e0198429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Transparency International (2019) Corruption Perceptions Index. Available at https://www.transparency.org/en/cpi/2019/index/nzl (Accessed 11 January 2021).Google Scholar
United Nations Development Programme (2019) Human Development Report 2019. New York: United Nations Development Programme.Google Scholar
van Deurzen, I (2017) And justice for all: examining corruption as a contextual source of mental illness. Social Science & Medicine 173, 2634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van de Velde, S, Huijts, T, Bracke, P and Bambra, C (2013) Macro-level gender equality and depression in men and women in Europe. Sociology of Health & Illness 35, 682698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vàsquez, I and Porčnik, T (2019) The Human Freedom Index 2019. Washington DC: Cato Institute.Google Scholar
Vermetten, E, Stein, DJ and McFarlane, AC (2016) Is there a vulnerability paradox in PTSD? Pitfalls in cross-national comparisons of epidemiological data. British Journal of Psychiatry 209, 527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (2021) Global Health Observatory. Available at https://www.who.int/data/gho (Accessed 14 October 2020).Google Scholar
World Values Survey (2020) Online Data Analysis. Available at http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSOnline.jsp (Accessed 3 September 2020).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Association of country characteristics with the prevalence of sadness

Figure 1

Table 2 Association of country characteristics with the prevalence of worry

Figure 2

Table 3 Association of country characteristics with the prevalence of unhappiness

Figure 3

Table 4 Correlations among covariates in the Gallup Survey countries: Pearson correlation (N)

Supplementary material: File

Jorm and Mulder supplementary material

Jorm and Mulder supplementary material 1

Download Jorm and Mulder supplementary material(File)
File 30.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Jorm and Mulder supplementary material

Jorm and Mulder supplementary material 2

Download Jorm and Mulder supplementary material(File)
File 40.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Jorm and Mulder supplementary material

Jorm and Mulder supplementary material 3

Download Jorm and Mulder supplementary material(File)
File 22.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Jorm and Mulder supplementary material

Figures S1-S3

Download Jorm and Mulder supplementary material(File)
File 45.4 KB