Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T09:23:51.328Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Signatures of geography, climate and foliage on given names of baby girls

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2022

Raymond B. Huey*
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
Donald B. Miles
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, Ohio University, Athens, Ohio, USA
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Parents often weigh social, familial and cultural considerations when choosing their baby's name, but the name they choose could potentially be influenced by their physical or biotic environments. Here we examine whether the popularity of month and season names of girls covary geographically with environmental variables. In the continental USA, April, May and June (Autumn, Summer) are the most common month (season) names: April predominates in southern states (early springs), whereas June predominates in northern states (later springs). Whether April's popularity has increased with recent climate warming is ambiguous. Autumn is most popular in northern states, where autumn foliage is notably colourful, and in eastern states having high coverage of deciduous foliage. On a continental scale, Autumn was most popular in English-speaking countries with intense colouration of autumn foliage. These analyses are descriptive but indicate that climate and vegetation sometimes influence parental choice of their baby's name.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: Geographic patterns of popularity of month and of season names suggest that physical and biotic environments can influence choice of baby name.

Introduction

Babies begin life with a given name chosen by their parents. That choice can be influenced by the parents’ own social experiences, family relationships and cultural sensitivity (Schlesinger, Reference Schlesinger1941; Lieberson & Bell, Reference Lieberson and Bell1992; Fryer & Levitt, Reference Fryer and Levitt2004; Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Dawson and Campbell2016) or even by a name's sound symbolism (Berger et al., Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012; Pitcher et al., Reference Pitcher, Mesoudi and McElligott2013; Suire et al., Reference Suire, Mesa, Raymond and Barkat-Defradas2019). The chosen name – whatever prompted its choice – can have life-long effects on the offspring's self-identity (Allport, Reference Allport1937) and achievements (Moss-Racusin et al., Reference Moss-Racusin, Dovidio, Brescoll, Graham and Handelsman2012; Goldstein & Stecklov, Reference Goldstein and Stecklov2016), as well as on its exposure to stereotyping and prejudice (Bertrand & Mullainathan, Reference Bertrand and Mullainathan2004; Moss-Racusin et al., Reference Moss-Racusin, Dovidio, Brescoll, Graham and Handelsman2012; Okonofua & Eberhardt, Reference Okonofua and Eberhardt2015).

Some names are perennially and broadly popular (Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022), but others show striking boom and bust patterns of popularity (Lieberson & Bell, Reference Lieberson and Bell1992; Berger & Le Mens, Reference Berger and Le Mens2009; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Rosenberg and Greenbaum2022; Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022) (Figure S1). Still other names vary geographically in popularity, suggesting that regional cultural patterns can influence name choice (Varnum & Kitayama, Reference Varnum and Kitayama2011; Bentley & Ormerod, Reference Bentley and Ormerod2012; Barucca et al., Reference Barucca, Rocchi, Marinara, Parisi and Ricci-Tersenghi2015; Pomorski et al., Reference Pomorski, Krawczyk, Kułakowski, Kwapień and Ausloos2016; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Rosenberg and Greenbaum2022).

Climate is a composite environmental factor that has diverse effects on human systems and culture (van de Vliert, Reference van de Vliert2013; Carleton & Hsiang, Reference Carleton and Hsiang2016) and might influence name choice, either directly or via its influence on the local biota and its phenology. This climatic influence has, however, rarely been examined (Berger et al., Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012; He et al., Reference He, Chen and Ren2021). Here we evaluate whether climatic and biotic environments influence name choice on geographic scales. We focus on calendar names (of months and seasons), which link directly to environmental seasonality. April, May and June are by far the most common month names for baby girls (Figure 1a). These months are associated with spring-like weather and vegetive renewal in the Northern Hemisphere and symbolise new life, youth and bounty, as in traditional English novels and poems (Supplementary Materials 1). If popularity of these names is linked to climate, then the relative frequency of each month name should shift geographically in concert with the timing of spring weather. By extension, the relative frequency of season names (e.g. Autumn) should shift geographically with seasonality of climate and especially with the intensity of colouration of deciduous foliage in autumn.

Figure 1. Numbers of babies with month or season names in the USA (1910–2021). (a) Total numbers of girls and boys with each month name. April, May and June are most common for girls, whereas August is the only common name for boys. (b) Season names are much more common for girls than boys, and Autumn and Summer are the most common season names for girls.

We focused on the relative frequencies of month and of season names (by state) of baby girls in the USA, because boys are rarely given calendar names (Figure 1). We proposed two hypotheses before inspecting or compiling data. First, because spring-like weather arrives earlier in southern than in northern states (Figure 2a), April should be relatively more common in the south, whereas May or June should be most common in the north. Second, because recent climate warming has advanced the onset of spring and associated phenology (Körner & Basler, Reference Körner and Basler2010), April should be increasing in frequency (relative to May and June). Despite making these a priori predictions, we were initially concerned that any environmental effects on names might be swamped by cultural fads in name popularity (Lieberson & Bell, Reference Lieberson and Bell1992; Berger & Le Mens, Reference Berger and Le Mens2009; Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022) (Figures S1 & S2) and drift (Hahn & Bentley, Reference Hahn and Bentley2003), as well as by population shifts and immigration (Rogerson & Kim, Reference Rogerson and Kim2005). Nevertheless, we proceeded to obtain and analyse name data.

Figure 2. Onset of spring and autumn in USA. (a) Onset of spring is indexed by the median date of the last frost (by state) in spring by state. (b) Onset of autumn is indexed by the median date of the first frost in autumn. [Note: populated regions of Hawaii do not experience frosts.]

Soon after we began analysing month names, we realised that we could also evaluate whether season names (e.g. Spring) showed geographic trends. However, when we found that Autumn (not Spring) was the most common season name (Figure 1b), but still before quantifying potential geographic patterns, we predicted that Autumn would be relatively frequent in high-latitude states, where autumn arrives relatively early (Figure 2b) and where deciduous foliage is most intensely coloured in autumn (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Yu and Donnelly2017).

As with all large-scale analyses of baby names (Bentley & Ormerod, Reference Bentley and Ormerod2012; Barucca et al., Reference Barucca, Rocchi, Marinara, Parisi and Ricci-Tersenghi2015; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Rosenberg and Greenbaum2022; Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022), our analyses are descriptive. However, most hypotheses we evaluate here were derived a priori and were based on environmental factors that vary geographically and that potentially influence name choice. Our resulting investigations fit within a general theoretical framework emphasising the sensitivity of human culture to the environment (van de Vliert, Reference van de Vliert2009; Carleton & Hsiang, Reference Carleton and Hsiang2016).

Materials and methods

Sources of month and season names

We accessed baby name data (N = 351.8M, 1910–2021) for the USA from the Social Security Administration (Supplementary Materials 2). ‘National files’ (1910–2021) report year of birth, assigned sex, given name and number of occurrences of each name. ‘State-specific’ files also indicate state of birth (1910–2021). To maintain privacy the Social Security Administration excludes names that occurred fewer than five times in a file. Data sources for name frequency in some other English-speaking countries, as well as sources of environmental and related data (e.g. coordinates of population centres of states, mean altitude, coverage of deciduous trees and shrubs), are available (Supplementary Materials 3 and 4).

Spatial analyses

We determined geographic and climatic correlates of month and of season names, and then inferred patterns of parental choices (Gureckis & Goldstone, Reference Gureckis and Goldstone2009; Acerbi & Bentley, Reference Acerbi and Bentley2014). We analysed only traditional English names for months and seasons, because variants (e.g. Apryl for April) are rare or sometimes ambiguous (Supplementary Materials 6). We computed the relative frequencies (by state) of each of the three common month names for girls (April, May, June; Figure 3a). Because such compositional data are non-independent, we analysed the log ratio of N April/(N April + N May + N June), thereby achieving ‘subcompositional coherence’ (Greenacre, Reference Greenacre2021). Similarly, we computed the log ratio of Autumn (N Autumn/(N Autumn + N Winter + N Spring + N Summer). Because season names were extremely rare before about 1975 (Figure 4b; Supplemental Materials 2, Figure S2d), we restricted seasonal analyses to 1975–2021. We used climate normals (1981–2010) to estimate median dates of the last frost and of first frost by state as indices of the start of spring and of autumn (Figure 2), respectively. However, because frost dates were tightly correlated with latitude, we included only latitude in statistical models to avoid collinearity. Also, because variation in the timing of birth varies with latitude (Martinez-Bakker et al., Reference Martinez-Bakker, Bakker, King and Rohani2014; Figure S3), thus potentially biasing latitudinal patterns of name popularity, we computed log ratio April births (or log ratio Autumn births) based on the numbers of births per month (season) by state and included these ratios in spatial analyses.

Figure 3. Geographic and climatic trends of relative frequency of April and of Autumn names of girls (1910–2021). (a) Choropleth map of relative percentage of all girls with ‘spring-month’ names that are named April. (b) Log ratio of April (equivalent proportion is on right y-axis) is negatively correlated (see text) with population-centred latitude of each CONUS state (i.e. states in continental USA). Points for Alaska (AK) and Hawaii (HI) are highlighted. (c) Log ratio of April is inversely correlated with median date of last frost in spring. [Note: Hawaii does not experience frosts and is absent from panels (c) and (f).] (d) Map of relative proportion of girls with season names that are named Autumn. (e) Log ratio of Autumn is positively correlated with latitude and (f) inversely correlated with the date of the first frost in autumn.

Figure 4. (a). Heat map of proportion of all AMJ girls named April by state and by year. States are arrayed by latitude (top = high latitude). White boxes indicate years when fewer than five girls had an AMJ (or season) name. Note the bubble in April names in essentially all states from ~1970 to ~1990 that began declining in the early 1990s, especially at many mid- to high-latitude states. (b) Heat map of the proportion of girls named Autumn by state and season. Before the mid-1970s, few girls had a season name. A modest bubble in Autumn names occurred during ~1980–1990, but mainly in mid- to high-latitude states.

Statistical models accounted for spatial autocorrelation via (Simultaneous Autoregressive Model, ‘SAR,’ Bivand et al., Reference Bivand, Millo and Piras2021). Spatial weights were generated from the adjacency of states (see Supplementary Materials 1.8).

Results

Month names (geographic patterns)

Month names were uncommon for both girls and boys (Rogerson, Reference Rogerson2016), but month names were 9.4 times more numerous for girls than boys (0.278 vs. 0.030% of babies of each sex, 1910–2021; Figure 1a). Season names were less common than month names (Figure 1b), but season names were 182 times more numerous for girls than boys (0.135% vs. < 0.001%; Figure 1b). Since 1975, however, season names have increased in popularity (Figure S2).

Parents may choose a month or season name for various reasons unrelated to temporal meaning (e.g. honouring a relative, loved one, celebrity). Interestingly, only 41.6% of a sample of girls with a month name were born in that same month, and only 37.5% of girls with season names were born in that same season. Thus, month (Rogerson, Reference Rogerson2016) and season names are only partially associated with birth timing, potentially blurring links between names and seasonal environmental factors. However, as shown below, those links are still strong.

The relative percentage of April names varies substantially among states (range 23.6–77.4%; Figure 3a). Consistent with our first a priori prediction, April was the dominant spring-month name in low-latitude CONUS states (Figure 3a,b; note, CONUS is an acronym for Continental United States), where spring starts early (Figure 2a). In contrast, June was dominant in high-latitude states (Figure S4a), where spring starts later (Figure 2b). Indeed, the percentage of April names for the eight states along the southern US border (median = 65.5%, range = 50.1–73.2%) does not overlap with those for the 12 states on the northern border (median = 38.9%, range = 23.4–44.6%).

In a spatial autocorrelation analysis, the log ratio of April declined strongly with log latitude (p << 0.0001) but was independent of log mean altitude (p = 0.155) and log April births (p = 0.53; Supplemental Materials, Table S1). Extra-limital states (Alaska or Hawaii) are striking outliers (Figure 3b, see Supplemental Materials 9).

Month names (temporal patterns)

Our second a priori prediction was that April would have increased in relative frequency in recent years because recent climate warming has advanced the onset of spring (Körner & Basler, Reference Körner and Basler2010). The frequency of April did shift, but non-linearly and wildly over time (Figure 4a). Initially, April was uncommon but then became almost the exclusive month name (a name ‘bubble’) from the late 1960s until the end of the twentieth century. Thereafter, April has dropped in relative popularity, especially in northern states (top right Figure 4a).

To examine whether overall latitudinal trends (Figure 3b) persisted in the face of such extreme temporal shifts (Figure 4a), we partitioned the data into three periods (below), based on the frequency of April crossing a 75% frequency threshold. In the initial period (1910–1965), the relative percentage of April was low among states (median = 10.6%). Even so, the log ratio of April declined significantly with log latitude during this period (p < 0.0023, Figure S5, Tables S4). During the name bubble in the middle period (~1966 to ~2008), however, virtually all girls with spring-month names were named April (>84.5% in all states, median = 97.4%, Figure S5), and no latitudinal trend was evident (p = 0.660, Table S5). During the recent period (2009–2021), April has declined in popularity (median = 31.7%), but the log ratio of April re-established the negative correlation with log latitude (p = 0.002, Table S6.). April is currently more popular than in the first period (paired t-test, p <<< 0.001), consistent with a climate-warming prediction, but whether it continues to drop or stabilises at a relatively elevated level can only be determined in the future.

Season names

Next, we evaluated our prediction that Autumn would be the most common season name at high latitudes, where autumn weather comes early (Figure 2b) and where deciduous autumn foliage is typically most intensely coloured (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Yu and Donnelly2017). Season names became common only after ~1975 (Figure 4b, Figure S2d) and since then have been relatively stable in popularity (Figure 4b). For 1975–2021, the log ratio of Autumn increased significantly with log latitude (Figure 3e, p = 0.009), but not with elevation (p = 0.346) or log Autumn births (p = 0.386)(Table S2). The mean percentage of Autumn among season names was 70.3% (58.4–86.6%) for the northern border states vs. only 51.8% (45.5–67.4%) for the southern border states – essentially a 36% increase in average popularity.

The percentage of a state's area that is covered by deciduous foliage is available for 30 eastern states (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhang, Yu and Donnelly2017; Ye & Zhang, Reference Ye and Zhang2021). For these states, the log ratio of Autumn was positively correlated with log latitude (p << 0.0001) and with the percentage of the state's area currently covered by deciduous or mixed foliage (Figure 5; spatial analysis, p = 0.035, Table S3).

Figure 5. Autumn as a percentage of all season names (by state) increases significantly (p = 0.035, Table S3, seasonalty) with the proportion of that state (30 eastern states only) covered by deciduous shrubs or trees.

The seasonality of climate has diverse ecological and cultural effects (van de Vliert, Reference van de Vliert2009). For example, unique nicknames in China are relatively uncommon in seasonally demanding climates (He et al., Reference He, Chen and Ren2021). Consequently, we examined whether the diversity (Shannon index) of month or of season names was correlated with climate seasonality (difference between summer and winter temperatures; Supplemental Materials Figure S6, Table S6). Month-name diversity was unrelated to seasonality (p = 0.331), but season-name diversity was conspicuously low in highly seasonal environments (p = 0.0009). It is unclear why only the season-name pattern is consistent with an expectation that cultural diversity is constrained by harsh climates (He et al., Reference He, Chen and Ren2021).

Hemispheric and continental comparisons

Our month-name predictions were generated specifically for the Northern Hemisphere (NH) but should not hold in English-speaking countries in the Southern Hemisphere (SH), where April, May and June coincide with autumnal, not spring-like weather. Even so, these three names are still the most common month names in Australia (Figure S7). Consequently, we checked whether these month names are nonetheless relatively less popular in the SH than in the NH, as expected based on their oppositional seasonal associations. Any direct comparison of proportional name usage would be confounded by unmeasured cultural, historical and ethnic differences between hemispheres, but the ratio of total month to total season names provides a paired, within-country index of the relative popularity of month vs. season names. Name compilations for major English-speaking counties have limited temporal coverage (Supplementary Materials 2), and we analysed available data for 2000–2020. The month:season ratio varies markedly among countries (Table S7) but is always higher in NH countries (Scotland, Northern Ireland, England and Wales, Canada, CONUS) than in the SH (Australia, New Zealand) (Table S8).

The most popular season name differs between hemispheres or continents. From 2000 to 2020, Autumn was the most popular season name in the USA and Canada, whereas Summer was most popular season name in the UK (except Scotland) and almost exclusively so in Australia and New Zealand (Table S7). The relative popularity of Autumn among continents increases with the continent's proportion of deciduous trees (rs = 0.850, p = 0.015, Table S9). Specifically, North America has a much higher proportion of red-coloured deciduous species than does Europe (Renner & Zohner, Reference Renner and Zohner2020), and temperate Australia and New Zealand have relatively few deciduous trees (Dreiss & Volin, Reference Dreiss, Volin and Wang2014). The exceptionally bright reds of North American leaves may reflect relatively high concentrations of anthocyanins and xanthophylls, which provide protection against relatively high autumnal UV radiation in North America compared with Europe (Renner & Zohner, Reference Renner and Zohner2020).

Discussion

Shifts in popularity of baby names are typically viewed through a cultural evolutionary lens. Indeed, given names can be viewed as a cultural ‘product’, and changes in the popularity of such products (e.g. books, music, names) are potentially driven by their intrinsic quality, ‘context-based selection’, drift and negative frequency dependence (Hahn & Bentley, Reference Hahn and Bentley2003; Berger et al., Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012; Acerbi & Bentley, Reference Acerbi and Bentley2014; Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Dawson and Campbell2016; Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022). Given names have limited intrinsic merit (but see Berger et al., Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012; Pitcher et al., Reference Pitcher, Mesoudi and McElligott2013; Suire et al., Reference Suire, Mesa, Raymond and Barkat-Defradas2019), but their popularity is subject to cultural factors such as context, drift, sexual selection and frequency dependence (Gureckis & Goldstone, Reference Gureckis and Goldstone2009; Pitcher et al., Reference Pitcher, Mesoudi and McElligott2013; Suire et al., Reference Suire, Mesa, Raymond and Barkat-Defradas2019; Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022). Here we will argue that – at least for month and season names – context potentially includes not only the social, cultural and political environment of parents, but also their physical and biotic environment.

We proposed and then evaluated three hypotheses relating the popularity of month and season names to geography, climate and foliage. Two were strongly supported. First, April was relatively most popular in low-latitude states (Figure 3a,b) where spring-like weather comes early (Figure 2a). Second, Autumn was most popular in high-latitude states (Figure 3d,e) and in states with a high coverage of deciduous vegetation (Figure 5).

The overall observed covariation of names and environmental factors (Figures 3 and S2.4, S2.5; Tables S1–S3) is consistent with these expectations, and the marked strength of these latitudinal patterns is surprising (Figure 3), especially given that fewer than half of girls with month (season) names were born in that same month (season) (Rogerson, Reference Rogerson2016). Alaska and Hawaii are conspicuous outliers for month names (Figure 3b,c). The reason is unclear but might reflect these states’ ‘frontier’ geography (Varnum & Kitayama, Reference Varnum and Kitayama2011), small population size, relatively high overall proportion of immigrants and (for Alaska) the relatively high proportion of immigrants from southern CONUS states (see Supplementary Materials 9). Curiously, these two states are not marked outliers for season names (Figure 3e and f).

Our third prediction was that the popularity of April has increased with climate warming. April is more common now than prior to ~1960 but has been declining in popularity (Figure 4a; Figure S2C). Where and if it stabilises can be determined only in future years.

Some given names have marked boom–bust cycles of popularity (Berger & Le Mens, Reference Berger and Le Mens2009; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Maruvka, Ouren and Shnerb2012; Xi et al., Reference Xi, Zhang, Zhang, Ge, She and Zhang2014; Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022; Figure S1). Generation-length bubbles appear to be driven by negative frequency-dependent interactions (Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022) that involve cultural preferences for novelty vs. commonness (Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Dawson and Campbell2016; Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022) or for conformity vs. non-conformity (Acerbi & Bentley, Reference Acerbi and Bentley2014; Denton et al., Reference Denton, Liberman and Feldman2021; Newberry & Plotkin, Reference Newberry and Plotkin2022). In contrast, those names showing geographic signatures potentially suggest the involvement of sustained social, cultural or environmental influences (Fryer & Levitt, Reference Fryer and Levitt2004; Varnum & Kitayama, Reference Varnum and Kitayama2011; Berger et al., Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012; Xi et al., Reference Xi, Zhang, Zhang, Ge, She and Zhang2014; Barucca et al., Reference Barucca, Rocchi, Marinara, Parisi and Ricci-Tersenghi2015; Pomorski et al., Reference Pomorski, Krawczyk, Kułakowski, Kwapień and Ausloos2016) or sometimes the influence of transient events such as hurricanes or a politician's popularity (Berger et al., Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012; Kułakowski et al., Reference Kułakowski, Kulczycki, Misztal, Dydejczyk, Gronek and Krawczyk2016).

Month names illustrate both pulsed and geographic patterns. Most dramatically, April was common at low latitude for decades but then became almost the exclusive spring name around the end of the twentieth century (Figure 4a). Since then, April has declined most strongly at high latitudes (Figure 4a) and has re-established a negative correlation of its popularity with latitude (Figure S5). The end-of-the-century bubble for April coincides with a period of known name instability in the USA (Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Abebe and Campbell2010; Barucca et al., Reference Barucca, Rocchi, Marinara, Parisi and Ricci-Tersenghi2015; Pomorski et al., Reference Pomorski, Krawczyk, Kułakowski, Kwapień and Ausloos2016) as well as in the UK (Bush, Reference Bush2020). It partially overlaps with reproductive years of the Baby Boom Generation (born 1946–1964, Rogerson & Kim, Reference Rogerson and Kim2005) – a generation known for its disruptive impacts.

Ultimately, direct surveys (Lindsay & Dempsey, Reference Lindsay and Dempsey2017) will be needed to establish why parents chose particular baby names, but conducting such surveys on a geographic scale will be logistically challenging and raise privacy concerns. For the present, inferences of individual causation must rely on retrospective analyses of composite data (Acerbi & Bentley, Reference Acerbi and Bentley2014). Causation in such approaches may be obscured (or induced) by drift (Hahn & Bentley, Reference Hahn and Bentley2003), migration (Rogerson, Reference Rogerson2021) or transient fads (Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Dawson and Campbell2016).

Our analyses do not question the dominant role of culture and of social learning in name choice. For one thing, month and season names are too uncommon to serve as models for name choice. Nevertheless, the relative popularity of month and season names of baby girls does correlate strongly with geography and the environment, even though most girls with a month or season name were not born in that month or season. Overall, we interpret these patterns as indirect evidence that environmental factors sometimes override cultural influences in choice of a baby's name.

Acknowledgements

We thank A. Rutschmann for digitising coverage data of deciduous shrubs and trees. Hadley Wickham's ‘babynames’ R package helped inspire our analyses. Two reviewers provided constructive suggestions.

Author contributions

RBH: conceptualisation, formal analyses, writing, review and editing. DBM: contributed to project development, spatial analyses, and writing and review. Both authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Financial support

D.B.M. acknowledges support from NSF (DEB-1950636).

Conflict of interest declaration

We declare no conflict of interest in relation to our work.

Research transparency and reproducibility

To view the code for the main text, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.53.

Data availability statement

All data are publicly available. Sources are listed in the Supplementary Materials.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.53.

References

Acerbi, A., & Bentley, R. A. (2014). Biases in cultural transmission shape the turnover of popular traits. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 228236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. Holt.Google Scholar
Barucca, P., Rocchi, J., Marinara, E., Parisi, G., & Ricci-Tersenghi, F. (2015). Cross-correlations of American baby names. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 112, 79437947. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1507143112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentley, R. A., & Ormerod, P. (2012). Accelerated innovation and increased spatial diversity of U.S. popular culture. Advances in Complex Systems, 15, 11500111150011. https://doi.org/10.1142/S0219525911003232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, J., Bradlow, E. T., Braunstein, A., & Zhang, Y. (2012). From Karen to Katie: Using baby names to understand cultural evolution. Psychological Science, 23, 10671073. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, J., & Le Mens, G. (2009). How adoption speed affects the abdonment of cultural tastes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0812647106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. American Economic Review, 94, 9911013. http://www.jstor.org/stable/3592802CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bivand, R., Millo, G., & Piras, G. (2021). A review of software for spatial econometrics in R. Mathematics, 9, 1276. https://doi.org/10.3390/math9111276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bush, S. J. (2020). Ambivalence, avoidance, and appeal: alliterative aspects of Anglo anthroponynms. Names, 68, 141155. https://doi.org/10.1080/00277738.2020.1775471CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carleton, T. A., & Hsiang, S. M. (2016). Social and economic impacts of climate. Science, 353, aad9837. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aad9837CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denton, K. K., Liberman, U., & Feldman, M. W. (2021). On randomly changing conformity bias in cultural transmission. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 118, e2107204118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2107204118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dreiss, L. M., & Volin, J. C. (2014). Forests: Temperate evergreen and deciduous. In Wang, Y. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of natural resources, Vol. 1 (pp. 214223). CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fryer, R., Jr. & Levitt, S. (2004). The causes and consequences of distinctively Black names. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 119, 767805. https://doi.org/10.1162/0033553041502180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, J. R., & Stecklov, G. (2016). From Patrick to John F.: Ethnic names and occupational success in the last era of mass migration. American Sociological Review, 81(1), 85106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122415621910CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenacre, M. (2021). Compositional data analysis. Annual Review of Statistics and its Application, 8, 271299. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-statistics-042720-124436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gureckis, T. M., & Goldstone, R. L. (2009). How you named your child: Understanding the relationshjip betwen individual decision making and collective outcomes. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 651674. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01046.xCitCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, M. W., & Bentley, R. A. (2003). Drift as a mechanism for cultural change: An example from baby names. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 270, S120S123. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, L., Chen, Y., & Ren, X. (2021). Climato-economic origins of variations in uniqueness of nickname on Sina Weibo. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 599750. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.599750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, D. A., Maruvka, Y. E., Ouren, J., & Shnerb, N. M. (2012). You name it – How memory and delay govern first name dynamics. PLoS One, 7, e38790. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038790CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Körner, C., & Basler, D. (2010). Phenology under global warming. Science, 327, 14611462. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1186473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kułakowski, K., Kulczycki, P., Misztal, K., Dydejczyk, A., Gronek, P., & Krawczyk, M. J. (2016). Naming boys after U.S. presidents in 20th century. Acta Physica Polonica A, 129, 10381044. https://doi.org/10.12693/APhysPolA.129.1038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberson, S., & Bell, E. O. (1992). Children's first names: An empirical study of social taste. American Journal of Sociology, 98, 511554. https://doi.org/10.1086/230048CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindsay, J., & Dempsey, J. (2017). First names and social distinction: Middle-class naming practices in Australia. Journal of Sociology, 53, 577591. https://doi.org/10.1177/1440783317690925CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, L., Zhang, X., Yu, U., & Donnelly, A. (2017). Detecting spatiotemporal changes of peak foliage coloration in deciduous and mixedforests across the central and eastern United States. Environmental Research Letters, 12, 024013. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/aa5b3aCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, X., Rosenberg, N. A., & Greenbaum, G. (2022). Extracting hierarchical features of cultural variation using network-based clustering. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 4, e18. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez-Bakker, M., Bakker, K. M., King, A. A., & Rohani, P. (2014). Human birth seasonality: Latitudinal gradient and interplay with childhood disease dynamics. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20132438. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss-Racusin, C. A., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J., … Handelsman, J. (2012). Science faculty's subtle gender biases favor male students. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 109, , 1647416479. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1211286109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newberry, M. G., & Plotkin, J. B. (2022). Measuring frequency-dependent selection in culture. Nature Human Behaviour, 6, 1048-1055. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01342-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okonofua, J. A., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2015). Two strikes: Race and the disciplining of young students. Psychological Science, 26, 617624. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615570365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitcher, B. J., Mesoudi, A., & McElligott, A. G. (2013). Sex-based sound symbolism in English-language first names. PLoS One, 8, e64825. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064825CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomorski, M., Krawczyk, M. J., Kułakowski, K., Kwapień, J., & Ausloos, M. (2016). Inferring cultural regions from correlation networks of given baby names. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 445, 167175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2015.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, S. S., & Zohner, C. M. (2020). Further analysis of 1532 deciduous woody species from North America, Europe, and Asia supports continental-scale differences in red autumn colouration. New Phytologist, 228, 814815. https://doi.org/10.1111/nph.16594CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogerson, P. A. (2016). The effects of months, holidays, and birthdates of presidents on choice of baby names. Names, 64, 234241. https://doi.org/10.1080/00277738.2016.1223121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogerson, P. A. (2021). Historical change in the large-scale population distribution of the United States. Applied Geography, 136, 102563. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apgeog.2021.102563CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogerson, P. A., & Kim, D. (2005). Population distribution and redistribution of the baby-boom cohort in the United States: Recent trends and implications. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102(43), 1531915324. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0507318102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlesinger, A. M. (1941). Patriotism names the baby. The New England Quarterly, 14, 611618. https://doi.org/10.2307/360597CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suire, A., Mesa, A. B., Raymond, M., & Barkat-Defradas, M. (2019). Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 1, E7. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twenge, J. M., Abebe, M. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2010). Fitting in or standing out: Trends in American parents’ choices for children's names, 1880–2007. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1, 1925. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550609349515CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twenge, J. M., Dawson, L., & Campbell, W. K. (2016). Still standing out: Children's names in the United States during the Great Recession and correlations with economic indicators. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46, 663670. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van de Vliert, E. (2009). Climate, affluence, and culture. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
van de Vliert, E. (2013). Climato-economic habitats support patterns of human needs, stresses, and freedoms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 465580. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12002828CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varnum, M. E. W., & Kitayama, S. (2011). What's in a name?: Popular names are less common on frontiers. Psychological Science, 22, 176183. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610395396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xi, N., Zhang, Z.-K., Zhang, Y.-C., Ge, Z., She, L., & Zhang, K. (2014). Cultural evolution: The case of babies’ first names. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 406, 139144. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2014.03.042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ye, Y., & Zhang, X. (2021). Exploration of global spatiotemporal changes of fall foliage coloration in deciduous forests and shrubs using the VIIRS land surface phenology product. Science of Remote Sensing, 4, 100030. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.srs.2021.100030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Numbers of babies with month or season names in the USA (1910–2021). (a) Total numbers of girls and boys with each month name. April, May and June are most common for girls, whereas August is the only common name for boys. (b) Season names are much more common for girls than boys, and Autumn and Summer are the most common season names for girls.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Onset of spring and autumn in USA. (a) Onset of spring is indexed by the median date of the last frost (by state) in spring by state. (b) Onset of autumn is indexed by the median date of the first frost in autumn. [Note: populated regions of Hawaii do not experience frosts.]

Figure 2

Figure 3. Geographic and climatic trends of relative frequency of April and of Autumn names of girls (1910–2021). (a) Choropleth map of relative percentage of all girls with ‘spring-month’ names that are named April. (b) Log ratio of April (equivalent proportion is on right y-axis) is negatively correlated (see text) with population-centred latitude of each CONUS state (i.e. states in continental USA). Points for Alaska (AK) and Hawaii (HI) are highlighted. (c) Log ratio of April is inversely correlated with median date of last frost in spring. [Note: Hawaii does not experience frosts and is absent from panels (c) and (f).] (d) Map of relative proportion of girls with season names that are named Autumn. (e) Log ratio of Autumn is positively correlated with latitude and (f) inversely correlated with the date of the first frost in autumn.

Figure 3

Figure 4. (a). Heat map of proportion of all AMJ girls named April by state and by year. States are arrayed by latitude (top = high latitude). White boxes indicate years when fewer than five girls had an AMJ (or season) name. Note the bubble in April names in essentially all states from ~1970 to ~1990 that began declining in the early 1990s, especially at many mid- to high-latitude states. (b) Heat map of the proportion of girls named Autumn by state and season. Before the mid-1970s, few girls had a season name. A modest bubble in Autumn names occurred during ~1980–1990, but mainly in mid- to high-latitude states.

Figure 4

Figure 5. Autumn as a percentage of all season names (by state) increases significantly (p = 0.035, Table S3, seasonalty) with the proportion of that state (30 eastern states only) covered by deciduous shrubs or trees.

Supplementary material: File

Huey and Miles supplementary material

Huey and Miles supplementary material 1

Download Huey and Miles supplementary material(File)
File 2.6 MB
Supplementary material: File

Huey and Miles supplementary material

Huey and Miles supplementary material 2

Download Huey and Miles supplementary material(File)
File 3.6 MB