Hostname: page-component-f554764f5-rvxtl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-15T01:31:39.876Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relationship between trackmakers of the Laetoli footprints from gait synchronization

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2025

Wataru Nakahashi*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Social Sciences, Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan

Abstract

The parallel trails of footprints at Laetoli site G are important fossils for studying the characteristics of Australopithecus afarensis. However, the relationship between the trackmakers – i.e. whether it was that of an adult male–female pair or of parent–offspring – remains unclear. The footprints show that the two individuals walked side by side with a narrow and constant distance between them and synchronized their leg movements and step lengths (gait synchronization), although they had a large height difference. In this study, live camera videos were collected to obtain data on gait synchronization in Homo sapiens, the closest extant species to A. afarensis. The data showed that when two humans with a large height difference walked alongside each other, with (at least) one of the pair having their arm around the other’s shoulder or back, adult male–female pairs (couples) frequently synchronized their gait, but parent–offspring pairs did not, whereas both couples and parent–offspring seldom synchronized when they walked side by side without connection or with handholding. Two individuals only maintained a narrow and constant distance like that between the Laetoli footprints when they walked with an arm-around connection. Therefore, assuming that A. afarensis had the same gait synchronization tendency as H. sapiens, the trackmakers were more likely to be an adult male–female pair than a parent–offspring one.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press.

1. Introduction

The Laetoli footprints are important trace fossils for investigating the morphology and behaviour of early hominins, particularly Australopithecus afarensis (Getty Conservation Institute, 1996, 2000, 2001, 2011; Leakey, Reference Leakey1981; Leakey & Hay, Reference Leakey and Hay1979; Tattersall, Reference Tattersall1998; White & Suwa, Reference White and Suwa1987). Although footprints at site A received considerable attention recently (McNutt et al., Reference McNutt, Hatala, Miller, Adams, Casana, Deane, DeSilva, Fabian, Fannin, Gaughan, Gill, Gurtu, Gustafson, Hill, Johnson, Kallindo, Kilham, Kilham, Kim and DeSilva2021), those that have attracted the most interest were found at site G, where three individuals had made parallel trails (Fig. 1). Small (G1) and large (G2) individuals walked side by side, and an intermediate-sized individual (G3) followed behind, stepping on the footprints of G2 (Getty Conservation Institute, 1996, 2000, 2001, 2011; Leakey, Reference Leakey1981; Tattersall, Reference Tattersall1998). Many researchers have investigated the relationship between them on the basis of footprint shape. Focusing on the small and large individuals (because the possibility that G3 walked there some time after the two other individuals had done so cannot be excluded), some consider that they were an adult male and female pair (Tattersall, Reference Tattersall1998), while others argue that they were an adult and a child (probably a parent–offspring pair) (Leakey, Reference Leakey1981; Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016). Considering that the footprints are not well preserved and existing foot fossils of A. afarensis are of poor quality and quantity, reaching a consensus is difficult. Recently, new footprints (S1 and S2) were found in Laetoli (Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016), which are appreciably larger than previous ones, suggesting a considerable body size variation in this species that might indicate large sexual dimorphism. However, if G1 and G2 were an adult male–female pair, this would suggest that the variation existed not only between sexes but also within males.

The parallel trails of G1 and G2 have an interesting feature: gait synchronization. That is, the two individuals synchronized their leg movements and step lengths (Leakey, Reference Leakey1981; Tattersall, Reference Tattersall1998). This feature may not have been a coincidence, because G1 and G2 had a large difference in foot length, which is a good indicator of height (Jasuja et al., Reference Jasuja, Singh and Jain1991; Tuttle, Reference Tuttle, Leakey and Harris1987; Tuttle et al., Reference Tuttle, Webb, Weidl and Baksh1990; Wiseman & De Groote, Reference Wiseman and De Groote2022). Height and step length generally correlate in humans (Guest et al., Reference Guest, Miguel-Hurtado, Stevenage and Black2017; Jasuja, Reference Jasuja1993; Jasuja et al., Reference Jasuja, Harbhajan and Anupama1997), and a newly discovered tall individual (S1), who walked alone, had a longer step length than G1 and G2 (Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016; Table 1), suggesting that the same correlation existed in A. afarensis. Moreover, the step length-to-height ratio of S1 lies between those of G1 and G2 (Table 1; calculated from Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016), which indicates that G2 may have shortened his/her step length, as the ratio generally decreases slightly with height in humans (Ahmedov et al., Reference Ahmedov, Lesinger, Oytun, Filiz and Özkan2022). This confirms that the two individuals did not walk at different times, as G2’s shortened step length would be inexplicable if G2 had walked alone. Although some researchers have investigated gait synchronization in humans (Zivotofsky & Hausdorff, Reference Zivotofsky and Hausdorff2007; van Ulzen et al., Reference van Ulzen, Lamoth, Daffertshofer, Semin and Beek2008; Zivotofsky et al., Reference Zivotofsky, Gruendlinger and Hausdorff2012, Reference Zivotofsky, Bernad-Elazari, Grossman and Hausdorff2018; Chambers et al., Reference Chambers, Kong, Wei and Kording2019; reviewed in Felsberg & Rhea, Reference Felsberg and Rhea2021), it is uncertain which type of pair, an adult male–female pair (couple) or a parent–offspring one, more frequently synchronize their gait if they have a large difference in height. Given that Homo sapiens is the closest extant species to A. afarensis, the characteristics of human locomotion may provide valuable insights for estimating those of A. afarensis.

Table 1. Data and estimates for G1, G2, and S1 (from Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016)

2. Methods

2.1. Overview

In this study, live camera videos were collected from YouTube to determine the frequency of gait synchronization between human couples and between a parent and an offspring. To compare with the Laetoli footprints, it is important to investigate human walking under natural conditions. The methodology complies with the terms of service and use of YouTube, and satisfies the ethics regulations related to research with human subjects at Waseda University.

Considering that the estimated height of G1 is approximately 20% smaller than that of G2 (Leakey, Reference Leakey1981; Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016), two individuals walking side by side were sampled, with one being one to two heads shorter than the other. Additionally, given that the serial parallel trails of footprints at the southern sector of Laetoli site G consist of around 10 steps (Leakey, Reference Leakey1981; Getty Conservation Institute, 1996, 2000, 2001, 2011; Fig. 1), a case in which two individuals synchronized their leg movements for 10 or more steps was defined as gait synchronization. That is, starting from the moment when two individuals stepped (almost) simultaneously, if an individual with a shorter step length (typically a smaller individual) had not started their eleventh step (toe-off) by the time the other (typically a larger individual) had finished their tenth step (heel-contact), their gaits were considered synchronized. In fact, most unsynchronized dyads differed in their step counts by around five steps, while most synchronized dyads continued synchronization for more than 10 steps; therefore, their categorization was straightforward. Note that 10-step synchronization is the minimum criterion for gait synchronization, although the methods and criteria used to identify it vary across previous studies, which have often analysed the gaits of dyads of similar heights under controlled laboratory conditions over longer distances (reviewed in Felsberg & Rhea, Reference Felsberg and Rhea2021). It is reasonable to apply the most lenient criteria in this study – which observed dyads with a large height difference under natural conditions – where their walking patterns were not perfectly consistent or straight. Moreover, since YouTube videos were used in this study, the more precise methods employed in previous studies, such as 3D motion capture, were unavailable. However, this was not a significant issue, as synchronized and unsynchronized dyads were generally easily distinguishable, probably because dyads with a large height difference were specifically sampled. Note that quantifying the phase synchronization of gait rhythms reveals a bimodal distribution, indicating that dyad gaits are classified as either synchronized or unsynchronized (Zivotofsky et al., Reference Zivotofsky, Gruendlinger and Hausdorff2012). Additionally, although the synchronization pattern of dyads may have differed outside the frame, considering that the Laetoli footprints represent nearly a 10-step snapshot, sampling 10-step snapshots from the webcam provides a reasonable comparison.

Figure 1. Shaded 3D photogrammetric elevation model of the footprints at the southern sector of Laetoli site G (modified from Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016).

The contact between two individuals was categorized into three types: unconnected, handholding, and arm-around. Handholding includes cases where one individual grasps the hand or arm of the other, and the two individuals walk arm in arm. Arm-around includes cases where at least one individual places an arm around the other’s shoulder or back. If a smaller individual was a child, the pair was considered as a parent–offspring pair, and if she was an adult female, they were a couple. However, this assumption does not matter because what should be investigated is whether the small individual of the Laetoli footprints (G1) was a child. Since live camera videos taken at the beach in summer were used, many couples and families who were lightly dressed and walked with sandals or barefoot were included in the footage. Therefore, their ages, sexes, and heights were easily estimated. Note that errors in the estimation of age and sex do not affect the significant results, as they reduce the differences between categories.

2.2. Sample collection

Live camera videos were collected from the YouTube account ‘Beaches Be Trippin’ (https://www.youtube.com/@BeachesBeTrippin) in Ocean City, Maryland, USA, in July 2023. Weekend videos were primarily used to collect samples effectively. To obtain a sufficient sample size, videos of walking couples were collected for one day (around 12 hours), those of parent–offspring pairs for two days, and dyads with an arm-around connection for 10 days. The detailed dates and URLs of videos are shown in Supplementary Table S1. The recorded data include the relationship between two individuals (couple/father–son/father–daughter/mother–son/mother–daughter), the walking side of the taller individual (right/left), the contact between the two individuals (unconnected/handholding/arm-around (single/mutual)), gait synchronization (unsynchronized/synchronized), and the phase of synchronization (in-phase/anti-phase) if synchronized. In all samples, the taller individuals were males or parents, probably because dyads with a height difference of one to two heads were sampled. When individuals changed walking sides or the type of contact, or the phase of synchronization in the videos changed, the more clearly observable (closer to the camera) or smaller sample data were used. Considering that distinguishing between all individuals is impossible, all applicable samples were counted if they satisfied the following regulations, although some of them seemed to include the same individuals. If they were excluded, an unintentional bias may have arisen, because individuals with gait synchronization tend to be observed more carefully.

2.3. Sampling regulations

Videos of two individuals with a height difference of one to two heads walking side by side were collected. Since gait synchronization was defined as the synchronization of leg movements for 10 or more steps, dyads without observable 10 steps (e.g. due to walking behind others) or those who frequently changed positions and did not walk side by side for 10 steps were excluded. The following individuals were also excluded: jogging individuals, individuals carrying a pushchair, and more than two individuals walking side by side without connection.

2.4. Statistical analysis

To calculate the frequency of gait synchronization (number of synchronized dyads/total number) in each category, the samples that did or did not satisfy the gait synchronization criteria were counted (Supplementary Tables S2–S12). Pearson’s chi-squared test was primarily used to calculate the P-values of the data, but Fisher’s exact test was used when one or more expected values were below five. When two frequencies were significantly different, asterisks (*, **, ***) were used in the figures to indicate P < 0.05, P < 0.01, and P < 0.001, respectively.

3. Results

The data showed that the frequency of gait synchronization between couples was not significantly different from that between parent and offspring if unconnected (P = 0.10, chi-squared test), but higher if handholding (P < 0.01, chi-squared test, Fig. 2). In addition, both between couples and between parent and offspring, the frequency was not significantly different between unconnected and handholding (P = 0.71 and P = 0.20, respectively, chi-squared test). Pooling unconnected and handholding, the frequency of gait synchronization between couples was higher than that between parent and offspring (P < 0.01, chi-squared test). The following data were pooled in analyses because no significant differences in relation to gait synchronization were observed (Supplementary Tables S2–S12): the walking side of the taller individual (right/left), the sexes of parent–offspring (father–son/father–daughter/mother–son/mother–daughter), and the phase of synchronization (in-phase/anti-phase). Despite the lenient gait synchronization criteria in this study, the frequency of gait synchronization was lower than in previous studies (Chambers et al., Reference Chambers, Kong, Wei and Kording2019; van Ulzen et al., Reference van Ulzen, Lamoth, Daffertshofer, Semin and Beek2008; Zivotofsky et al., Reference Zivotofsky, Gruendlinger and Hausdorff2012, Reference Zivotofsky, Bernad-Elazari, Grossman and Hausdorff2018; Zivotofsky & Hausdorff, Reference Zivotofsky and Hausdorff2007), probably because only dyads with a large height difference under natural conditions were sampled. This may justify the use of the lenient criteria in this study.

Figure 2. Frequencies of gait synchronization for unconnected dyads, handholding dyads, and the combined group (mean + SEM). **P < 0.01.

Despite significant differences in the frequency of gait synchronization between couples and parent–offspring, they may have been due to differences in height between the two individuals. That is, although the sampled dyads had a height difference of one to two heads, the average difference between couples may have been smaller than that between parent and offspring because lower height differences were more common in couples, whereas this tendency was not observed in parent–offspring. Therefore, no essential differences in the frequency of gait synchronization were found between couples and parent–offspring, as long as they walked side by side without connection or with handholding.

Compared to the frequency of gait synchronization in unconnected or handholding dyads, that in arm-around dyads was significantly higher in couples (P < 0.001, chi-squared test), but no significant difference was found in parent–offspring (P = 0.49, Fisher’s exact test, Fig. 3). The difference between couples and parent–offspring was large (P < 0.001, chi-squared test); i.e. the frequency of gait synchronization between couples was more than 10 times higher than that between parent and offspring. In other words, couples frequently synchronized their gait if one placed an arm around the other, whereas parent–offspring did not.

Figure 3. Frequencies of gait synchronization for unconnected or handholding dyads and arm-around dyads (mean + SEM). **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

This relationship might stem from the differing reasons for the arm-around connection between couples and parent–offspring. Specifically, an arm-around walk between couples generally expresses deep affection, whereas in parent–offspring, it sometimes implies that the parent is guiding the child to prevent their walking in the wrong direction. In investigating this influence, arm-around dyads were categorized into two types: single and mutual. Mutual arm-around corresponds to cases in which both individuals place an arm around the other’s shoulder or back, while single arm-around indicates that only one individual places an arm around the other. Children who want to walk freely are unlikely to place their arm around their parents, so a mutual arm-around implies deep affection, whether between couples or parent–offspring. The data showed that regardless of the type of arm-around, the frequency of gait synchronization between couples was significantly higher than that between parent and offspring (P < 0.001 in both types, chi-squared test, Fig. 4). Therefore, the hypothesis that the differing reasons for the arm-around connection caused the large difference in the frequency of gait synchronization between couples and parent–offspring was not supported. However, the frequency of gait synchronization between single arm-around couples was significantly lower than that between mutual arm-around couples (P < 0.01, chi-squared test) but higher than that between unconnected or handholding couples (P < 0.001, chi-squared test). In contrast, the frequency of gait synchronization between parent and offspring did not differ across unconnected, handholding, single, and mutual arm-around conditions (P = 0.55, Fisher’s exact test). This implies that closer contact enhances gait synchronization between couples but not between parent and offspring. Note that the possibility that affectionate couples tend to prefer close contact and to synchronize their gait cannot be excluded. In fact, a few couples initiated an arm-around connection during the walk, but they synchronized their gait beforehand.

Figure 4. Frequencies of gait synchronization for single arm-around dyads and mutual arm-around dyads (mean + SEM). **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

4. Discussion

Previous studies have shown that gait synchronization frequently occurs when tactile feedback is presented (Zivotofsky et al., Reference Zivotofsky, Gruendlinger and Hausdorff2012; Zivotofsky & Hausdorff, Reference Zivotofsky and Hausdorff2007) and when individuals are not paying attention to other tasks (Zivotofsky et al., Reference Zivotofsky, Bernad-Elazari, Grossman and Hausdorff2018), which is consistent with the result between couples. However, these tendencies were not observed between parent and offspring under natural conditions. Although determining the reason for this observation is difficult, several hypotheses can be proposed. For example, parents may hold their children more tightly if they are more restless, but such children may also walk independently, preventing gait synchronization. Additionally, parents may have extensive experience of walking side by side with their young children, who have extremely short step lengths. As a result, they become accustomed to unsynchronized gaits and continue this behaviour even after their children have grown. In any case, provided that the difference in gait synchronization tendencies between couples and parent–offspring is not due to a human-specific factor, A. afarensis could be considered to have exhibited the same distinction as H. sapiens.

Let us look at the Laetoli footprints at site G again (Fig. 1). The distance between two individuals (G1 and G2) is quite narrow (approximately 30 cm) and remains constant throughout the trails (Getty Conservation Institute, 1996, 2000, 2001, 2011; Leakey, Reference Leakey1981; Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016; Tattersall, Reference Tattersall1998). Considering the body size of A. afarensis, almost no space was found between two individuals (Leakey, Reference Leakey1981; Tattersall, Reference Tattersall1998). When humans walk side by side without connection or with handholding, they must maintain a comfortable distance, which often changes because humans do not walk in a straight line under natural conditions. Therefore, two individuals can maintain a narrow and constant distance only when walking with an arm-around connection. These observations strongly suggest that G1 and/or G2 placed an arm around the other’s shoulder or back. Note that the constant distance between G1 and G2 further confirms that they did not walk at different times. This study shows that when two humans walk side by side with an arm-around connection, couples frequently synchronize their gait, while parent–offspring pairs rarely do so under natural conditions. Therefore, discovering human footprints resembling those at Laetoli site G might indicate the presence of a couple rather than a parent–offspring. The odd ratio would be more than 10-fold, although it depends on the frequency of parent–offspring pairs walking side by side with the arm-around connection relative to that of couples. Although the site observed in this study may not represent human society, the number of couples who left footprints similar to those at Laetoli site G (i.e. a narrow and constant distance between two individuals (arm-around) with in-phase gait synchronization) was 20 times larger than that of parent–offspring.

Given that the society of A. afarensis is not well understood, estimating the frequency of adult male–female pairs and parent–offspring walking side by side with an arm-around connection is difficult. However, humans invest significantly larger effort in child-rearing compared to other primates due to their long childhoods. Therefore, the frequency of parent–offspring walking side by side relative to that of couples would not be lower than that in other primates. Considering that the childhood of A. afarensis was shorter than that of H. sapiens (Dean et al., Reference Dean, Leakey, Reid, Schrenk, Schwartz, Stringer and Walker2001; Smith, Reference Smith1994), the relative frequency of parent–offspring pairs walking side by side with an arm-around connection in A. afarensis could not be considered high compared to that in humans. Therefore, if A. afarensis had the same gait-synchronization tendency as H. sapiens (i.e. when walking with the arm-around connection, adult male–female pairs frequently synchronize their gait, while parent–offspring seldom do), the Laetoli footprints at site G would be highly likely to belong to an adult male–female pair rather than a parent–offspring one.

The conclusion that an adult male–female pair left the footprints at Laetoli site G may suggest a large body-size variation among A. afarensis males, as S1 was significantly larger than G2 (Masao et al., Reference Masao, Ichumbaki, Cherin, Barili, Boschian, Iurino and Manzi2016). This suggestion is interesting because some primate males are dimorphic (e.g. orangutans), but the possibility that S1 was exceptionally large cannot be excluded. More samples are needed to discuss this problem. Additionally, the conclusion may indicate large sexual dimorphism in A. afarensis, but it is important to consider the small sample size of Laetoli trackmakers, which consists of only five. Therefore, sexual dimorphism in A. afarensis should be comprehensively investigated by considering other fossil evidence (Grabowski et al., Reference Grabowski, Hatala, Jungers and Richmond2015; Plavcan et al., Reference Plavcan, Lockwood, Kimbel, Lague and Harmon2005; Reno et al., Reference Reno, Meindl, McCollum and Lovejoy2003, Reference Reno, Meindl, McCollum and Lovejoy2005, Reference Reno, McCollum, Meindl and Lovejoy2010). Sexual dimorphism often reflects the mating system of the species, where large sexual dimorphism is assumed to imply polygyny (Leutenegger, Reference Leutenegger1978; Weckerly, Reference Weckerly1998). However, sexual dimorphism is not the only factor that indicates the mating system of early hominins (Lovejoy, Reference Lovejoy2009; Nakahashi, Reference Nakahashi2016; Nakahashi & Horiuchi, Reference Nakahashi and Horiuchi2012; Nakahashi et al., Reference Nakahashi, Horiuchi and Ihara2018). When studying the mating system in A. afarensis, it is important to focus on their behaviour, in that an adult male–female pair walked side by side with an arm-around connection and synchronized their gait. Moreover, the conclusion of this study may offer valuable insights for morphological studies of the Laetoli footprints. For example, some researchers have analysed 3D imaging of the trails to investigate whether their bipedal gait was kinematically similar to modern humans (Crompton et al., Reference Crompton, Pataky, Savage, D’août, Bennett, Day and Sellers2012; Hatala et al., Reference Hatala, Demes and Richmond2016; Raichlen et al., Reference Raichlen, Gordon, Harcourt-Smith, Foster, Haas and Rosenberg2010). However, considering that two individuals walked with one placing an arm around the other’s shoulder or back, the weight distribution of their legs may have differed from that of individuals walking alone. This perspective may also be important when discussing the significant asymmetry of foot angles in the G1 trail, which was previously thought to have been caused by trauma or disease in his/her right lower limb (Tuttle, Reference Tuttle, Leakey and Harris1987; Tuttle et al., Reference Tuttle, Webb, Weidl and Baksh1990). In any case, given that two individuals synchronized their leg movements and step lengths, direct comparisons of their footprints with those of humans walking alone are inappropriate.

To sum up, the trails at Laetoli site G show that two individuals synchronized their leg movements and step lengths while maintaining a narrow and constant distance between them. This feature suggests that they walked with one individual having an arm around the other’s shoulder or back. Observations of human dyads walking side by side indicate that gait synchronization occurs far more frequently between couples than between parent and offspring when one places an arm around the other. Applying this tendency to A. afarensis, it is likely that the Laetoli footprints at site G were left by an adult male–female pair rather than by a parent–offspring one.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2025.10.

Acknowledgements

I thank Dr Daisuke Kubo and Dr Hisao Baba for their valuable suggestions. ChatGPT-4o mini was used in part for English editing.

Author contributions

WN did everything.

Financial support

This research was supported by JSPS KAKENHI (Grant Numbers JP20H05137, JP20K06838) and Individual Research Allowance, Waseda University.

Competing interests

WN declares none.

Research transparency and reproducibility interest

All data used in this study are provided within the manuscript or supplementary file.

References

Ahmedov, S., Lesinger, F. Y., Oytun, M., Filiz, B., & Özkan, F. K. (2022). Walking speed gender differences in prepubertal children: An observational study. Cyprus Journal of Medical Sciences, 7(3), 404408. https://doi.org/10.4274/cjms.2022.2021-226Google Scholar
Chambers, C., Kong, G., Wei, K., & Kording, K. (2019). Pose estimates from online videos show that side-by-side walkers synchronize movement under naturalistic conditions. PloS One, 14(6), e0217861. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217861Google Scholar
Crompton, R. H., Pataky, T. C., Savage, R., D’août, K., Bennett, M. R., Day, M. H., … Sellers, W. I. (2012). Human-like external function of the foot, and fully upright gait, confirmed in the 3.66 million year old Laetoli hominin footprints by topographic statistics, experimental footprint-formation and computer simulation. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 9(69), 707719. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2011.0258Google Scholar
Dean, C., Leakey, M. G., Reid, D., Schrenk, F., Schwartz, G. T., Stringer, C., & Walker, A. (2001). Growth processes in teeth distinguish modern humans from Homo erectus and earlier hominins. Nature, 414(6864), 628631. https://doi.org/10.1038/414628aGoogle Scholar
Felsberg, D. T., & Rhea, C. K. (2021). Spontaneous interpersonal synchronization of gait: A systematic review. Archives of Rehabilitation Research and Clinical Translation, 3(1), 100097. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arrct.2020.100097Google Scholar
Getty Conservation Institute (1996). Laetoli project: Conservation of the hominid trackway site at Laetoli, Tanzania: Report on the 1995 field season (3 July – 2 September 1995). Getty Conservation Institute. http://hdl.handle.net/10020/gci_pubs/laetoli_field_report_1995Google Scholar
Getty Conservation Institute (2000). Laetoli project: Report on the 1996–1997 field seasons and the Olduvai museum exhibition. Getty Conservation Institute. http://hdl.handle.net/10020/gci_pubs/laetoli_field_report_1996_98Google Scholar
Getty Conservation Institute. (2001). Laetoli hominid trackway, site G: Condition records, G1 and G2/3 footprints. http://hdl.handle.net/10020/gci_pubs/laetoli_condition_recordsGoogle Scholar
Getty Conservation Institute. (2011). The Laetoli conservation project: A visual summary: A collaboration of the Tanzanian Department of Antiquities and the Getty Conservation Institute (1993–1998) (Rev. ed.). http://hdl.handle.net/10020/gci_pubs/laetoli_visual_summaryGoogle Scholar
Grabowski, M., Hatala, K. G., Jungers, W. L., & Richmond, B. G. (2015). Body mass estimates of hominin fossils and the evolution of human body size. Journal of Human Evolution, 85, 7593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2015.05.005Google Scholar
Guest, R., Miguel-Hurtado, O., Stevenage, S., & Black, S. (2017). Exploring the relationship between stride, stature and hand size for forensic assessment. Journal of Forensic and Legal Medicine, 52, 4655. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jflm.2017.08.006Google Scholar
Hatala, K. G., Demes, B., & Richmond, B. G. (2016). Laetoli footprints reveal bipedal gait biomechanics different from those of modern humans and chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 283(1836), 20160235. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.0235Google Scholar
Jasuja, O. P. (1993). Estimation of stature from footstep length. Forensic Science International, 61(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/0379-0738(93)90244-5Google Scholar
Jasuja, O. P., Harbhajan, S., & Anupama, K. (1997). Estimation of stature from stride length while walking fast. Forensic Science International, 86(3), 181186. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0379-0738(97)00038-8Google Scholar
Jasuja, O. P., Singh, J., & Jain, M. (1991). Estimation of stature from foot and shoe measurements by multiplication factors: A revised attempt. Forensic Science International, 50(2), 203215. https://doi.org/10.1016/0379-0738(91)90152-9Google Scholar
Leakey, M. D. (1981). Tracks and tools. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 292(1057), 95102. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1981.0017Google Scholar
Leakey, M. D., & Hay, R. L. (1979). Pliocene footprints in the Laetolil beds at Laetoli, northern Tanzania. Nature, 278(5702), 317323. https://doi.org/10.1038/278317a0Google Scholar
Leutenegger, W. (1978). Scaling of sexual dimorphism in body size and breeding system in primates. Nature, 272(5654), 610611. https://doi.org/10.1038/272610a0Google Scholar
Lovejoy, C. O. (2009). Reexamining human origins in light of Ardipithecus ramidus. Science, 326(5949), 7474e8. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1175834Google Scholar
Masao, F. T., Ichumbaki, E. B., Cherin, M., Barili, A., Boschian, G., Iurino, D. A., … Manzi, G. (2016). New footprints from Laetoli (Tanzania) provide evidence for marked body size variation in early hominins. Elife, 5, e19568. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.19568Google Scholar
McNutt, E. J., Hatala, K. G., Miller, C., Adams, J., Casana, J., Deane, A. S., DeSilva, J. M., Fabian, K., Fannin, L. D., Gaughan, S., Gill, S. V., Gurtu, J., Gustafson, E., Hill, A. C., Johnson, C., Kallindo, S., Kilham, B., Kilham, P., Kim, E., … DeSilva, J. M. (2021). Footprint evidence of early hominin locomotor diversity at Laetoli, Tanzania. Nature, 600(7889), 468471. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04187-7Google Scholar
Nakahashi, W. (2016). Coevolution of female ovulatory signals and male–male competition in primates. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 392, 1222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2015.12.007Google Scholar
Nakahashi, W., & Horiuchi, S. (2012). Evolution of ape and human mating systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 296, 5664. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.11.026Google Scholar
Nakahashi, W., Horiuchi, S., & Ihara, Y. (2018). Estimating hominid life history: The critical interbirth interval. Population Ecology, 60, 127142. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10144-018-0610-0Google Scholar
Plavcan, J. M., Lockwood, C. A., Kimbel, W. H., Lague, M. R., & Harmon, E. H. (2005). Sexual dimorphism in Australopithecus afarensis revisited: How strong is the case for a human-like pattern of dimorphism? Journal of Human Evolution, 48(3), 313320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.09.006Google Scholar
Raichlen, D. A., Gordon, A. D., Harcourt-Smith, W. E., Foster, A. D., Haas, W. R., Jr, & Rosenberg, K. (2010). Laetoli footprints preserve earliest direct evidence of human-like bipedal biomechanics. PloS One, 5(3), e9769. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009769Google Scholar
Reno, P. L., McCollum, M. A., Meindl, R. S., & Lovejoy, C. O. (2010). An enlarged postcranial sample confirms Australopithecus afarensis dimorphism was similar to modern humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1556), 33553363. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0086Google Scholar
Reno, P. L., Meindl, R. S., McCollum, M. A., & Lovejoy, C. O. (2003). Sexual dimorphism in Australopithecus afarensis was similar to that of modern humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(16), 94049409. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1133180100Google Scholar
Reno, P. L., Meindl, R. S., McCollum, M. A., & Lovejoy, C. O. (2005). The case is unchanged and remains robust: Australopithecus afarensis exhibits only moderate skeletal dimorphism. A reply to Plavcan et al. (2005). Journal of Human Evolution, 49(2), 279288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.04.008Google Scholar
Smith, B. H. (1994). Patterns of dental development in Homo, Australopithecus, Pan, and Gorilla. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 94(3), 307325. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330940303Google Scholar
Tattersall, I. (1998). The Laetoli diorama. Scientific American, 279(3), 53. https://www.jstor.org/stable/26057941Google Scholar
Tuttle, R. H. (1987). Kinesiological inferences and evolutionary implications from Laetoli bipedal trails G-l, G-2/3, and A. In Leakey, M. D. & Harris, J. M. (Eds.), Laetoli, a Pliocene site in Northern Tanzania (pp. 503523). Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Tuttle, R., Webb, D., Weidl, E., & Baksh, M. (1990). Further progress on the Laetoli trails. Journal of Archaeological Science, 17(3), 347362. https://doi.org/10.1016/0305-4403(90)90028-4Google Scholar
van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, G. R., & Beek, P. J. (2008). Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neuroscience Letters, 432(2), 8893. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2007.11.070Google Scholar
Weckerly, F. W. (1998). Sexual-size dimorphism: Influence of mass and mating systems in the most dimorphic mammals. Journal of Mammalogy, 79(1), 3352. https://doi.org/10.2307/1382840Google Scholar
White, T. D., & Suwa, G. (1987). Hominid footprints at Laetoli: Facts and interpretations. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 72(4), 485514. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330720409Google Scholar
Wiseman, A. L., & De Groote, I. (2022). One size fits all? Stature estimation from footprints and the effect of substrate and speed on footprint creation. The Anatomical Record, 305(7), 16921700. https://doi.org/10.1002/ar.24833Google Scholar
Zivotofsky, A. Z., Bernad-Elazari, H., Grossman, P., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2018). The effects of dual tasking on gait synchronization during over-ground side-by-side walking. Human Movement Science, 59, 2029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2018.03.009Google Scholar
Zivotofsky, A. Z., Gruendlinger, L., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2012). Modality-specific communication enabling gait synchronization during over-ground side-by-side walking. Human Movement Science, 31(5), 12681285. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2012.01.003Google Scholar
Zivotofsky, A. Z., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: An initial investigation. Journal of Neuroengineering & Rehabilitation, 4(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-4-28Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Data and estimates for G1, G2, and S1 (from Masao et al., 2016)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Shaded 3D photogrammetric elevation model of the footprints at the southern sector of Laetoli site G (modified from Masao et al., 2016).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Frequencies of gait synchronization for unconnected dyads, handholding dyads, and the combined group (mean + SEM). **P < 0.01.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Frequencies of gait synchronization for unconnected or handholding dyads and arm-around dyads (mean + SEM). **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Frequencies of gait synchronization for single arm-around dyads and mutual arm-around dyads (mean + SEM). **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Supplementary material: File

Nakahashi supplementary material

Nakahashi supplementary material
Download Nakahashi supplementary material(File)
File 32.8 KB