Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T05:36:31.391Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal grandmothers buffer the effects of ethnic discrimination among pregnant Latina mothers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 November 2023

Delaney A. Knorr*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
Molly M. Fox
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
*
Corresponding author: Delaney A. Knorr; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Ethnic discrimination during pregnancy is linked to maternal psychological distress, adverse birth outcomes and increased offspring morbidity and mortality. An evolutionary perspective reframes offspring health issues as a risk to maternal fitness. We argue that kin may be evolutionarily motivated to buffer psychosocial stressors for the mother during pregnancy. Previously, we found that the relationship of a pregnant woman with her own mother (fetus’ maternal grandmother) had a positive association on maternal prenatal psychology, above and beyond her relationship with her fetus’ father. Here, we ask if grandmothers buffer mothers’ prenatal psychological distress from ethnic discrimination. Using self-report data collected from 216 pregnant Latina women living in Southern California, we found discrimination to be significantly, positively associated with depression, anxiety, and stress in linear regression models. Maternal grandmother communication attenuated the association of discrimination and all three psychological distress measures, adjusting for the mother's relationship with the father. Maternal grandmother emotional support similarly significantly moderated the relationship of discrimination with depression and anxiety. We did not observe any significant interactions for paternal grandmother relationships. Geographic proximity was not a significant stress buffer. Results suggest the important role maternal grandmothers play in perinatal mental health, and that these benefits exist uncoupled from geographic proximity.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: Grandmother–mother relationships buffer against ill-effects of ethnic discrimination on prenatal psychological distress.

1. Introduction

The framework of developmental origins of health and disease, DOHaD, describes how mothers’ prenatal environmental exposures (including psychosocial stressors) can create long-term phenotypic changes in offspring that lead to elevated disease risk across the life course (Gluckman et al., Reference Gluckman, Hanson, Cooper and Thornburg2008). When pregnant women experience psychosocial stressors, perturbations to fetal development, birth timing and infant development can occur. Prenatal maternal psychological distress (including stress, anxiety, and depression) has also been associated with low-birth weight and preterm birth (Grigoriadis et al., Reference Grigoriadis, VonderPorten, Mamisashvili, Tomlinson, Dennis, Koren and Radford2013, Reference Grigoriadis, Graves, Peer, Mamisashvili, Tomlinson, Vigod and Richter2018; Grote et al., Reference Grote, Bridge, Gavin, Melville, Iyengar and Katon2010), which in turn are associated with the offspring's long-term disease-risk, including cardiovascular disease, obesity, diabetes and psychopathology (Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, MacDorman, Rasmussen, Qin and Lackritz2006; Eshete et al., Reference Eshete, Alemu and Zerfu2019).

We have argued elsewhere that allomothers may be adaptively motivated to offset prenatal risks to the offspring for inclusive fitness benefits, extending allomaternal care to the prenatal period (Figure 1A; Knorr & Fox, Reference Knorr and Fox2023). Thus, allomothers may buffer maternal prenatal psychosocial stressors because such stressors relate to offspring fitness. Our previous work found that greater levels of emotional social support from and communication with maternal grandmothers, over and above father involvement, was related to lower prenatal psychological distress for mothers. Here, we extend this argument beyond assessing if mothers’ allomother relationships correlate with reduced psychological distress to investigate if these relationships directly buffer maternal stressors (Figure 1B).

Figure 1. Panel A depicts a broader conceptual model connected to the overall state of the literature. There is an epidemiological trend of extrinsic stressors relating to altered birth outcomes (path A). This trend likely stems from stressors negatively influencing maternal psychological and physiological stress systems (path A1), which are biologically signaled through the placenta in a way that impacts fetal growth and development trajectories (path A2) that ultimately lead to altered birth outcomes like low birth weight (path A3). Overall, we suggest that allomothers may be motivated to buffer this cascade (path B1) for the benefit of the developing infant. Panel B depicts the conceptual model of this study. In this paper, we focus on how allomaternal relationship characteristics (particularly between grandmothers and mothers) buffers (B1) the relationship of extrinsic stressors and maternal distress (A1). This particular paper focuses on how the relationship of discrimination and psychological distress is moderated by allomother relationship characteristics. The abbreviations MGM and PGM stand for maternal and paternal grandmother, respectively.

A psychosocial stressor often studied in this pathway is racial/ethnic discrimination (henceforth ‘discrimination’), or how people are unjustly grouped and differentially treated by society. While race/ethnicity is not biological, issues related to discrimination have long been recognised for their association with increased psychological distress and physical health (Pascoe & Richman, Reference Pascoe and Richman2009; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Neighbors and Jackson2003). Depression was found to mediate the effect of discrimination on physical health among Latino immigrants (Cariello et al., Reference Cariello, Perrin, Williams, Espinoza, Paredes and Moreno2022). Recent work further suggests a causal link; for example, increases in discrimination over time are linked to higher rates of psychological distress and depression, but not the other way around (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Printz and DeLapp2018).

Here, we analyse data from a cohort of pregnant Latina women in Southern California to ask if allomothers buffer the prenatal psychological distress associated with discrimination. Latinos are the largest demographic group in California (Public Policy Institute of California, 2020), and often experience unique institutional barriers and psychosocial stressors. Institutional barriers (such as limited access to economic and social resources like higher education and equitable healthcare) and psychosocial stressors (such as discrimination and mistreatment in healthcare) are often due to institutional racism and cultural incompetency of people in power (American Psychiatric Association, 2017; Santos et al., Reference Santos, Adynski, Harris, Bhattacharya, Incollingo Rodriguez, Cali and Murgatroyd2021). For foreign-born Latinos, language barriers and concerns about deportation are also common psychosocial stressors (American Psychiatric Association, 2017).

Unfortunately, a salient experience for many Latinos living in the US is ethnic discrimination. Hence, we chose to focus on discrimination because of its strong implication in the DOHaD literature and the Latino-American experience. In a PEW research study conducted in 2021, 54% of Latinos reported experiencing some discrimination event in the last 12 months, with the number higher for certain sub-groups (PEW Research Center, 2021). Frequent perceptions of discrimination, including everyday discrimination, has been shown to be a significant source of stress (Williams & Mohammed, Reference Williams and Mohammed2009). Recent events in US politics have also led to increased discrimination and negative sentiment against Latinos as well as increased psychological distress among Latinos living in the US (Fox, Reference Fox2022; PEW Research Center, 2018). Examples of recent political events and circumstances that could detrimentally influence Latino mental health include negative statements against Mexican Americans and other Latino groups by US President Donald Trump during his 2016 campaign and term as president, the growth of White-nationalist terrorist groups, as well as increasingly aggressive rhetoric regarding immigration at the border leading to the inhumane treatment of Latino migrants at the US–Mexico border. Our research question is particularly meaningful for our cohort of Latina women owing to (1) the unique forms of discrimination in the US and (2) the high rates of perinatal mood disorders and adverse birth outcomes among Latinas.

Latina women experience rates of psychological distress 20% higher than non-Latina White women (CDC, 2019) and a disproportionate burden of depression during and after pregnancy. Latina women living in the US report elevated rates of postpartum depression compared with non-Latina White women (46.8% compared with 31.3%; Howell et al., Reference Howell, Mora, Horowitz and Leventhal2005). These disparities are similarly pronounced for depression during pregnancy (Gavin et al., Reference Gavin, Melville, Rue, Guo, Dina and Katon2011), although prenatal depression is studied less often than postnatal. Rates of maternal depression are also impacted by generational status; second-generation (US-born) Latina women experience substantially higher depression (44%) compared with first-generation (foreign-born) Latina women (34%; Huang et al., Reference Huang, Wong, Ronzio and Yu2007). More exposure to ethnic discrimination during pregnancy is associated with more depressive symptoms longitudinally across pregnancy among an ethnically diverse cohort (Noroña-Zhou et al., Reference Noroña-Zhou, Aran, Garcia, Haraden, Perzow, Demers and Davis2022), suggesting that pregnancy is a vulnerable period throughout gestation and that discrimination effects are cumulative.

Psychological distress can become embodied or ‘get under the skin’, which contributes to health disparities between minority and majority ethnic groups (Gravlee, Reference Gravlee2009). For pregnant women, embodiment of adverse experiences can influence stress physiology and, in turn, fetal development, birth timing and infant outcomes (Path A in our conceptual model, Figure 1A). Indeed, connections between discrimination and greater cortisol during pregnancy, lower offspring birth weight, shorter gestation length and greater stress reactivity in infants have been repeatedly observed (Carty et al., Reference Carty, Kruger, Turner, Campbell, DeLoney and Lewis2011; Collins et al., Reference Collins, David, Handler, Wall and Andes2004; Thayer et al., Reference Thayer, Bécares and Atatoa Carr2019) (Path A2). Previous studies cast discrimination as a robust predictor of Latina perinatal psychological health and the trajectories of health for the next generation (Paths A1 and A3). Specifically for Latinas, greater levels of discrimination were associated with elevated rates of depression and anxiety among 150 pregnant Latinas, at 24–32 weeks’ gestation and 4–6 weeks postpartum, while controlling for acculturation, acculturative stress and economic hardship (Santos et al., Reference Santos, Adynski, Harris, Bhattacharya, Incollingo Rodriguez, Cali and Murgatroyd2021). In a multisite cohort study of approximately 2000 Latina and Black women, those who experienced higher levels of discrimination were more likely to give birth prematurely compared with those who experienced discrimination less than once per year (Fryer et al., Reference Fryer, Vines and Stuebe2020). Discrimination during pregnancy has been associated with increased odds of low birth weight, mediated by depression among a two-thirds Latina cohort (Earnshaw et al., Reference Earnshaw, Rosenthal, Lewis, Stasko, Tobin, Lewis and Ickovics2013). Analysis of multi-generational birth records reveals that any advantages of foreign-born birth weight, compared with the non-Latino White majority, disappear in subsequent generations and disappear more rapidly among Black Americans than Latino Americans (Andrasfay & Goldman, Reference Andrasfay and Goldman2020; Collins et al., Reference Collins, Wu and David2002), further suggesting that it is the exposure to socio-economic disadvantage and discrimination that creates a birthweight disparity between ethnic groups. Thus, this particular cohort (Latina pregnant women) and psychosocial stressor (ethnic discrimination) are a well-documented backdrop against which to ask our allomothering research question.

Humans have a flexible roster of kin and non-kin who help the mother–child dyad (Kramer, Reference Kramer2010). Grandmothers are often critical allomothers perinatally because of their experience, knowledge and availability owing to their own non-reproductive phase of life. Most studies on grandmother inclusive fitness suggest that the benefits to grandchild survivorship are mostly apparent at weaning age (Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, O'Connell, Jones, Alvarez and Charnov1998; Meehan et al., Reference Meehan, Quinlan and Malcom2013). Other scholars have extended the window of critical grandmaternal care to the perinatal period including during breastfeeding, at birth and during pregnancy (Knorr & Fox, Reference Knorr and Fox2023; Myers et al., Reference Myers, Page and Emmott2021; Rosenberg & Trevathan, Reference Rosenberg and Trevathan2002; Scelza & Hinde, Reference Scelza and Hinde2019). While variation exists and is ecologically dependent (Hill & Hurtado, Reference Hill and Hurtado2009; Strassmann & Kurapati, Reference Strassmann and Kurapati2010), maternal grandmothers (MGMs) are associated with grandchild health benefits more consistently than paternal grandmothers (PGMs) (Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Lahdenperä, Pettay, Lynch and Lummaa2021; Coall & Hertwig, Reference Coall and Hertwig2010; Nenko et al., Reference Nenko, Chapman, Lahdenperä, Pettay and Lummaa2021; Sear & Mace, Reference Sear and Mace2008; Strassmann & Garrard, Reference Strassmann and Garrard2011). While the maternal grandfather may also play a support role during pregnancy, we chose to focus on the grandmothers owing to the shared reproductive experiences of women.

Motivated by the lack of research on allocare during pregnancy, we previously found that maternal, but not paternal, grandmother social support (specifically, emotional support) and communication in the same cohort had a direct and positive effect on pregnant women's mental health (Knorr and Fox, Reference Knorr and Fox2023). That was the first paper, to our knowledge, that took an overtly evolutionary lens on social support during pregnancy. This paper, in contrast, considers the stress-buffering potential of different allomothers during pregnancy.

Our research question is salient to our cohort of Latina women living in the US as many cultures within the ‘Latino’ ethnic category share a prioritisation to family in decision-making and deference domains – a concept known as familismo (Sabogal et al., Reference Sabogal, Marín, Otero-Sabogal, Marín and Perez-Stable1987). A longitudinal study among Mexican-American women living in Arizona found that values of familismo appeared to buffer the harmful effects of discrimination on depression, while neighbourhood cohesion did not (Curci et al., Reference Curci, Hernández, Winstone, Perez and Luecken2022). We therefore can focus on the role of family specifically over other social units (like neighbourhood). Emotional support from family has been shown to buffer psychological distress associated with high levels of everyday discrimination experienced by Asian Americans (Mossakowski & Zhang, Reference Mossakowski and Zhang2014). Among Mexican-American adults living in California, the negative physical health effects of discrimination were only seen among the group where instrumental social support was low; additionally, the number of family members in the US was also protective against the ill-effects of discrimination (Finch & Vega, Reference Finch and Vega2003). Based upon previous postnatal allomaternal research and our previous findings of a prenatal allomaternal effect, we predict that MGMs will exert a positive effect on maternal mental health, and the relationship of maternal discrimination with negative mental health will be attenuated for those mothers who have higher levels of MGM emotional support, communication, and geographic proximity (Pathway B1 in Figure 1). Similarly based on previous findings, we predict weaker effects for PGMs’ influence buffering maternal prenatal psychological distress.

We analyse the grandmother relationship characteristics of emotional support, communication and geographic proximity, respective to the pregnant mother, because they capture unique parts of the grandmother–mother relationship. Emotional support is a critical component of psychological stress-buffering (Turner & Marino, Reference Turner and Marino1994). Generally, the construct of social support measures the care provided by or potentially available from a known individual, which can come in many forms including emotional, informational and instrumental (Dunkel Schetter & Brooks, Reference Schetter, & Brooks, Reis and Sprecher2009). We chose an emotional support metric for several reasons. Firstly, previous studies have shown that instrumental and emotional support have different effects on perinatal outcomes (Bedaso et al., Reference Bedaso, Adams, Peng and Sibbritt2021; Emmott & Mace, Reference Emmott and Mace2015). Secondly, emotional support has been extensively tied to stress-buffering in pregnant cohorts (Bedaso et al., Reference Bedaso, Adams, Peng and Sibbritt2021; Seguin et al., Reference Seguin, Potvin, Stdenis and Loiselle1995), while instrumental support has been shown to buffer stress and improve emotional well-being during pregnancy only when emotional support was also present (Morelli et al., Reference Morelli, Lee, Arnn and Zaki2015). Additionally, our measure of social support is a subjective measure that asks how satisfied the pregnant mother is with the emotional support she receives. A subjective measure of support allows us to describe how much help a woman receives above or below expected amounts of emotional support. Therefore, the difference in how much maternal and paternal grandmothers may be expected to help is already included in the models as it is measured by the participant's own assessment.

During pregnancy, women exhibit unique psychological responses and sensitivities to stressors, justifying the need to examine this effect in the context of pregnancy (Slade et al., Reference Slade, Cohen, Sadler and Miller2009). Additionally, the relationships between discrimination, emotional support and mental health during pregnancy must be examined carefully because of the complex interactions between these constructs. For example, African-American pregnant women who reported greater experiences of discrimination (owing to race, gender, age and education) also reported lower levels of social support (Dailey, Reference Dailey2009; specifically, Dailey uses the ‘Interpersonal Relationship Inventory’ which assesses emotional support). In contrast, Giurgescu et al. (Reference Giurgescu, Zenk, Engeland, Garfield and Templin2017) find that while social support and discrimination were independently associated with psychological well-being in a cohort of 107 African-American pregnant women, there was no significant interaction of these two variables on psychological well-being. Social support in Giurgescu et al. is assessed by the Medical Outcomes Study Social Support Survey, which is built to be a general metric of many domains of social support but includes many emotional support items. Thus, more research is needed for understanding the influence of discrimination and various domains of social support on psychology in pregnancy among different groups of people.

Geographic proximity is a variable used in much of the classic grandmother literature to proxy grandmother involvement (Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, MacDorman, Rasmussen, Qin and Lackritz2006; Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Pettay, Lahdenperä and Lummaa2018; Engelhardt et al., Reference Engelhardt, Bergeron, Gagnon, Dillon and Pelletier2019; Eshete et al., Reference Eshete, Alemu and Zerfu2019; Kemkes-Grottenthaler, Reference Kemkes-Grottenthaler2005; Madrigal & Meléndez-Obando, Reference Madrigal and Meléndez-Obando2008; Voland & Beise, Reference Voland and Beise2002). Greater geographic proximity between grandmother–mother–offspring is usually associated with better mother–offspring outcomes (Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Pettay, Lahdenperä and Lummaa2018; Engelhardt et al., Reference Engelhardt, Bergeron, Gagnon, Dillon and Pelletier2019; Sear & Mace, Reference Sear and Mace2008; Strassmann & Garrard, Reference Strassmann and Garrard2011). In historical and non-market integrated societies (i.e. societies that practice foraging, pastoralism, or other forms of subsistence living), greater proximity is often a strong predictor of social support generally. However, in market-integrated societies, many types of social support do not depend on geographic location; for example, emotional, informational and financial support can be exchanged at great distances through phone or internet connections. Therefore, our study implementing a comparison of geographic proximity and communication allows us to see a clearer picture of grandmother–mother relationships since geographic proximity reflects physical availability and instrumental support and communication reflects emotional and informational support.

Greater levels of family communication among adult, immigrant Mexican women were associated with greater levels of perceived emotional support (Vega et al., Reference Vega, Kolody, Valle and Weir1991). However, communication itself is not inherently positive and can reflect and exacerbate negative relationships. Verbal conflict among grandmother–mother relationships has been associated with negative parenting and child behaviour outcomes (Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Mills-Koonce, Gustafsson, Cox and Investigators2012). We measure communication levels broadly, so both positive and negative allomother interactions are possible. We capture how communication may influence maternal psychology in either direction.

Latinos share high rates of both multigenerational residence (grandmothers and grandchildren living in the same home) (PEW Research Center, 2010) as well as, conversely, high rates of geographic separation owing to their being the largest immigrant group in the US (PEW Research Center, 2020). This built-in contrast of close and far proximity is a further boon to our study. Overall, our research question integrates DOHaD frameworks and evolutionary theory. At the same time, our question addresses how and through what means family networks support pregnant women during vulnerable periods and in vulnerable situations.

2. Methods

2.1. Cohort

This study emerges from Wave 1 of Mothers’ Cultural Experiences (MCE) study, a multi-site cohort among whom data were collected in 2016–2018 to answer research questions surrounding culture, stress and mental health. This project used an observational study design with self-report survey data from 361 pregnant and postpartum women. Women were approached in clinic waiting rooms across four sites in Southern California and asked if they met the following eligibility criteria: (1) self-identified as Latina, Chicana, Hispanic, or Mexican; (2) spoke English or Spanish; (3) were pregnant or recently postpartum; and (4) 18 years of age or older. Participants answered a 30–45-minute questionnaire independently and were compensated $10. Data is not publicly available because participants did not consent to sharing individual-level data publicly.

For this study, women who did not meet the eligibility criteria (N = 13) did not receive social support scales in their survey versions (N = 87), and those who were postnatal (N = 104) were removed before analysis leaving a final analytic cohort of N = 216 (some overlap of disqualifications). The 87 women who did not receive social support scales were the first women recruited in the MCE study, receiving Version 1.0 of the survey. Several measures were added with the implementation of Version 2.0 in order to maximise the number of constructs assessed. We dropped these earliest participants from analyses here in order to avoid non-random missingness. Since our research question involves expanding allocare research into the realm of pregnancy, we did not analyse data of postnatal participants.

2.2. Variable operationalisation

Predictor variable: discrimination

We used the validated Everyday Discrimination Scale as the primary predictor in both models, with instructions to consider each question in relation to their ethnicity/race (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Yu, Jackson and Anderson1997). This scale has been cross-analysed in different ethnic groups and genders (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Yang, Jacobs and Fitchett2012) finding overall similarities except differential item functioning for one to two items depending on ethnic group. Since all participants self-identified as part of the same ethnic group, differential item functioning is unlikely to be an issue here. Indeed, we find overall high reliability of the discrimination measure with an overall Cronbach alpha (α) of 0.90. This Cronbach α is 0.91 for English-speaking participants (αE) and 0.86 for Spanish-speaking participants (αS). For a break-down of the items and values in all validated scales used in this study, see the Supplementary Material.

Outcome variables: maternal prenatal mental health

The maternal mental health outcome variables are operationalised with the following validated questionnaire-based instruments: Edinburgh Perinatal Depression Scale (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987), Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory – Short Form (Marteau & Bekker, Reference Marteau and Bekker1992) and the Shortened Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983). We find the following reliabilities for depression α= 0.85 (αE = 0.86, αS = 0.84); state-anxiety α = 0.81 (αE = 0.84, αS = 0.77); and perceived stress α = 0.52 (αE = 0.86, αS = 0.33). Owing to the low PSS Cronbach's α in the Spanish version, post-hoc, we repeated the PSS models separately for the cohort subsets who took the survey in English and Spanish and did not find evidence of differential effects by language (see the Supplementary Material).

Predictor variables: allomother relationships characteristics

We define family roles based on each individual's relationship to the fetus. Although the relationships are not typically titled this way until after the offspring is born, for clarity we will describe relationships from the perspective of the fetus: MGM, father and PGM. Maternal grandmother in this study is the mother who raised the pregnant participant. While a biological relationship was not required for our analysis, only 3.3% of MGMs were reported as not the biological mother to our participants (Table 1). Similarly, we define the fetus's father as who the participant felt would be the main father figure; in all cases, this was either her current relationship partner or the biological father. In our cohort, 87% of participants were in a relationship, 95% of which were with the baby's father (Table 1). The PGM is who participants identified as their baby's father figure's mother. Instructions to the participants clarified that this might be the participant's mother-in-law, boyfriend's mother or someone else.

Table 1. Demographics of the study cohort and descriptive statistics of the measures used in this study. SD: standard deviation. See SM for explanation of clinically significant cut-off scores for depression.

To measure each allomother's emotional support, we used the validated Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (Zimet et al., Reference Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet and Farley1988), which measures an individuals’ general perception of acceptable levels of social support from different sources. For this study, we adapted the four-item family sub-scale of Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support to refer specifically to the baby's grandmothers and father. Therefore, participants were asked to rate social support from specific individuals on a three-point Likert rating on four statements (e.g. ‘I get the emotional help and support I need from my mother’), which was then averaged (Zimet et al., Reference Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet and Farley1988). Since half of the items directly reflect emotional support and the more general items did not change the mean scores when removed, we conclude that the scale is primarily a reflection of emotional support (see Supplementary Material in this paper and in (Knorr & Fox, Reference Knorr and Fox2023). Our data showed a high internal reliability for the emotional support scale adapted for the MGM α = 0.94 (αE = 0.94, αS = 0.93), PGM α = 0.94 (αE = 0.95, αS = 0.93) and father α = 0.97 (αE = 0.97, αS = 0.97).

Communication and geographic proximity were each operationalised based on one question: ‘How nearby does your [mother/baby's father/baby's father's mother] live?/¿Qué tan cerca vive su [madre/padre de su bebe/madre del padre de su bebé]?’ and ‘How often do you communicate with your [mother/baby's father/baby's father's mother]?/¿Qué tan seguido se comunica con su [madre/padre de su bebe/madre del padre de su bebé]?’ For each question, participants were offered a list of options. For communication, options were 1 = every day, 2 = more than once a week, 3 = more than once a month, 4 = once a month or less and 5 = never. We reverse-coded so that greater numbers indicated greater levels of communication. For geographic proximity, options were 1 = in my home, 2 = in my neighbourhood, 3 = outside my neighbourhood but close enough to visit during the day and 4 = too far to visit during the day. We reverse-coded this variable so greater numerical values were associated with greater levels of geographic proximity.

Control variables

We included six covariates in each model: father relationship characteristics (i.e. father emotional support, communication and geographic proximity), maternal age, trimester, parity, foreign-born status and a composite measure of socio-economic status (SES). We take into account father relationship characteristics in order to ensure that any significant PGM association was not just a proxy for father involvement. We do not use relationship status because of high collinearity with father relationship characteristics, which was a higher priority control variable. For SES, we made a composite variable, used elsewhere (Fox, Reference Fox2022; Knorr & Fox, Reference Knorr and Fox2023), that unitises and sums the MacArthur Subjective Socioeconomic Status Scale (Adler et al., Reference Adler, Epel, Castellazzo and Ickovics2000), maternal education (operationalised as ‘less than high school’, ‘high school or equivalent’ (examples of high school equivalency include receiving a General Education Diploma, which the US and Canada uses to certify attainment of knowledge equivalent to high school education) or ‘more than high school’ (including bachelor degrees or higher, some college, or a vocational degree)) and food security (2-Item Screen to Identify Families at Risk for Food Insecurity by Hager et al. (Reference Hager, Quigg, Black, Coleman, Heeren, Rose-Jacobs and Frank2010)). All other covariates used a single question (e.g. foreign-born status = ‘What country were you born in?/¿En que país nació?’). We control for SES, foreign-born status, maternal age, trimester and parity because these were suspected as possible confounders owing to previous studies demonstrating associations with mental health (Bottino et al., Reference Bottino, Nadanovsky, Moraes, Reichenheim and Lobato2012; Campos et al., Reference Campos, Schetter, Abdou, Hobel, Glynn and Sandman2008; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Tyrovolas, Koyanagi, Chatterji, Leonardi, Ayuso-Mateos and Haro2016). These covariates may all theoretically influence the relationship and availability of different alloparents as well (Supplemental Figure S2). Despite the high comorbidity of depression and anxiety (Hirschfeld, Reference Hirschfeld2001), we did not control for other mental health variables in our particular model because this could open backdoor paths and create confounding owing to collider variables (Supplemental Figure S1).

2.3. Statistical models

The overarching research question is: do allomothers buffer the association of stressors, such as discrimination, with maternal prenatal psychological distress? We answer this question with two sets of multiple linear regression models: (1) testing the association of discrimination and maternal prenatal psychological distress; and (2) testing how allomother relationship characteristics moderate the relationship between discrimination and maternal prenatal psychological distress. We expect the first set of models (henceforth, Set 1) to be significant as this is a well-established relationship in the literature. After replicating this finding in our dataset, we explore the second set of models (henceforth, Set 2) by adding an interaction term. This interaction term serves as a stress-buffering measure with three characteristics of the allomother–mother relationship: (a) emotional support, (b) communication and (c) geographic proximity. Each model in Set 2 assesses stress-buffering on three separate measures of psychological distress: depression, state-anxiety and perceived stress. All models control for the same list of covariates described above. All statistical models were conducted using RStudio and R version 4.0.3 and packages including tidyverse, ggplot2, mice, lmtest and car (Fox & Weisberg, Reference Fox and Weisberg2019; van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011; Wickham, Reference Wickham2016; Wickham et al., Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan, Chang, McGowan, François and Yutani2019; Zeileis & Hothorn, Reference Zeileis and Hothorn2002). For a complete list of packages used, see the code provided.

This study was pre-registered at https://osf.io/sn7e4.

Model Set 1: is discrimination is associated with psychological distress?

$${\rm Y}_{Psychological \;Distress\;} = \beta _0 + \beta _{1 \;\rm Discrimination} + \beta _{\rm i \; Covariates}$$

Model Set 2: do allomother relationship characteristics moderate the association of discrimination and mental health?

$${\rm Y}_{Psychological \;Distress} = \beta _0 + \beta _{1 \;\rm Discrimination}\;{\rm x}\; ( \beta _{2-4 \;\rm Relationship \;characteristics \;with \;each \;allomother}) + \beta _{\rm i \; Covariates}$$

2.4. Missingness, imputation, power analysis and regression diagnostics

The analytic dataset had 7% overall missingness. In order to preserve sample size, we used multiple chain imputation with the MICE package in R, which uses group-level relationships between variables to impute five complete datasets (Section 2 in the Supplementary Material and Figure S3). The data are probably missing at random because the analytic cohort does not include any individuals who have systemic missingness (for post-hoc analysis surrounding our missing at random assumption, see Section 2.1 of the Supplementary Material).

We conducted regression analysis on each of the five imputed datasets and then pooled the output. Our models have 12 predictors (four variables of interest, five control variables and three interaction variables). With a sensitivity analysis for F-tests in G*Power (v3.1) and our parameters set to an α-error of 0.05 and a sample size of 216, our models can detect effect sizes down to 0.084 with 80% power (a medium effect size). Regression diagnostics, including Breusch–Pagan tests and variation inflation factor calculations, were run iteratively on all the models. These diagnostics are included in the Supplementary Material (Supplementary Tables S7–9). From these tests, we decided to use robust standard errors in all models to account for heteroscedasticity and remain conservative in our estimates.

3. Results

The women in our cohort were, on average, 29 years old (18–45 years old, standard deviation 6.12), in a relationship (87%), parous (63.9%), in their third trimester (60.6%) and educated with high school equivalency (66%) or less (14%) (Table 1). There was variation in the perceived levels of discrimination and psychological distress reported in this cohort, with high rates of self-reported depression (16.7%) and anxiety (22.7%) similar to what has been reported for Latina pregnant and postpartum women in other studies (Ponting et al., Reference Ponting, Mahrer, Zelcer, Dunkel Schetter and Chavira2020). Additionally, Latinos disproportionately experience food insecurity in the US (16–22% compared with the national average of 11–14%; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Crowder, Rodriguez, Redwine and Stern2021), although we find even higher levels of reported food insecurity among our cohort (38%).

3.1. Discrimination

In the first set of models, discrimination was a significant predictor of all three psychological distress measures. As predicted, higher self-reported levels of ethnic discrimination were associated with higher maternal depression (pooled β, 2.58; robust SE, 0.49; p-value < 0.001), state-anxiety (pooled β, 0.22; robust SE, 0.06; p-value < 0.001) and perceived stress (pooled β, 1.30; robust SE, 0.27; p-value < 0.001) (Table 2; Figure 2). Since these measures are all on unique scales the betas cannot be compared across models.

Table 2. Regression Results of Model Set 1 - The Relationship of Ethnic Discrimination and Prenatal Psychological Distress

The relationship of reported levels of everyday ethnic discrimination (row 2) on maternal depression, state-anxiety, and perceived stress (columns 1-3, respectively), holding certain covariates constant (row 3-7). Each cell contains the pooled beta, with stars indicating significance level (***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05) and pooled robust standard errors in the parentheses. Each model was run on a sample of N = 216. R2, the pooled coefficient of determination indicating how much variation in mental health is explained by the predictor and control variables, is also presented. Model comparison calculated from 5 imputed data sets against their respective null models produced the following pooled (F-statistics; p-values): depression (5.959; <0.0001), state anxiety (3.704; 0.001), perceived stress (5.314; <0.0001).

Figure 2. This figure shows the relationship between ethnic discrimination and depression (panel A), state anxiety (panel B), and perceived stress (panel C). The blue line represents the calculated beta slope from the regression model, while the gray shaded region represents the 95% confidence intervals of the estimates. These plots show non-pooled beta slopes from regressions using imputed dataset 2. We chose one dataset randomly, for clarity. The dots show up darker if there are multiple participants occupying that space.

3.2. Grandmother stress-buffering

In the second set of models, we evaluate how allomother relationship characteristics moderate the association between discrimination and psychological distress. As predicted, the interaction of MGM emotional support with discrimination moderated the relationship of discrimination and depression (pooled β, −1.61; robust SE, 0.73; p-value, 0.03) and of discrimination and anxiety (pooled β, −0.28; robust SE, 0.10; p-value, 0.005) (Table 3; Figure 3). Additionally, the interaction of MGM communication with discrimination was a significant moderator of discrimination and depression (pooled β, −0.78; robust SE, 0.39; p-value, 0.045), anxiety (pooled β, −0.14; SE, 0.05; p-value, 0.006), and stress (pooled β, −0.47; SE, 0.22; p-value, 0.035) (Table 4 and Figure 3). The PGM was not a significant moderator of any discrimination–psychological distress relationship. Geographic proximity of allomothers was not a significant buffer for any discrimination–psychological distress relationship (Table 5; Figure 3).

Table 3. Regression Results of Model Set 2 - How the interaction between emotional support and ethnic discrimination relates to prenatal psychological distress

The moderation of emotional support from allomothers on ethnic discrimination (row 11-13) on depression, state-anxiety, and perceived stress (column 1-3, respectively), holding certain main effects (rows 2-5) and covariates (rows 6-10) constant. Each cell contains a pooled beta, with stars indicating the significance level (*** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05) and robust standard errors in the parentheses. Each model was run on a sample of N = 216. R2 is also presented. Model comparisons calculated from 5 imputed data sets against their respective null models produced the following pooled F-statistics and p-values (respectively) for depression (5.760; <0.0001), state-anxiety (4.503; <0.0001), and perceived stress (3.563; <0.0001).

Figure 3. The How the interaction between allomother relationship characteristics and ethnic discrimination relates to prenatal psychological distress

Table 4. Regression Results of Model Set 2 - How the interaction between communication and ethnic discrimination relates to prenatal psychological distress

The moderation of communication from allomothers on ethnic discrimination (row 11-13) on depression, state-anxiety, and perceived stress (column 1-3, respectively), holding certain main effects (rows 2-5) and covariates (rows 6-10) constant. Each cell contains a pooled beta, with stars indicating the significance level (*** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05) and robust standard errors in the parentheses. Each model was run on a sample of N = 216. R2 is presented. Model comparisons calculated from 5 imputed data sets against their respective null models produced the following pooled F-statistics and p-values (respectively) for depression (3.669; <0.0001), state-anxiety (2.655; 0.005), and perceived stress (2.788; 0.003).

Table 5. Regression Results of Model Set 2 - How the interaction between geographic proximity and discrimination relates to prenatal psychological distress

The relationship of discrimination (row 2) on depression, state-anxiety, and perceived stress (columns 1-3, respectively), holding certain covariates constant (row 3-7). Each cell contains the pooled beta, with stars indicating significance level (*** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05) and pooled robust standard errors in the parentheses. R2, the pooled coefficient of determination indicating how much variation in mental health is explained by the predictor and control variables, is also presented. Each model is run on a sample size of 216. Model comparison calculated from 5 imputed data sets against their respective null models produced the following pooled (F-statistics; p-values): depression (1.302, 0.230), state anxiety 1.039; 0.406), perceived stress (1.528; 0.133).

4. Discussion

Motivated by the epidemiological trends tying stressors to low-birth weight and preterm birth, we evaluate whether grandmothers buffer the psychological distress pregnant mothers experience from the stressor of racial/ethnic discrimination (henceforth discrimination) (Figure 1). In previous studies of postnatal allomothers, MGMs are shown to have a particular importance in offsetting the energetic expence of motherhood by providing childcare and direct provisioning of grandchildren during weaning (Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, O'Connell, Jones, Alvarez and Charnov1998; Meehan et al., Reference Meehan, Quinlan and Malcom2013). Grandmaternal allocare also occurs during the prenatal period (Knorr & Fox, Reference Knorr and Fox2023). Here, we show that MGM can directly buffer the negative psychological response to stress during pregnancy. For Model Set 1, we replicate previous findings that discrimination is a significant stressor implicated in self-reported levels of depression, anxiety and perceived stress among pregnant women (Giurgescu et al., Reference Giurgescu, Zenk, Engeland, Garfield and Templin2017; Mukherjee et al., Reference Mukherjee, Trepka, Pierre-Victor, Bahelah and Avent2016; Santos et al., Reference Santos, Adynski, Harris, Bhattacharya, Incollingo Rodriguez, Cali and Murgatroyd2021). For Model Set 2, we find that the relationship between maternal prenatal psychological distress and discrimination was moderated by greater levels of emotional support and communication with MGMs over and above any buffering effects of fathers. These results are consistent with the plausibility of the theoretical model presented in Figure 1, where MGMs are engaging in stress-buffering activities for pregnant mothers, potentially to improve the birth outcomes and later-in-life fitness outcomes of grandoffspring.

Research on grandmothers as critical allomothers often operationalises MGM and PGM help through proxies like grandmaternal survival, co-residence or geographic proximity, as these studies are most often conducted with historical records (e.g. Kemkes-Grottenthaler, Reference Kemkes-Grottenthaler2005; Madrigal & Meléndez-Obando, Reference Madrigal and Meléndez-Obando2008; Voland & Beise, Reference Voland and Beise2002). Our work improves upon these studies by measuring a particular aspect of grandmother support more explicitly in addition to proxy variables like geographic proximity. This is similar to other postnatal studies that have also explored multiple domains of grandmothering (Emmott & Mace, Reference Emmott and Mace2015; Myers et al., Reference Myers, Page and Emmott2021; Scelza & Hinde, Reference Scelza and Hinde2019; Sheppard & Sear, Reference Sheppard and Sear2016).

We predicted weaker effects for PGMs based on previous research. Consistent with this prediction, we find no evidence of PGM stress-buffering. This may be due to the absence of an effect or simply our analyses being underpowered to detect a smaller effect size compared with that of MGMs. The magnitude of each effect might be different for MGMs vs. PGMs given differences in expectations of help. Our emotional support measure is a subjective measure that asks women if they are satisfied with the level of support they are receiving from this person, so these differences in expectations of help are already included in the participant's assessment of social support. In contrast, geographic proximity and communication are objective measures, allowing us to harness both perspectives to compare different allomothers. Maternal grandmothers may be more consistent buffers of stress because mother–daughter relationships reflect a lifelong intimacy that is not comparable with mother-in-law relationships or even romantic relationships. Other cultural reasons may also account for this difference.

Since ethnic discrimination is such a rampant and distinct problem in large-scale, diverse societies like the US, these results may also be relevant to the minority health research discourse. Family relationships, specifically MGM–mother relationships, may play a particular role among Latina mothers, contributing to resilience against the ill effects of discrimination. This is an especially important public health application given that discrimination has been associated with adverse birth outcomes (Earnshaw et al., Reference Earnshaw, Rosenthal, Lewis, Stasko, Tobin, Lewis and Ickovics2013; Fryer et al., Reference Fryer, Vines and Stuebe2020). Adverse birth outcomes, like low birthweight and preterm birth, are frequent causes of infant morbidity and mortality (Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, MacDorman, Rasmussen, Qin and Lackritz2006; Eshete et al., Reference Eshete, Alemu and Zerfu2019). The developmental origins of health and disease framework explains how maternal psychological distress may modulate stress-related biochemical processes in utero, leading to adverse birth outcomes and affecting the offspring's lifespan disease-risk (including cardiovascular disease, obesity, diabetes, and psychopathology) (Thornburg & Marshall, Reference Thornburg and Marshall2015).

Family is a critical aspect to resilience. Individuals who report higher levels of familismo values tend to have greater levels of social support (as measured by the Medical Outcomes Study, a validated social support scale which reflects emotional support) as well as reduced risk of affective disorders (Campos et al., Reference Campos, Schetter, Abdou, Hobel, Glynn and Sandman2008). Our findings suggest that communication and emotional support are more critical buffers of discrimination than geographic proximity. This work suggests that encouraging strong social ties to community and extended family is important, supporting a broader anthropological discussion that an exclusively mother–father family unit is not better or even best (Sear, Reference Sear2016). While in-person MGM care may be critical for instrumental support, these results show that maintaining positive relationships over the phone or internet could also have real and meaningful benefits.

5. Limitations

Our data does not include measures of fitness, such as number of children, grandchildren or offspring survivorship, so we make no claim of testing fitness. Additionally, our cross-sectional and observational design does not allow us to draw causal inferences. We also are limited by the scales used; for example, the discrimination scale does not ask about cumulative exposure to ethnic discrimination. Nor do we ask about intersectional experiences of discrimination, such as discrimination owing to gender or weight. Both cumulative exposures and the intersectional nature of identity can alter the experience of discrimination. We use the term ‘Latina’ to describe people who have been grouped together by wider socio-political power structures in addition to a specific set of cultural identities and experiences. Within this term, there exist many cultures and lifestyles, which we do not claim to fully capture.

While we suggest that emotional support is decoupled from geographic proximity and can act independently, we did not test whether certain types of remote communication were more effective for transmitting emotional support compared to others.

6. Conclusions

This study finds that discrimination is a significant stressor implicated in self-reported levels of depression, anxiety and perceived stress among pregnant Latina women. Additionally, we observe that MGMs buffer mothers’ psychological response to stress during pregnancy. This suggests that MGMs are likely participating in prenatal stress-buffering activities for mothers. Future work may connect these prenatal activities to improved birth outcomes and later-in-life fitness outcomes of grandoffspring through longitudinal study designs and the inclusion of infant outcome measures.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2023.27

Acknowledgements

The authors want to share a deep appreciation to the study participants and their families as well as the students and volunteers of the MCE study, Kristine Chua and Kotrina Kajokaite. We thank Maridet Ibanez and the team at Women's Infants and Children in Santa Ana CA, Celia Bernstein and the team at Westside Family Health Center, Lirona Katzir and the team at Olive View–UCLA Medical Center, and Yvette Bojorquez and the team at MOMS Orange County, as well as Patricia Greenfield, Gail Greendale, Rachel Brook, Janet P. Pregler, Laura M. Glynn, Curt A. Sandman and Chris Dunkel Schetter.

Author contributions

D.K. and M.F. conceived and designed the study. D.K. conducted the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. M.F. edited the manuscript and acquired funding.

Financial support

This study was funded by NIH National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases K01 DK105110 and R03 DK125524 to M.F., and the UCLA Center for the Study of Women Faculty Research Grant to M.F. D.K. was funded by the UCLA Institute of Kindness and the Institute of American Cultures. D.K. also acknowledges the CCPR infrastructure grant from NICHD: P2C HD041022 for work-space.

Conflicts of interest

Authors D.K. and M.F. declare none.

Research transparency and reproducibility

This paper was pre-registered at https://osf.io/sn7e4. For the statistical code, software, or other questions please contact the corresponding author.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are not available because individuals only consented to sharing pooled results in publications, and not individual responses. The R code without the data is provided.

References

Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, White women. Health Psychology, 19(6), 586592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2017). Mental health disparities: Hispanics and Latinos. Council on Minority Mental Health and Health Disparities. https://www.psychiatry.org/File%20Library/Psychiatrists/Cultural-Competency/Mental-Health-Disparities/Mental-Health-Facts-for-Hispanic-Latino.pdfGoogle Scholar
Andrasfay, T., & Goldman, N. (2020). Intergenerational change in birthweight: Effects of foreign-born status and race/ethnicity. Epidemiology, 31(5), 649658. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0000000000001217Google Scholar
Barnett, M. A., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gustafsson, H., Cox, M., & Investigators, F. L. P. K. (2012). Mother–grandmother conflict, negative parenting, and young children's social development in multigenerational families. Family Relations, 61(5), 864877. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3729.2012.00731.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedaso, A., Adams, J., Peng, W., & Sibbritt, D. (2021). The relationship between social support and mental health problems during pregnancy: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Reproductive Health, 18(1), 162. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12978-021-01209-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottino, M. N., Nadanovsky, P., Moraes, C. L., Reichenheim, M. E., & Lobato, G. (2012). Reappraising the relationship between maternal age and postpartum depression according to the evolutionary theory: Empirical evidence from a survey in primary health services. Journal of Affective Disorders, 142(1), 219224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.030Google Scholar
Callaghan, W. M., MacDorman, M. F., Rasmussen, S. A., Qin, C., & Lackritz, E. M. (2006). The Contribution of preterm birth to infant mortality rates in the United States. Pediatrics, 118(4), 15661573. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2006-0860CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campos, B., Schetter, C. D., Abdou, C. M., Hobel, C. J., Glynn, L. M., & Sandman, C. A. (2008). Familialism, social support, and stress: Positive implications for pregnant Latinas. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14(2), 155162. https://doi.org/10.1037/1099-9809.14.2.155Google Scholar
Cariello, A. N., Perrin, P. B., Williams, C. D., Espinoza, G. A., Paredes, A. M., & Moreno, O. A. (2022). Moderating influence of social support on the relations between discrimination and health via depression in Latinx immigrants. Journal of Latina/o Psychology, 10(2), 98111. https://doi.org/10.1037/lat0000200Google Scholar
Carty, D. C., Kruger, D. J., Turner, T. M., Campbell, B., DeLoney, E. H., & Lewis, E. Y. (2011). Racism, health status, and birth outcomes: Results of a participatory community-based intervention and health survey. Journal of Urban Health, 88(1), 8497. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11524-010-9530-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
CDC (2019). Mental and behavioral health – Hispanics – The Office of Minority Health. https://minorityhealth.hhs.gov/omh/browse.aspx?lvl=4&lvlid=69Google Scholar
Chapman, S. N., Lahdenperä, M., Pettay, J. E., Lynch, R. F., & Lummaa, V. (2021). Offspring fertility and grandchild survival enhanced by maternal grandmothers in a pre-industrial human society. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 3652. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-83353-3Google Scholar
Chapman, S. N., Pettay, J. E., Lahdenperä, M., & Lummaa, V. (2018). Grandmotherhood across the demographic transition. PLOS ONE, 13(7), e0200963. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200963Google Scholar
Coall, D. A., & Hertwig, R. (2010). Grandparental investment: Past, present, and future. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09991105Google Scholar
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, J. W., David, R. J., Handler, A., Wall, S., & Andes, S. (2004). Very low birthweight in African American infants: The role of maternal exposure to interpersonal racial discrimination. American Journal of Public Health, 94(12), 21322138. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.94.12.2132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, J. W., Wu, S.-Y., & David, R. J. (2002). Differing intergenerational birth weights among the descendants of US-born and foreign-born Whites and African Americans in Illinois. American Journal of Epidemiology, 155(3), 210216. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/155.3.210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 150, 782786. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782Google Scholar
Curci, S. G., Hernández, J. C., Winstone, L. K., Perez, M., & Luecken, L. J. (2022). Discrimination and depressive symptoms Among Mexican American women: Exploring multilevel sociocultural moderators. Clinical Psychological Science, 21677026221129628. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026221129628Google ScholarPubMed
Dailey, D. E. (2009). Social stressors and strengths as predictors of infant birth weight in low-income African American women. Nursing Research, 58(5), 340347. https://doi.org/10.1097/NNR.0b013e3181ac1599CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schetter, Dunkel, & Brooks, C., K. (2009). The nature of social support. In Reis, H. T. & Sprecher, S. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of human relationships (pp. 15651570). Sage.Google Scholar
Earnshaw, V. A., Rosenthal, L., Lewis, J. B., Stasko, E. C., Tobin, J. N., Lewis, T. T., … Ickovics, J. R. (2013). Maternal experiences with everyday discrimination and infant birth weight: A test of mediators and moderators among young, urban women of color. Annals of Behavioral Medicine : A Publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, 45(1), 1323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-012-9404-3Google Scholar
Emmott, E. H., & Mace, R. (2015). Practical support from fathers and grandmothers is associated with lower levels of breastfeeding in the UK Millennium Cohort Study. PLOS ONE, 10(7), e0133547. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0133547Google Scholar
Engelhardt, S. C., Bergeron, P., Gagnon, A., Dillon, L., & Pelletier, F. (2019). Using geographic distance as a potential proxy for help in the assessment of the grandmother hypothesis. Current Biology, 29(4), 651-656.e3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.027Google Scholar
Eshete, A., Alemu, A., & Zerfu, T. A. (2019). Magnitude and risk of dying among low birth weight neonates in rural Ethiopia: A community-based cross-sectional study. International Journal of Pediatrics, 2019, 9034952. https://doi.org/10.1155/2019/9034952Google Scholar
Finch, B. K., & Vega, W. A. (2003). Acculturation stress, social support, and self-rated health among Latinos in California. Journal of Immigrant Health, 5(3), 109117. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1023987717921Google Scholar
Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2019). An R companion to applied regression (3rd ed.). Sage.Google Scholar
Fox, M. (2022). How demographics and concerns about the Trump administration relate to prenatal mental health among Latina women. Social Science & Medicine, 307, 115171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115171Google Scholar
Freeman, A., Tyrovolas, S., Koyanagi, A., Chatterji, S., Leonardi, M., Ayuso-Mateos, J. L., … Haro, J. M. (2016). The role of socio-economic status in depression: Results from the COURAGE (aging survey in Europe). BMC Public Health, 16(1), 1098. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3638-0Google Scholar
Fryer, K. E., Vines, A. I., & Stuebe, A. M. (2020). A multisite examination of everyday discrimination and the prevalence of spontaneous preterm birth in African American and Latina women in the United States. American Journal of Perinatology, 37(13), 13401350. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0039-1693696Google ScholarPubMed
Gavin, A. R., Melville, J. L., Rue, T., Guo, Y., Dina, K. T., & Katon, W. J. (2011). Racial differences in the prevalence of antenatal depression. General Hospital Psychiatry, 33(2), 8793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2010.11.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giurgescu, C., Zenk, S. N., Engeland, C. G., Garfield, L., & Templin, T. N. (2017). Racial discrimination and psychological wellbeing of pregnant women. MCN. The American Journal of Maternal Child Nursing, 42(1), 813. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMC.0000000000000297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluckman, P. D., Hanson, M. A., Cooper, C., & Thornburg, K. L. (2008). Effect of in utero and early-life conditions on adult health and disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 359(1), 6173. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra0708473Google Scholar
Gravlee, C. C. (2009). How race becomes biology: Embodiment of social inequality. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 139(1), 4757. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20983CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigoriadis, S., Graves, L., Peer, M., Mamisashvili, L., Tomlinson, G., Vigod, S. N., … Richter, M. (2018). Maternal anxiety during pregnancy and the association with adverse perinatal outcomes: Systematic review and meta-analysis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 79(5), 00. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.17r12011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigoriadis, S., VonderPorten, E. H., Mamisashvili, L., Tomlinson, G., Dennis, C.-L., Koren, G., … Radford, K. (2013). The impact of maternal depression during pregnancy on perinatal outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 74(4), 00. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.12r07968Google Scholar
Grote, N. K., Bridge, J. A., Gavin, A. R., Melville, J. L., Iyengar, S., & Katon, W. J. (2010). A Meta-analysis of depression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(10), 10121024. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hager, E. R., Quigg, A. M., Black, M. M., Coleman, S. M., Heeren, T., Rose-Jacobs, R., … Frank, D. A. (2010). Development and validity of a 2-item screen to identify families at risk for food insecurity. Pediatrics, 126(1), e26e32. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-3146CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., Jones, N. G. B., Alvarez, H., & Charnov, E. L. (1998). Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 13361339. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.1336Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). Cooperative breeding in South American hunter–gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1674), 38633870. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1061Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, R. M. A. (2001). The comorbidity of major depression and anxiety disorders: Recognition and management in primary care. Primary Care Companion to The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 3(6), 244254.Google ScholarPubMed
Howell, E. A., Mora, P. A., Horowitz, C. R., & Leventhal, H. (2005). Racial and ethnic differences in factors associated with early postpartum depressive symptoms. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 105(6), 14421450. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.AOG.0000164050.34126.37CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Z. J., Wong, F. Y., Ronzio, C. R., & Yu, S. M. (2007). Depressive symptomatology and mental health help-seeking patterns of U.S.- and foreign-born mothers. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 11(3), 257267. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-006-0168-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemkes-Grottenthaler, A. (2005). Of grandmothers, grandfathers and wicked step-grandparents. Differential impact of paternal grandparents on grandoffspring survival. Historical Social Research, 30(3). https://doi.org/10.12759/HSR.30.2005.3.219-239Google Scholar
Knorr, D. A., & Fox, M. (2023). An evolutionary perspective on the association between grandmother–mother relationships and maternal mental health among a cohort of pregnant Latina women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 44(1), 3038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.10.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, K. L. (2010). Cooperative breeding and its significance to the demographic success of humans. Annual Review of Anthropology, 39(1), 417436. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.012809.105054CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, T. T., Yang, F. M., Jacobs, E. A., & Fitchett, G. (2012). Racial/ethnic differences in responses to the everyday discrimination scale: A differential item functioning analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology, 175(5), 391401. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwr287Google Scholar
Madrigal, L., & Meléndez-Obando, M. (2008). Grandmothers’ longevity negatively affects daughters’ fertility. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 136(2), 223229. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20798CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marteau, T. M., & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31(3), 301306. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb00997.xGoogle Scholar
Meehan, C. L., Quinlan, R., & Malcom, C. D. (2013). Cooperative breeding and maternal energy expenditure among aka foragers. American Journal of Human Biology, 25(1), 4257. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.22336Google Scholar
Morelli, S. A., Lee, I. A., Arnn, M. E., & Zaki, J. (2015). Emotional and instrumental support provision interact to predict well-being. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 15(4), 484493. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000084Google Scholar
Mossakowski, K. N., & Zhang, W. (2014). Does social support buffer the stress of discrimination and reduce psychological distress among Asian Americans? Social Psychology Quarterly, 77(3), 273295. https://doi.org/10.1177/0190272514534271Google Scholar
Mukherjee, S., Trepka, M. J., Pierre-Victor, D., Bahelah, R., & Avent, T. (2016). Racial/ethnic disparities in antenatal depression in the United States: A systematic review. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 20(9), 17801797. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-1989-xGoogle Scholar
Myers, S., Page, A. E., & Emmott, E. H. (2021). The differential role of practical and emotional support in infant feeding experience in the UK. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1827), rstb.2020.0034, 20200034. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0034Google Scholar
Nenko, I., Chapman, S. N., Lahdenperä, M., Pettay, J. E., & Lummaa, V. (2021). Will Granny save me? Birth status, survival, and the role of grandmothers in historical Finland. Evolution and Human Behavior, 42(3), 239246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.11.002Google Scholar
Noroña-Zhou, A., Aran, Ö., Garcia, S. E., Haraden, D., Perzow, S. E. D., Demers, C. H., … Davis, E. P. (2022). Experiences of discrimination and depression trajectories over pregnancy. Women's Health Issues, 32(2), 147155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.whi.2021.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascoe, E. A., & Richman, L. S. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 531554. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016059Google Scholar
PEW Research Center (2010). The return of the multi-generational family household. https://www.pewresearch.org/social-trends/2010/03/18/the-return-of-the-multi-generational-family-household/Google Scholar
PEW Research Center (2018, October 25). Latinos’ experiences with discrimination. https://www.pewresearch.org/hispanic/2018/10/25/latinos-and-discrimination/Google Scholar
PEW Research Center (2021). Many Americans continue to experience mental health difficulties as pandemic enters second year. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2021/03/16/many-americans-continue-to-experience-mental-health-difficulties-as-pandemic-enters-second-year/Google Scholar
Ponting, C., Mahrer, N. E., Zelcer, H., Dunkel Schetter, C., & Chavira, D. A. (2020). Psychological interventions for depression and anxiety in pregnant Latina and Black women in the United States: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 27(2), 249265. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2424Google Scholar
Public Policy Institute of California (2020). California's Population. Public Policy Institute of California. https://www.ppic.org/publication/californias-population/Google Scholar
Rodriguez, C., Crowder, S. L., Rodriguez, M., Redwine, L., & Stern, M. (2021). Food insecurity and the Hispanic population during the COVID-19 pandemic. Ecology of Food and Nutrition, 60(5), 548563. https://doi.org/10.1080/03670244.2021.1974014Google Scholar
Rosenberg, K., & Trevathan, W. (2002). Birth, obstetrics and human evolution. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics & Gynaecology, 109(11), 11991206. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1471-0528.2002.00010.xGoogle Scholar
Sabogal, F., Marín, G., Otero-Sabogal, R., Marín, B. V., & Perez-Stable, E. J. (1987). Hispanic familism and acculturation: What changes and what doesn't? Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 9(4), 397412. https://doi.org/10.1177/07399863870094003Google Scholar
Santos, H. P., Adynski, H., Harris, R., Bhattacharya, A., Incollingo Rodriguez, A. C., Cali, R., … Murgatroyd, C. (2021). Biopsychosocial correlates of psychological distress in Latina mothers. Journal of Affective Disorders, 282, 617626. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.193Google Scholar
Scelza, B. A., & Hinde, K. (2019). Crucial contributions: A biocultural study of grandmothering during the perinatal period. Human Nature, 30(4), 371397. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-019-09356-2Google Scholar
Sear, R. (2016). Beyond the nuclear family: An evolutionary perspective on parenting. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 98103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.013Google Scholar
Sear, R., & Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seguin, L., Potvin, L., Stdenis, M., & Loiselle, J. (1995). Chronic stressors, social support, and depression during pregnancy. Obstetrics & Gynecology, 85(4), 583589. https://doi.org/10.1016/0029-7844(94)00449-NCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheppard, P., & Sear, R. (2016). Do grandparents compete with or support their grandchildren? In Guatemala, paternal grandmothers may compete, and maternal grandmothers may cooperate. Royal Society Open Science, 3(4). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160069Google Scholar
Slade, A., Cohen, L. J., Sadler, L. S., & Miller, M. (2009). The psychology and psychopathology of pregnancy: Reorganization and transformation. In Handbook of infant mental health (3rd ed., pp. 2239). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Strassmann, B. I., & Garrard, W. M. (2011). Alternatives to the grandmother hypothesis: A meta-analysis of the association between grandparental and grandchild survival in patrilineal populations. Human Nature, 22(1–2), 201222. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-011-9114-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strassmann, B. I., & Kurapati, N. T. (2010). Are humans cooperative breeders?: Most studies of natural fertility populations do not support the grandmother hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(1), 3539. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09991749CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thayer, Z., Bécares, L., & Atatoa Carr, P. (2019). Maternal experiences of ethnic discrimination and subsequent birth outcomes in Aotearoa New Zealand. BMC Public Health, 19(1), 1271. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7598-zGoogle Scholar
Thornburg, K. L., & Marshall, N. (2015). The placenta is the center of the chronic disease universe. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 213(4 Suppl), S14-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.08.030Google Scholar
Turner, R. J., & Marino, F. (1994). Social support and social structure: A descriptive epidemiology. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35(3), 193212. https://doi.org/10.2307/2137276Google Scholar
van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(3). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v045.i03Google Scholar
Vega, W., Kolody, B., Valle, R., & Weir, J. (1991). Social networks, social support, and their relationship to depression among immigrant Mexican women. Human Organization, 50(2), 154162. https://doi.org/10.17730/humo.50.2.p340266397214724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voland, E., & Beise, J. (2002). Opposite effects of maternal and paternal grandmothers on infant survival in historical Krummhörn. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 52(6), 435443. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-002-0539-2Google Scholar
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis (2nd ed.). Springer International. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L., François, R., … Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686Google Scholar
Williams, D. R., & Mohammed, S. A. (2009). Discrimination and racial disparities in health: Evidence and needed research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32(1), 2047. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D. R., Neighbors, H. W., & Jackson, J. S. (2003). Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: Findings from community studies. American Journal of Public Health, 93(2), 200208. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.93.2.200Google Scholar
Williams, D. R., Yu, Y., Jackson, J. S., & Anderson, N. B. (1997). Racial differences in physical and mental health: Socioeconomic status, stress, and discrimination. Journal of Health Psychology, 2(3), 335351.Google Scholar
Williams, M. T., Printz, D. M. B., & DeLapp, R. C. T. (2018). Assessing racial trauma with the Trauma Symptoms of Discrimination Scale. Psychology of Violence, 8(6), 735747. https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000212Google Scholar
Zeileis, A., & Hothorn, T. (2002). Diagnostic checking in regression relationships. R News, 2(3), 710.Google Scholar
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52(1), 3041. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Panel A depicts a broader conceptual model connected to the overall state of the literature. There is an epidemiological trend of extrinsic stressors relating to altered birth outcomes (path A). This trend likely stems from stressors negatively influencing maternal psychological and physiological stress systems (path A1), which are biologically signaled through the placenta in a way that impacts fetal growth and development trajectories (path A2) that ultimately lead to altered birth outcomes like low birth weight (path A3). Overall, we suggest that allomothers may be motivated to buffer this cascade (path B1) for the benefit of the developing infant. Panel B depicts the conceptual model of this study. In this paper, we focus on how allomaternal relationship characteristics (particularly between grandmothers and mothers) buffers (B1) the relationship of extrinsic stressors and maternal distress (A1). This particular paper focuses on how the relationship of discrimination and psychological distress is moderated by allomother relationship characteristics. The abbreviations MGM and PGM stand for maternal and paternal grandmother, respectively.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographics of the study cohort and descriptive statistics of the measures used in this study. SD: standard deviation. See SM for explanation of clinically significant cut-off scores for depression.

Figure 2

Table 2. Regression Results of Model Set 1 - The Relationship of Ethnic Discrimination and Prenatal Psychological Distress

Figure 3

Figure 2. This figure shows the relationship between ethnic discrimination and depression (panel A), state anxiety (panel B), and perceived stress (panel C). The blue line represents the calculated beta slope from the regression model, while the gray shaded region represents the 95% confidence intervals of the estimates. These plots show non-pooled beta slopes from regressions using imputed dataset 2. We chose one dataset randomly, for clarity. The dots show up darker if there are multiple participants occupying that space.

Figure 4

Table 3. Regression Results of Model Set 2 - How the interaction between emotional support and ethnic discrimination relates to prenatal psychological distress

Figure 5

Figure 3. The How the interaction between allomother relationship characteristics and ethnic discrimination relates to prenatal psychological distress

Figure 6

Table 4. Regression Results of Model Set 2 - How the interaction between communication and ethnic discrimination relates to prenatal psychological distress

Figure 7

Table 5. Regression Results of Model Set 2 - How the interaction between geographic proximity and discrimination relates to prenatal psychological distress

Supplementary material: File

Knorr and Fox supplementary material 1

Knorr and Fox supplementary material
Download Knorr and Fox supplementary material 1(File)
File 718.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Knorr and Fox supplementary material 2

Knorr and Fox supplementary material
Download Knorr and Fox supplementary material 2(File)
File 3.7 MB
Supplementary material: File

Knorr and Fox supplementary material 3

Knorr and Fox supplementary material
Download Knorr and Fox supplementary material 3(File)
File 2.9 MB