Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T09:01:50.611Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Childhood adversity and psychosis: generalised or specific effects?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2015

E. Longden*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
M. Sampson
Affiliation:
Early Intervention Services, Auckland District Health Board, Auckland, New Zealand
J. Read
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr E. Longden, Institute of Psychology, Health and Society, Block B 2nd Floor, Waterhouse Building, University of Liverpool, Liverpool L69 3GL, UK. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background.

This study examines relationships between childhood adversity and the presence of characteristic symptoms of schizophrenia. It was hypothesised that total adversity exposures would be significantly higher in individuals exhibiting these symptoms relative to patients without. Recent proposals that differential associations exist between specific psychotic symptoms and specific adversities was also tested, namely: sexual abuse and hallucinations, physical abuse and delusions, and fostering/adoption and delusions.

Method.

Data were collected through auditing 251 randomly selected medical records, drawn from adult patients in New Zealand community mental health centres. Information was extracted on presence and subtype of psychotic symptoms and exposure to ten types of childhood adversity, including five types of abuse and neglect.

Results.

Adversity exposure was significantly higher in patients experiencing hallucinations in general, voice hearing, command hallucinations, visions, delusions in general, paranoid delusions and negative symptoms than in patients without these symptoms. There was no difference in adversity exposure in patients with and without tactile/olfactory hallucinations, grandiose delusions or thought disorder. Indication of a dose–response relationship was detected, in that total number of adversities significantly predicted total number of psychotic symptoms. Although fostering/adoption was associated with paranoid delusions, the hypothesised specificity between sexual abuse and hallucinations, and physical abuse and delusions, was not found. The two adversities showing the largest number of associations with psychotic symptoms were poverty and being fostered/adopted.

Conclusions.

The current data are consistent with a model of global and cumulative adversity, in which multiple exposures may intensify psychosis risk beyond the impact of single events. Implications for clinical intervention are discussed.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015

Exposure to childhood loss, stress and victimisation has been extensively studied as a risk factor for psychosis, with meta-analyses reporting odds ratios of between 2.78 (Varese et al. Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer, Read, van Os and Bentall2012) and 3.60 (Matheson et al. Reference Matheson, Shepherd, Pinchbeck, Laurens and Carr2013) for multiple forms of adversity. This relationship has been replicated across different populations and study designs, with further confirmation emanating from consistent findings of a dose–response; that is, an increase in risk according to number or severity of exposures. For example, analysis of two large community samples found evidence of a dose–response effect for cumulative trauma exposure (childhood neglect, physical abuse, physical attack or assault, rape, sexual molestation) and psychosis likelihood (Shevlin et al. Reference Shevlin, Houston, Dorahy and Adamson2008); while a more recent population-based household survey reported that childhood physical or sexual abuse plus incidence of other adverse life events (e.g., bereavement, serious accident or injury, witnessing violence) combine ‘synergistically’ to increase psychosis risk beyond the effect of each adversity individually (Morgan et al. Reference Morgan, Reininghaus, Reichenberg, Frissa, Hotopf and Hatch2014).

Several propositions have been made for the mechanisms by which cumulative adversity confers psychosis risk, including the suggestion that exposures create vulnerability to psychotic experience through toxic effects on biological (Read et al. Reference Read, Fosse, Moskowitz and Perry2014), cognitive (Gracie et al. Reference Gracie, Freeman, Green, Garety, Kuipers, Hardy, Ray, Dunn, Bebbington and Fowler2007) and affective (Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, Schreier, Zammit, Maughan, Munafò, Lewis and Wolke2013) systems, which in turn may be amplified by additive environmental stressors over time. In this respect, while research has generally prioritised factors such as childhood sexual abuse (CSA), physical abuse (CPA) and physical neglect (CPN), greater attention is now being paid to other forms of adversity. For example, recent meta-analyses have proposed a heightened risk of psychotic symptoms in association with such factors as peer bullying (van Dam et al. Reference Van Dam, van der Ven, Velthorst, Selten, Morgan and De Haan2012), parental communication deviance (de Sousa et al. Reference De Sousa, Varese, Sellwood and Bentall2014) and urbanicity (Vassos et al. Reference Vassos, Pedersen, Murray, Collier and Lewis2012). Empirical work with large cross-sectional datasets have likewise emphasised the potential role of deprivation and social inequalities (Wickham et al. Reference Wickham, Taylor, Shevlin and Bentall2014) and being raised in institutional care (Bentall et al. Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012) in predicting psychotic symptoms, as well as the role of attachment quality in mediating between adversity exposure and subsequent psychosis (Sitko et al. Reference Sitko, Bentall, Shevlin and Sellwood2014).

In addition to aggregate adversity and disadvantage, research has also examined whether a degree of specificity may exist between particular types of childhood adversity and particular symptoms of psychosis. For example, analysis of the UK 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey found that childhood rape was associated with an increased risk of hallucinations (but not paranoid delusions) when controlling for other adversities and psychotic symptoms; whereas paranoia was specifically associated with CPA and being raised in institutional care when adjusting for adversity exposure and co-occurring hallucinations (Bentall et al. Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012). Analysis of the US National Comorbidity Survey (Sitko et al. Reference Sitko, Bentall, Shevlin and Sellwood2014) likewise reported differential associations between hallucinations and CSA, and paranoid beliefs and neglect (conceived by the authors as comparable with institutional care, in that a failure to adequately meet the child's emotional, physical or intellectual needs is indicative of attachment disruption and can induce the same severe impact as physical separation). Another study (Shevlin et al. Reference Shevlin, McAnee, Bentall and Murphy2014), which analysed data from 3142 UK prison inmates, likewise found that CSA produced the highest odds ratio for hallucinations (2.37) with paranoia specifically predicted by childhood bullying (1.99) and being raised in institutional care (1.49). The associations remained stable when controlling for prison-based adversity exposure, suggesting that they are not confounded by the experience of substantial adulthood adversity. These investigations are notable for their large samples and statistical adjustment for potential confounders. However, less controlled observational research has also found that CSA survivors may be more likely to report hallucinations, particularly auditory, relative to delusions (Read & Argyle, Reference Read and Argyle1999; Hainsworth et al. Reference Hainsworth, Starling, Brand, Groen and Munro2011; Sheffield et al. Reference Sheffield, Williams, Blackford and Heckers2013).

Taken together, this literature reflects a wider conceptual shift in psychosis research that advocates ‘complaint-orientated’ (Bentall, Reference Bentall2006) or ‘staging and profiling’ (Wigman et al. Reference Wigman, van Os, Thiery, Derom, Collip, Jacobs and Wichers2013) approaches. That is, that phenomenon such as hallucinations and delusions can be independently examined in their own right, rather than subsuming their study within the context of diagnostic categories such as ‘schizophrenia,’ which is a heterogeneous, disjunctive construct with poor reliability (Read, Reference Read, Read and Dillon2013a ). Refining accounts of specific associations between adversity exposures and outcomes is an important endeavour in terms of promoting better understandings of how particular risk factors might impact on different biological and psychological mechanisms to create a psychosis pathway. For example, Bentall & Fernyhough (Reference Bentall and Fernyhough2008) posit that paranoia is connected with heightened threat expectancy and a propensity to attribute adverse events to external sources; which are psychological mechanisms suggested to logically result from disempowerment (e.g., CPA) and disrupted attachment relationships (e.g., institutional care). In terms of voice hearing, these authors also suggest that formative adversity, particularly CSA, may hinder the source-monitoring mechanisms required to differentiate between external and self-generated stimuli, possibly in combination with adversity-induced dissociation. Although precise adversity-related and symptom-specific accounts of psychosis are still provisional, improved knowledge of these mechanisms could offer substantial clinical implications in terms of tailoring both pharmacological (e.g., ‘rational drug design’ targeted at component symptoms of psychosis: Fibiger, Reference Fibiger2012) and psychosocial (e.g., addressing specific processes such as dissociation or threat salience: Bentall et al. Reference Bentall, de Sousa, Varese, Wickham, Sitko, Haarmans and Read2014).

Study aims

The aim of the current study was to examine relationships between a broad range of childhood adversities (including the usual five main types of abuse and neglect, but also the less commonly researched variable of fostering/adoption) and the presence of DSM-IV characteristic symptoms of schizophrenia: hallucinations, delusions, thought disorder, negative symptoms and catatonia. It was hypothesised that the number of childhood adversity exposures would be significantly higher in individuals exhibiting these symptoms relative to patients who did not display signs of psychosis. In addition, it was hypothesised that specific associations would be identified between CSA and hallucinations; and between CPA, fostering/adoption (as an attachment disruption) and delusions.

Method

Procedure

Data were collected through reviewing electronic medical records of 251 adult service-users drawn from four urban community mental health centres (CMHCs) in New Zealand. Records were randomly selected from a computer-generated pool of 850 potential files. Files reporting no face-to-face contact with staff, or only face-to-face contact in a crisis scenario (e.g., a police station) or with non-CMHC staff were excluded. Files active for less than 3 days were also excluded on the grounds that a full assessment with a healthcare worker was unlikely to have taken place. A total of 141 files were omitted based on these criteria. All retained files were read in their entirety. Data were extracted by a Clinical Psychology graduate trainee (MS) and a registered clinical psychologist (JR) and documented on a specifically designed recording instrument that included: demographic characteristics; primary DSM-IV diagnosis; subtype and content of characteristic symptoms of DSM-IV criteria schizophrenia; and exposure to 13 different types of childhood adversity (CSA, CPA, CPN, emotional abuse [CEA], emotional neglect [CEN], bullying, poverty, fostering/adoption, death of a parent/caregiver, witnessing domestic violence, mental illness in a parent/caregiver, alcohol or substance use of a parent/caregiver, divorce of parents/caregivers). Childhood adversity was classified as that occurring prior to age 18.

Reliability

Owing to the observational nature of the data, operational definitions of the types of adversity examined were primarily based on that identified by clinicians and clients. For example, records stating ‘sexually abused as a child’ were considered sufficient to code for abuse having occurred. Files in which life history sections had not been completed were noted as ‘no abuse history taken’ and marked as missing data. If information suggestive of adversity was considered inconclusive, files were independently inspected by two researchers (MS and JR). To be included in the analysis, cases had to be rated as ‘95% or more probable’ to have occurred by both raters. Thirty-two files were judged to contain ambiguous information, in which agreement was reached in 29 cases (inter-rater reliability 91%, κ = 0.81). Examples of cases in which abuse was rated as <95% likely to have occurred included the statement ‘violent and abusive father,’ on the grounds that the description was vague and did not ascertain whether the father was abusive towards the client specifically, and ‘reported traumatic childhood’ with no clarifying details. An example of cases rated as 95% or more likely to have occurred stated ‘father began to drink heavily and took up the use of severe and frequent corporal punishment’ and ‘made to watch sexual activities as a child.’ In total, adversities in 14 of the 32 ambiguous files were rated as highly likely by both researchers and retained in the analysis.

The same criterion of 95% certainty estimation was adopted for coding psychotic symptoms. Information in 13 files was considered ambiguous, for which independent inter-rater agreement (EL and JR) was 100%. Examples of excluded data included ‘mildly grandiose – entitlement and her being ‘special’’ and ‘talks about conspiracy theories’ (for delusions), ‘racing thoughts that are difficult to stop’ and ‘rambling thoughts’ (for thought disorder) and ‘talk[ed] of often hearing people climbing over the fence at home but when she looks no one is there’ (for hallucinations). All 13 of these ambiguous cases were excluded from the analysis.

Data from the recording forms were manually entered into SPSS v.20.0 (IBM Corp., 2011) for analysis. To minimise data entry errors, 50 files (20.0%) were selected at random and double-checked against the hard-copy data collection from. Data entry agreement was 99.7%.

Statistical analysis

Between-group differences in clinical presentation and adversity exposure were assessed using Mann–Whitney U-tests to correct for the unequal group sizes and irregular data distributions. However, parametric statistics were used for descriptive summaries as they have closer correspondence with real world values than their non-parametric counterparts. Corresponding effect sizes were calculated using Cohen's r. Associations between specific clinical presentations and specific adversities were analysed using the phi-coefficient and unadjusted odds ratios. Associations between total adversity exposures and comorbidity for psychotic symptoms were assessed using bivariate linear regression.

Three of the 13 childhood adversity variables had more than 50% missing values and were not retained (bullying, witnessing domestic violence and alcohol or substance use of a parent/caregiver), leaving ten types of events for analysis: CSA, CPA, CPN, CEA, CEN, poverty, fostering/adoption, death of a parent/caregiver, mental illness of a parent/caregiver and divorce of parents/caregivers. Because of the large number of planned comparisons, alpha was set at a more stringent p ≤ 0.025 level to reduce the likelihood of type 1 (false positive) error.

Results

Participants

Demographic and clinical features of the sample are presented in Table 1. Participants consisted of 122 women and 129 men, with a mean age of 35.7 years (s.d.: 12.36). The majority were either New Zealand European (55.3%) or Māori (23.9%), and were commonly single (48.6%) and either unemployed or in receipt of sickness benefits (52.2%). The most frequent diagnoses were either mood (45.4%) or psychotic (23.1%) disorders.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Prevalence and characteristics of childhood adversities

Of the examined files, 141 (56.2%) reported at least one form of childhood abuse or neglect, the most common of which was CPA (89; 35.5%), followed by CEA (88; 35.1%), CSA (82; 32.7%), CEN (53; 21.1%) and CPN (18; 7.2%). At least one of the remaining five adversities were documented in 175 (69.7%) files, of which mental illness in a parent/caregiver was most prevalent (102; 40.6%) followed by divorce of parents/caregivers (100; 39.8%); fostering/adoption (38; 15.1%); poverty (31; 12.4%) and death of a parent/caregiver (28; 11.2%).

Prevalence and characteristics of psychotic symptoms

The mean number of psychotic symptoms reported across the sample was 3.47 (s.d.: 1.46). Of the charts examined, at least one form of hallucination was noted in 119 cases (47.4%). Auditory hallucinations were the most common modality (114; 95.8%: 45.4% of whole sample), of which 38 (31.9%; 15.1% of whole sample) were command hallucinations. Visions (48; 40.3%: 19.1% of whole sample) and tactile/olfactory hallucinations (15; 12.6%: 6.0% of whole sample) were less commonly reported. At least one type of delusion was noted in 110 cases (43.8%), which were more likely to be paranoid (104; 94.5%: 41.4% of whole sample) than grandiose (40; 36.4%: 15.9% of whole sample). Thought disorder was recorded in 59 cases (23.5%) followed by 47 instances of negative symptoms (18.7%). Catatonic symptoms were noted in only nine cases (3.6%), and were therefore not included in inferential analyses.

Group differences in adversity exposure

The total number of adverse childhood events was compared for each psychotic symptom and subtype, using patients without these symptoms as the reference group. The number of adversities was significantly higher in patients reporting hallucinations in general, voice hearing, visions, command hallucinations, delusions in general, paranoid delusions and negative symptoms (see Table 2). Using Cohen's r criteria (Reference Cohen1988), wherein ≥0.5 is considered a large effect, these differences were associated with moderate to small effect sizes. There were no significant differences in childhood adversity exposure in patients with and without thought disorder, tactile/olfactory hallucinations or grandiose delusions.

Table 2. Group differences and associated effect sizes in mean number of childhood adversity exposures in participants with and without psychotic symptoms

Significance level: *p ≤ 0.025; **p < 0.005; ***p < 0.001.

To determine whether combined adversity exposures increased the likelihood of higher comorbidity, a bivariate linear regression was conducted using total number of psychotic symptoms as the criterion variable and total number of adversities as the predictor. The equation was significant (F(1,159) = 14.92, p = 0.001) and indicated that patients with a higher number of adversity exposures reported a greater amount of psychotic symptoms (regression equation: 1.05 + 0.28 (× total adversities); R 2 = 0.09).

Associations between specific adversities and specific psychotic symptoms

Table 3 presents associations and respective odds ratios and confidence intervals between psychotic symptoms and adversity exposures. Contrary to predictions, there was no significant association between CSA and any hallucination subtype, or CPA and either type of delusion. However, the hypothesis that exposure to fostering/adoption would increase the likelihood of experiencing paranoid delusions was supported. The association between the latter and grandiose delusions also approached significance, but did not meet the adjusted alpha level set for this study (p = 0.028).

Table 3. Phi-coefficients (φ) and significant odds ratios between specific psychotic symptoms and specific childhood adversities

CSA, childhood sexual abuse; CPA, childhood physical abuse; CEA, childhood emotional abuse; CEN, childhood emotional neglect; CPN, childhood physical neglect; F/A, fostering/adoption; P, poverty; PD, death of a parent/caregiver; D, divorce of parents/caregivers; MI, mental illness in a parent/caregiver; NS, not significant.

Significance level: *p ≤ 0.025; ** p < 0.005; ***p < 0.001.

CSA, CEN, CPN, death of a parent/caregiver, divorce of parents/caregivers and mental illness in a parent/caregiver were not specifically associated with any psychotic symptoms. CPA was associated with an increased probability of reporting hallucinations in general, command hallucinations and negative symptoms; and CEA was associated with delusions, in general, paranoid delusions and negative symptoms.

The two adversities with the largest number of significant associations were poverty and fostering/adoption, which both showed the same specific relationships with hallucinations in general, voice hearing, command hallucinations, paranoid delusions and negative symptoms.

Discussion

The data confirm existing evidence that increased childhood adversity exposure is related to more severe psychiatric outcomes in adulthood, including psychosis (e.g., Varese et al. Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer, Read, van Os and Bentall2012), and that this association follows a dose-dependent pattern (e.g., Shevlin et al. Reference Shevlin, Houston, Dorahy and Adamson2008). However, although fostering/adoption was significantly associated with paranoid delusions, no relationship was found between CSA and any type of hallucination, or CPA and paranoid delusions. Thus the hypothesis (e.g., Bentall et al. Reference Bentall, Wickham, Shevlin and Varese2012; Shevlin et al. Reference Shevlin, McAnee, Bentall and Murphy2014; Sitko et al. Reference Sitko, Bentall, Shevlin and Sellwood2014) that differential associations exist between these particular adversities and psychotic symptoms was only partially supported.

Contrary to the specificity model, the current data are more consistent with a model of global cumulative adversity, in that number of exposures was significantly higher in patients experiencing hallucinations in general, voice hearing, command hallucinations, visions, delusions in general, paranoid delusions and negative symptoms (although not tactile/olfactory hallucinations, grandiose delusions or thought disorder) than patients without these symptoms; and that greater adversity exposure was associated with greater comorbidity.

The suggestion that combinations of different adversities might intensify psychosis risk beyond the impact of individual stressors has recently been examined in two large epidemiological studies. The first, a population-based survey of 1680 individuals in the UK, reported strong evidence for cumulative, ‘synergistic’ effects of abuse and adversity that were associated with a two- to fourfold increase of reporting low-level psychotic experience in the year preceding assessment (Morgan et al. Reference Morgan, Reininghaus, Reichenberg, Frissa, Hotopf and Hatch2014). The second, a combined sample for the Dutch NEMESIS studies (n = 13 722), likewise found strong, significant associations between childhood adversity and hallucinations, delusions, voice hearing and paranoia (van Nierop et al. Reference Van Nierop, Lataster, Smeets, Gunther, van Zelst, ten Have, van Dorsselaer, Bak, Myin-Germeys, Viechtbauer, van Os and van Winkel2014). However, when specific associations between CPA, CSA and foster care were examined using mixed-effects regression (capable of determining specific associations amongst multiple correlated outcomes), no differential relationships for delusions and hallucinations were found. Although both these studies were conducted within the general population, our random sample of psychiatric service-users yields a broadly comparable pattern of results in that more significant associations were found for multiple adversity exposure rather than specific associations between particular events and particular clinical outcomes.

It may therefore be notable that the two adversities in the current analysis with the largest number of significant associations with psychotic symptoms – poverty and fostering/adoption – could be seen as proxies for a range of more general (and cumulative) environmental risks and disadvantages (Neil, Reference Neil2000; Read, Reference Read2010; Read et al. Reference Read, Johnstone, Taitimu, Read and Dillon2013). For example, requiring substitute parental care is indicative of dysfunction, loss or stress in the family of origin and is associated with poorer long-term outcomes for adult adjustment, wellbeing and self-sufficiency (Buehler et al. Reference Buehler, Orme, Post and Patterson2000). This result therefore lends further support to a growing body of literature indicating that attachment quality and disruptions in attachment behaviours (particularly that occurring in early childhood) can contribute to the development of adulthood psychosis (Read & Gumley, Reference Read and Gumley2008; Harder, Reference Harder2014; Sitko et al. Reference Sitko, Bentall, Shevlin and Sellwood2014).

Poverty has been shown to be a strong predictor of psychosis for more than 60 years (Read, Reference Read2010; Read et al. Reference Read, Johnstone, Taitimu, Read and Dillon2013). Like fostering/adoption, it is also strongly associated with a greater incidence of childhood maltreatment (Drake & Pandey, Reference Drake and Pandey1996; Gillham et al. Reference Gillham, Tanner, Cheyne, Freeman, Rooney and Lambie1998; Lee & Goerge, Reference Lee and Goerge1999) and chronic stress dysregulation (Evans & Kim, Reference Evans and Kim2007). Complex interactions have also been proposed between inequality, deprivation, stress, discrimination, mistrust and lack of social support, as predictors of affective and non-affective psychosis (Wickham et al. Reference Wickham, Taylor, Shevlin and Bentall2014). Although the cross-sectional and correlational nature of the current data prohibits any causal assumptions, the consistency between these two variables is striking in that both exhibited the same pattern of significant associations with hallucinations in general, voice hearing, command hallucinations, paranoid delusions and negative symptoms.

In contrast, we found few significant associations between any psychotic symptom and the five types of abuse and neglect: none for CSA, CEN, or CPN and only three for CPA and CEA. Given the substantial literature associating these experiences with psychosis (see Read, Reference Read, Read and Dillon2013b ), this appears to be an unexpected result. However, it perhaps becomes more explicable when considering that formative exposure to any of these events can create vulnerability and stress sensitisation (Read et al. Reference Read, Fosse, Moskowitz and Perry2014) that may augment psychosis risk over time through subsequent social stressors and hardships. For example, recent large-scale epidemiological studies have found that factors like social marginalisation (Boyda et al. Reference Boyda, McFeeters and Shevlin2014) and attachment quality (Sitko et al. Reference Sitko, Bentall, Shevlin and Sellwood2014) mediate associations between interpersonal adversity and psychotic symptoms. As noted by Morgan et al. (Reference Morgan, Reininghaus, Reichenberg, Frissa, Hotopf and Hatch2014) ‘In so far as adverse social experiences tend to cluster in individuals, families and neighbourhoods and persist over time, it is essential to move on from identifying specific social and environmental risk factors for psychosis to examine the impact of multiple exposures, how they interact and the mechanisms through which they exert their effects’ (p. 352). In this respect research that incorporates a broader spectrum of stressors beyond abuse and neglect into their analyses is an important endeavour.

Clinical implications

Despite guidelines emphasising the need to routinely assess adversity exposure in psychiatric service-users (National Health Service Confederation, 2008), research suggests such recommendations are often not implemented (Read et al. Reference Read, Hammersley and Rudegeair2007; Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, Craig, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan, Lappin, Hutchinson, Doody, Jones, McGuffin, Murray, Leff and Morgan2011; Hepworth & McGowan, Reference Hepworth and McGowan2013).While staff should not pre-suppose a history of maltreatment unless confirmed by the client, the current findings support the contention that clinicians should receive support and training for making routine evaluations for possible experiences of maltreatment. This is particularly important given the significant under-detection of posttraumatic stress in patients diagnosed with psychosis (Salyers et al. Reference Salyers, Evans, Bond and Meyer2004; Lommen & Restifo, Reference Lommen and Restifo2009; Mauritz et al. Reference Mauritz, Goossens, Draijer and van Achterberg2013), and that such individuals are less likely to receive an appropriate clinical response (e.g., trauma-focused interventions) relative to those with non-psychotic diagnoses (Agar & Read, Reference Agar and Read2002; Salyers et al. Reference Salyers, Evans, Bond and Meyer2004; Grubaugh et al. Reference Grubaugh, Zinzow, Paul, Egede and Frueh2011) especially in instances where healthcare workers have strong convictions about biogenetic aetiology (Read & Fraser, Reference Read and Fraser1998; Young et al. Reference Young, Read, Barker-Collo and Harrison2001; Agar & Read, Reference Agar and Read2002).

The feasibility and utility of non-pharmacological approaches to psychosis have only become an area of systematic research interest within the last few decades and, with the exception of CBT, robust evidence for their efficacy (e.g., Cochrane reviews) is limited. There is a clear need for well-defined therapeutic approaches that can address the sequalae of interpersonal adversity in patients diagnosed with psychosis, particularly given the frequency with which previous victimisation may manifest in the content and maintenance of positive symptoms (Hardy et al. Reference Hardy, Fowler, Freeman, Smith, Steel, Evans, Garety, Kuipers, Bebbington and Dunn2005; Raune et al. Reference Raune, Bebbington, Dunn and Kuipers2006; Thompson et al. Reference Thompson, Nelson, McNab, Simmons, Leicester, McGorry, Bechdolf and Yung2010; Falukozi & Addington, Reference Falukozi and Addington2012; Corstens & Longden, Reference Corstens and Longden2013). One promising approach (Read et al. Reference Read, Agar, Argyle and Aderhold2003) is combining trauma-focused therapeutic models (e.g., Herman, Reference Herman1992; Ross & Halpern, Reference Ross and Halpern2009; Bacon & Kennedy, Reference Bacon and Kennedy2014) with treatments that have established effectiveness in alleviating psychotic symptoms (e.g., acceptance and commitment therapy (Gaudiano & Herbert, Reference Gaudiano and Herbert2006), cognitive therapy (Morrison et al. Reference Morrison, Turkington, Pyle, Spencer, Brabban, Dunn, Christodoulides, Dudley, Chapman, Callcott, Grace, Lumley, Drage, Tully, Irving, Cummings, Byrne, Davies and Hutton2014), compassion-focused therapy (Braehler et al. Reference Braehler, Gumley, Harper, Wallace, Norrie and Gilbert2013), early intervention strategies (McGorry et al. Reference McGorry, Killackey and Yung2008) and Open Dialogue (Seikkula et al. Reference Seikkula, Alakare and Aaltonen2011)).

Despite the attractive parsimony of unique associations between particular symptoms and stressors, a model of cumulative adversity also reiterates the inherent difficulty of devising causal pathways from specific events to specific clinical outcomes. It is important that therapeutic approaches can accommodate such heterogeneity, namely by acknowledging the complex, often idiosyncratic, processes that result in psychosis, while also recognising the limitations of trying to create predictable pathways from psychosocial events to subjective manifestations of distress. Individualised interventions, such as psychological formulation (e.g., Johnstone & Dallos, Reference Johnstone and Dallos2006; British Psychological Society Division of Clinical Psychology, 2011; Longden et al. Reference Longden, Corstens, Escher and Romme2012), represent one such ‘bottom-up’ strategy that can tailor therapeutic responses to the unique combination of social and emotional conflicts experienced by a particular client.

Limitations

The current findings must be interpreted in view of the study limitations, most notably the nature of the data. Medical record auditing is ultimately reliant on what is documented by healthcare workers, and while adversity prevalence in the current study is comparable with that of existing reviews (Wurr & Partridge, Reference Wurr and Partridge1996; Read, Reference Read1998; Read et al. Reference Read, Agar, Argyle and Aderhold2003) it is likely that some cases were unidentified. This means that both patients with and without psychotic symptoms may have experienced higher adversity rates than are currently reported. This may particularly be the case for neglect which can be identified less frequently by care services, including psychiatric facilities (Horwath, Reference Horwath2007). For example, Rossiter et al. (Reference Rossiter, Byrne, Wota, Nisar, Ofuafor, Murray, Byrne and Hallahan2015) have reported that when comparing the content of clinical records with structured measurement (the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ): Bernstein & Fink, Reference Bernstein and Fink1998) emotional and physical neglect were, respectively, 4.7 and 8.9 times more likely to be detected using the CTQ.

Aside from the assumption that adversity had been disclosed, believed, and recorded by the assessing clinician, the research had no formal contingencies for assessing the validity of reported exposures on a case-by-case basis. Accounts of childhood adversity may be vulnerable to contamination from factors such as traumatic amnesia (Freyd, Reference Freyd1994) and re-interpretive biases (Lewinsohn & Rosenbaum, Reference Lewinsohn and Rosenbaum1987) in adulthood, which in psychosis may be further complicated by processes such as cognitive impairments and impaired reality-testing. Nevertheless, it should be noted that retrospective accounts of adversity among groups with complex mental health problems have proven sufficiently valid and reliable to justify the use of such data (Herman & Schatzow, Reference Herman and Schatzow1987; Dill et al. Reference Dill, Chu, Grob and Eisen1991; Meyer et al. Reference Meyer, Muenzenmaier, Cancienne and Struening1996; Goodman et al. Reference Goodman, Thompson, Weinfurt, Corl, Acker, Mueser and Rosenberg1999; Fisher et al. Reference Fisher, Craig, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan, Lappin, Hutchinson, Doody, Jones, McGuffin, Murray, Leff and Morgan2011) with one study reporting that erroneous reports of sexual victimisation are no different between patients diagnosed with schizophrenia and the general population (Darves-Bornoz et al. Reference Darves-Bornoz, Lemperiere, Degiovanni and Gaillard1995).

Classification of psychotic symptoms was likewise reliant on the assessment of healthcare workers, and therefore did not reflect judgements trained to standardised levels of reliability for DSM-IV criteria. However, while independent and blinded diagnostic assessment would have been preferable, the current results do retain ecological validity in that they reflect how symptoms are assessed and classified in real-world clinical practice.

Finally, the relatively small sample, as well as numerous missing data values, meant we were not able to control for co-occurring psychotic symptoms as part of the specificity analysis. In addition, the largely categorical data prohibited the use of more rigorous inferential statistical models. It should also be noted that the large number of analyses increased the probability of type one errors, although this was addressed by avoiding post hoc testing and adjusting alpha to a more stringent level.

Acknowledgements

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards:

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Agar, K, Read, J (2002). What happens when people disclose sexual or physical abuse to staff at a community mental health centre? International Journal of Mental Health Nursing 11, 7079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bacon, T, Kennedy, A (2014). Clinical perspectives on the relationship between psychosis and dissociation: utility of structural dissociation and implications for practice. Psychosis: Psychological, Social and Integrative Approaches 7, 8191.Google Scholar
Bentall, RP (2006). Madness explained: why we must reject the Kraepelinian paradigm and replace it with a ‘complaint-orientated’ approach to understanding mental illness. Medical Hypotheses 66, 220233.Google Scholar
Bentall, RP, Fernyhough, C (2008). Social predictors of psychotic experiences: specificity and psychological mechanisms. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34, 10091011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentall, RP, Wickham, S, Shevlin, M, Varese, F (2012). Do specific early-life adversities lead to specific symptoms of psychosis? A study from the 2007 the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 734740.Google Scholar
Bentall, RP, de Sousa, P, Varese, F, Wickham, S, Sitko, K, Haarmans, M, Read, J (2014). From adversity to psychosis: pathways and mechanisms from specific adversities to specific symptoms. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 49, 10111022.Google Scholar
Bernstein, DP, Fink, L (1998). Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A Retrospective Self-report: Manual. Psychological Corporation: New York.Google Scholar
Boyda, D, McFeeters, D, Shevlin, M (2014). Intimate partner violence, sexual abuse, and the mediating role of loneliness on psychosis. Psychosis: Psychological, Social and Integrative Approaches 7, 113.Google Scholar
Braehler, C, Gumley, AI, Harper, J, Wallace, S, Norrie, J, Gilbert, P (2013). Exploring change processes in compassion focused therapy in psychosis: results of a feasibility randomized controlled trial. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 52, 199214.Google Scholar
British Psychological Society Division of Clinical Psychology (2011). Good Practice Guidelines on the use of Psychological Formulation. A Report by the British Psychological Society Division of Clinical Psychology. British Psychological Society: Leicester.Google Scholar
Buehler, C, Orme, JG, Post, J, Patterson, DA (2000). The long-term correlates of family foster care. Children and Youth Services Review 22, 595625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
Corstens, D, Longden, E (2013). The origins of voices: links between voice hearing and life history in a survey of 100 cases. Psychosis: Psychological, Social and Integrative Approaches 5, 270285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darves-Bornoz, J-M, Lemperiere, T, Degiovanni, A, Gaillard, P (1995). Sexual victimization in women with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 30, 7884.Google Scholar
De Sousa, P, Varese, F, Sellwood, W, Bentall, RP (2014). Parental communication deviance and psychosis: a meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 756768.Google Scholar
Dill, DL, Chu, JA, Grob, MC, Eisen, SV (1991). The reliability of abuse history report: a comparison of two inquiry formats. Comprehensive Psychiatry 32, 166169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drake, B, Pandey, S (1996). Understanding the relationship between neighborhood poverty and specific types of child maltreatment. Child Abuse and Neglect 20, 10031018.Google Scholar
Evans, GW, Kim, P (2007). Childhood poverty and health cumulative risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychological Science 18, 953957.Google Scholar
Falukozi, E, Addington, J (2012). Impact of trauma on attenuated psychotic symptoms. Psychosis: Psychological, Social and Integrative Approaches 4, 203212.Google Scholar
Fibiger, HC (2012). Psychiatry, the pharmaceutical industry, and the road to better therapeutics. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 649650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, HL, Craig, TK, Fearon, P, Morgan, K, Dazzan, P, Lappin, J, Hutchinson, G, Doody, GA, Jones, PB, McGuffin, P, Murray, RM, Leff, J, Morgan, C (2011). Reliability and comparability of psychosis patients’ retrospective reports of childhood abuse. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, 546553.Google Scholar
Fisher, HL, Schreier, A, Zammit, S, Maughan, B, Munafò, MR, Lewis, G, Wolke, D (2013). Pathways between childhood victimisation and psychosis-like symptoms in the ALSPAC birth cohort. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 10451055.Google Scholar
Freyd, J (1994). Betrayal trauma: traumatic amnesia as an adaptive response to childhood abuse. Ethics and Behavior 4, 307330.Google Scholar
Gaudiano, BA, Herbert, JD (2006). Acute treatment of inpatients with psychotic symptoms using acceptance and commitment therapy: pilot results. Behaviour Research and Therapy 44, 415437.Google Scholar
Gillham, B, Tanner, G, Cheyne, B, Freeman, I, Rooney, M, Lambie, A (1998). Unemployment rates, single parent density, and indices of child poverty: their relationship to different categories of child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect 22, 7990.Google Scholar
Goodman, L, Thompson, K, Weinfurt, K, Corl, S, Acker, P, Mueser, KT, Rosenberg, SD (1999). Reliability of reports of violent victimization and posttraumatic stress disorder among men and women with serious mental illness. Journal of Traumatic Stress 12, 587599.Google Scholar
Gracie, A, Freeman, D, Green, S, Garety, PA, Kuipers, E, Hardy, A, Ray, K, Dunn, G, Bebbington, P, Fowler, D (2007). The association between traumatic experience, paranoia and hallucinations: a test of the predictions of psychological models. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 116, 280289.Google Scholar
Grubaugh, AL, Zinzow, HM, Paul, L, Egede, LE, Frueh, BC (2011). Trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults with severe mental illness: a critical review. Clinical Psychology Review 31, 883899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hainsworth, C, Starling, J, Brand, F, Groen, K, Munro, K (2011). Trauma and psychotic symptoms: data from a pediatric mental health inpatient unit. Journal of Traumatic Stress 24, 491494.Google Scholar
Harder, S (2014). Attachment in schizophrenia – implications for research, prevention, and treatment. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 11891193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, A, Fowler, D, Freeman, D, Smith, B, Steel, C, Evans, J, Garety, P, Kuipers, E, Bebbington, P, Dunn, G (2005). Trauma and hallucinatory experience in psychosis. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 193, 501507.Google Scholar
Hepworth, L, McGowan, L (2013). Do mental health professionals enquire about childhood sexual abuse during routine mental health assessment in acute mental health settings? A substantive literature review. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 20, 473483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, JL (1992). Trauma and Recovery: The Aftermath of Violence from Domestic Abuse to Political Terror. Basic Books: New York.Google Scholar
Herman, J, Schatzow, E (1987). Recovery and verication of memories of childhood sexual trauma. Psychoanalytic Psychology 4, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horwath, J (2007). The missing assessment domain: personal, professional and organizational factors influencing professional judgements when identifying and referring child neglect. British Journal of Social Work 37, 12851303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
IBM Corp. (2011). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 20.0. IBM Corp: Armonk, NY.Google Scholar
Johnstone, L, Dallos, R (2006). Formulation in Psychology and Psychotherapy: Making Sense of People's Problems. Routledge: London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, BJ, Goerge, RM (1999). Poverty, early childbearing, and child maltreatment: a multinomial analysis. Children and Youth Services Review 21, 755780.Google Scholar
Lewinsohn, PM, Rosenbaum, M (1987). Recall of parental behaviour by acute depressives, remitted depressives, and nondepressives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52, 611619.Google Scholar
Lommen, MJJ, Restifo, K (2009). Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Community Mental Health Journal 45, 485496.Google Scholar
Longden, E, Corstens, D, Escher, S, Romme, M (2012). Voice hearing in biographical context: a model for formulating the relationship between voices and life history. Psychosis: Psychological, Social and Integrative Approaches 4, 224234.Google Scholar
Matheson, SL, Shepherd, AM, Pinchbeck, RM, Laurens, KR, Carr, VJ (2013). Childhood adversity in schizophrenia: a systematic meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 43, 225238.Google Scholar
Mauritz, MW, Goossens, PJ, Draijer, N, van Achterberg, T (2013). Prevalence of interpersonal trauma exposure and trauma-related disorders in severe mental illness. European Journal of Psychotraumatology 4, 10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.19985. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.19985 Google Scholar
McGorry, PD, Killackey, E, Yung, A (2008). Early intervention in psychosis: concepts, evidence and future directions. World Psychiatry 7, 148156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, I, Muenzenmaier, K, Cancienne, J, Struening, E (1996). Reliability and validity of a measure of sexual and physical abuse histories among women with serious mental illness. Child Abuse and Neglect 20, 213219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, C, Reininghaus, U, Reichenberg, A, Frissa, S, Hotopf, M, Hatch, SL (2014). Adversity, cannabis use and psychotic experiences: evidence of cumulative and synergistic effects. British Journal of Psychiatry 204, 346353.Google Scholar
Morrison, AP, Turkington, D, Pyle, M, Spencer, H, Brabban, A, Dunn, G, Christodoulides, T, Dudley, R, Chapman, N, Callcott, P, Grace, T, Lumley, V, Drage, L, Tully, S, Irving, K, Cummings, A, Byrne, R, Davies, LM, Hutton, P (2014). Cognitive therapy for people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders not taking antipsychotic drugs: a single-blind randomised controlled trial. The Lancet 383, 13951403.Google Scholar
National Health Service Confederation (NHS) (2008). Briefing 162: Implementing National Policy on Violence and Abuse. NHS: London.Google Scholar
Neil, E (2000). The reasons why young children are placed for adoption: findings from a recently placed sample and a discussion of implications for subsequent identity development. Child and Family Social Work 5, 303316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raune, D, Bebbington, P, Dunn, G, Kuipers, E (2006). Event attributes and the content of psychotic experiences in first-episode psychosis. Psychological Medicine 36, 221230.Google Scholar
Read, J (1998). Child abuse and severity of disturbance among adult psychiatric inpatients. Child Abuse and Neglect 22, 359368.Google Scholar
Read, J (2010). Can poverty drive you mad? “Schizophrenia,” socio-economic status and the case for primary prevention. New Zealand Journal of Psychology 39, 719.Google Scholar
Read, J (2013 a). Does ‘schizophrenia’ exist? Reliability and validity. In Models of Madness: Psychological, Social and Biological Approaches to Psychosis (ed. Read, J and Dillon, J), pp. 4761. Routledge: Hove.Google Scholar
Read, J (2013 b). Childhood adversity and psychosis: from heresy to certainty. In Models of Madness: Psychological, Social and Biological Approaches to Psychosis (ed. Read, J and Dillon, J), pp. 249275. Routledge: Hove.Google Scholar
Read, J, Argyle, N (1999). Hallucinations, delusions and thought disorders among Adult psychiatric inpatients with a history of child abuse. Psychiatric Services 50, 14671472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Read, J, Fraser, A (1998). Staff response to abuse histories of psychiatric inpatients. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 32, 206213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Read, J, Gumley, A (2008). Can attachment theory help explain the relationship between childhood adversity and psychosis? Attachment 2, 135.Google Scholar
Read, J, Agar, K, Argyle, N, Aderhold, V (2003). Sexual and physical abuse during childhood and adulthood as predictors of hallucinations, delusions and thought disorder. Psychology and Psychotherapy 76, 122.Google Scholar
Read, J, Hammersley, P, Rudegeair, T (2007). Why, when and how to ask about childhood abuse. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment 13, 101110.Google Scholar
Read, J, Johnstone, M, Taitimu, M (2013). Psychosis, poverty and ethnicity. In Models of Madness: Psychological, Social and Biological Approaches to Psychosis (ed. Read, J and Dillon, J), pp. 191209. Routledge: Hove.Google Scholar
Read, J, Fosse, R, Moskowitz, A, Perry, B (2014). The traumagenic neurodevelopmental model of psychosis revisited. Neuropsychiatry 4, 6579.Google Scholar
Ross, CA, Halpern, N (2009). Trauma Model Therapy: A Treatment Approach for Trauma, Dissociation and Complex Comorbidity. Manitou Communications Inc: Texas.Google Scholar
Rossiter, A, Byrne, F, Wota, A, Nisar, Z, Ofuafor, T, Murray, I, Byrne, C, Hallahan, B (2015). Childhood trauma levels in individuals attending adult mental health services: an evaluation of clinical records and structured measurement of childhood trauma. Child Abuse and Neglect 110.Google Scholar
Salyers, MP, Evans, LJ, Bond, GR, Meyer, PS (2004). Barriers to assessment and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder and other trauma-related problems in people with severe mental illness: clinician perspectives. Community Mental Health Journal 40, 1731.Google Scholar
Seikkula, J, Alakare, B, Aaltonen, J (2011). The comprehensive Open-Dialogue approach in western Lapland: II. Long-term stability of acute psychosis outcomes in advanced community care. Psychosis: Psychological, Social and Integrative Approaches 3, 191204.Google Scholar
Sheffield, JM, Williams, LE, Blackford, JU, Heckers, S (2013). Childhood sexual abuse increases risk of auditory hallucinations in psychotic disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry 54, 10981104.Google Scholar
Shevlin, M, Houston, JE, Dorahy, MJ, Adamson, G (2008). Cumulative traumas and psychosis: an analysis of the national comorbidity survey and the British Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34, 193199.Google Scholar
Shevlin, M, McAnee, G, Bentall, RP, Murphy, J (2014). Specificity of association between adversities and the occurrence and co-occurrence of paranoia and hallucinations: evaluating the stability of childhood risk in an adverse adult environment. Psychosis: Psychological, Social and Integrative Approaches 113.Google Scholar
Sitko, K, Bentall, RP, Shevlin, M, Sellwood, W (2014). Associations between specific psychotic symptoms and specific childhood adversities are mediated by attachment styles: an analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey. Psychiatric Research 217, 202209.Google Scholar
Thompson, A, Nelson, B, McNab, C, Simmons, M, Leicester, S, McGorry, PD, Bechdolf, A, Yung, AR (2010) Psychotic symptoms with sexual content in the “ultra high risk” for psychosis population: frequency and association with sexual trauma. Psychiatry Research 177, 8491.Google Scholar
Van Dam, DS, van der Ven, E, Velthorst, E, Selten, JP, Morgan, C, De Haan, L (2012). Childhood bullying and the association with psychosis in non-clinical and clinical samples: a review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 42, 24632474.Google Scholar
Van Nierop, M, Lataster, T, Smeets, F, Gunther, N, van Zelst, C, ten Have, M, van Dorsselaer, S, Bak, M, Myin-Germeys, I, Viechtbauer, W, van Os, J, van Winkel, R (2014). Psychopathological mechanisms linking childhood traumatic experiences to risk of psychotic symptoms: analysis of a large, representative population-based sample. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, S123S130.Google Scholar
Varese, F, Smeets, F, Drukker, M, Lieverse, R, Lataster, T, Viechtbauer, W, Read, J, van Os, J, Bentall, RP (2012). Childhood trauma increases the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 661671.Google Scholar
Vassos, E, Pedersen, CB, Murray, RM, Collier, DA, Lewis, CM (2012). Meta-analysis of the association of urbanicity with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 11181123.Google Scholar
Wickham, S, Taylor, P, Shevlin, M, Bentall, RP (2014). The impact of social deprivation on paranoia, hallucinations, mania and depression: the role of discrimination social support, stress and trust. PLoS ONE 9, e105140.Google Scholar
Wigman, JT, van Os, J, Thiery, E, Derom, C, Collip, D, Jacobs, N, Wichers, M (2013). Psychiatric diagnosis revisited: towards a system of staging and profiling combining nomothetic and idiographic parameters of momentary mental states. PLoS ONE 8, e59559.Google Scholar
Wurr, JC, Partridge, IM (1996). The prevalence of a history of childhood sexual abuse in an acute adult inpatient population. Child Abuse and Neglect 20, 867872.Google Scholar
Young, M, Read, J, Barker-Collo, S, Harrison, R (2001). Evaluating and overcoming barriers to taking abuse histories. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice 32, 407414.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Group differences and associated effect sizes in mean number of childhood adversity exposures in participants with and without psychotic symptoms

Figure 2

Table 3. Phi-coefficients (φ) and significant odds ratios between specific psychotic symptoms and specific childhood adversities