Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T21:00:03.199Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hunter territoriality creates refuges for threatened primates

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 March 2023

Brian M Griffiths*
Affiliation:
The Earth Commons, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, Georgetown University, 3700 O St NW, Washington, DC, USA
Mark Bowler
Affiliation:
School of Science, Technology and Engineering, University of Suffolk, Waterfront Building, Neptune Quay, Ipswich, IP4 1QJ, UK Suffolk Sustainability Institute, Waterfront Building, Neptune Quay, Ipswich, IP4 1QJ, UK
Michael P Gilmore
Affiliation:
School of Integrative Studies, George Mason University, 4400 University Drive, Fairfax, VA 22030, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Dr Brian M Griffiths, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Unsustainable hunting threatens biodiversity in the tropics through the removal of key seed-dispersing frugivorous primates. Traditionally, hunting in the Amazon Basin was managed through hunter territoriality, with the threat of social sanctions for overexploitation. We examined hunter territoriality and differential prey selection as alternative hypotheses to central-place foraging. Territoriality occurred beyond common hunting grounds, which were on major rivers and immediately surrounding the community. Hunters displayed selectivity in prey choice, with 50% of hunters not hunting primates. The combination of hunter territoriality and differential prey selection means that over 22% of the hunted area of the Sucusari river basin could be considered primate refuge. Of the remaining hunted area, 16% was hunted relatively little by primate hunters. We suggest that the combination of territoriality and selection against primates creates refuges, mitigating the effects of sustained hunting pressure and contributing to the conservation of these species.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Foundation for Environmental Conservation

Introduction

The unparalleled biodiversity (Brown Reference Brown2014) and productivity of Amazonian forests (Beer et al. Reference Beer, Reichstein, Tomelleri, Ciais, Jung and Carvalhais2010) are maintained in part by key ecosystem services provided by seed dispersers, such as frugivorous primates (Link & Fiore Reference Link and Fiore2006, Arévalo-Sandi et al. Reference Arévalo-Sandi, Bobrowiec, Chuma and Norris2018). The survival of many of these primate species, particularly large-bodied species that reproduce slowly (Mayor et al. Reference Mayor, El Bizri, Bodmer and Bowler2017), is threatened by unsustainable hunting, resulting in large-scale loss of ecosystem services (Barrera Zambrano et al. Reference Barrera Zambrano, Zambrano Moncada and Stevenson2008, Brodie et al. Reference Brodie, Helmy, Brockelman and Maron2009). The sustainability of hunting is often modelled using assumptions based on the central-place foraging theory (Orians & Pearson Reference Orians, Pearson, Horn, Mitchell and Stairs1979), in which hunters exploit resources near to the community or at other accessible points such as rivers first, producing a distance-based gradient of game abundance and availability (Charnov Reference Charnov1976, Winterhalder Reference Winterhalder, Panter-Brick, Layton and Rowley-Conwy2001, Venkataraman et al. Reference Venkataraman, Kraft, Dominy and Endicott2017). In the absence of large source areas, primates may be extirpated from a region even with relatively light hunting pressure (Levi et al. Reference Levi, Shepard, Ohl-Schacherer, Peres and Yu2009, Reference Levi, Lu, Yu and Mangel2011). The management of primate hunting is critical to the conservation of Amazonian biodiversity.

While primates have been hunted by Indigenous groups in the Amazon for millennia, recent evidence has shown that the protection of Indigenous lands and ways of life is intricately linked to the conservation of threatened primates (Estrada et al. Reference Estrada, Garber, Gouveia, Fernández-Llamazares, Ascensão and Fuentes2022). Sparse human populations and traditional adaptive management systems (Gadgil Reference Gadgil and Sarawathi1998, Berkes et al. Reference Berkes, Colding and Folke2000, Colding & Folke Reference Colding and Folke2001) may have contributed to the persistence of primates facing hunting pressure (Alvard Reference Alvard1995). One type of traditional management is territoriality of hunters (Dyson-Hudson & Smith Reference Dyson-Hudson and Smith1978), where restraint from the exploitation of others’ resources is governed by a fear of social sanctions. Territorial behaviour can be expected when game resources are moderate in density and evenly distributed and hunters can predict their location (Winterhalder Reference Winterhalder, Panter-Brick, Layton and Rowley-Conwy2001). These conventions spread out hunting pressure across the landscape, which increases sustainable harvest limits, and although widespread they are often employed without hunters acknowledging the role that territoriality plays in resource conservation (Gadgil et al. Reference Gadgil, Hemam, Reddy, Berkes and Folke1998). However, the erosion of traditional social systems may result in the abandonment of adaptive management practices, endangering species that were previously protected from overexploitation (Gadgil et al. Reference Gadgil, Hemam, Reddy, Berkes and Folke1998).

The Maijuna people are one of the most vulnerable Indigenous groups in Peru, with fewer than 600 individuals remaining in four communities (Gilmore et al. Reference Gilmore, Gilmore, Vriesendorp, Alverson, del Campo, Von May, López Wong and Ríos Ochoa2010). Their ancestral lands are currently threatened by a proposed highway development project, which would result in the destruction of both the ecosystem and the traditional culture and livelihoods of the Maijuna. Demonstrating ties between Maijuna culture and the conservation of natural resources, including wildlife, is critical to preserving the protected status of the region. This study aims to test whether the central foraging theory holds in the presence of territoriality among the hunters of the Maijuna community of Sucusari (Peru) and whether that territoriality, alongside partial prey preferences, could act as an adaptive management system for primate conservation. In this case, we describe territoriality as a passive strategy for spreading hunters across the landscape and maximizing the benefits from hunting, in which families willingly separate their hunting zones; we do not imply a defence of resources. We demonstrate that territoriality between families creates spatial heterogeneity in hunting pressure on primates across the landscape, but hunting was predicted to be heaviest surrounding the community, camps and major rivers and trails, following the central foraging theory. We found that large portions of the river basin may act as primate refugia due to the decision-making of individual hunters.

Methods

Study site

The Maijuna Indigenous community of Sucusari on the Rio Sucusari (Loreto, Peru; 72.92995° W, 3.24373° S; Fig. 1) is c. 120 km by river from the city of Iquitos, the commercial and political centre of Loreto. The titled land of the community encompasses 4771 ha and adjoins the Maijuna–Kichwa Regional Conservation Area (MKRCA), a 391 039.82-ha protected area collaboratively managed by Maijuna and Kichwa Indigenous communities and the regional government (El Peruano Reference Peruano2015). Sucusari has a population of 166 residents made up of monofamilial and plurifamilial households; 59% are ethnically Maijuna, 35% are mestizos and 6% are Kichwa (Roncal et al. Reference Roncal, Bowler and Gilmore2018).

Fig. 1. Diagrammatic map of the study site, the Maijuna community of Sucusari, the titled lands of the Sucusari community and the Maijuna–Kichwa Regional Conservation Area (MKRCA) in the north-eastern Peruvian Amazon.

Subsistence strategies of community members include hunting, fishing, swidden-fallow agriculture and the gathering of various non-timber forest products (Gilmore Reference Gilmore, Gilmore, Vriesendorp, Alverson, del Campo, Von May, López Wong and Ríos Ochoa2010). Community members sell agricultural products and non-timber forest products such as game meat for income in the city of Iquitos, in towns surrounding their communities on the Napo River and in the market of Mazán (Gilmore Reference Gilmore, Gilmore, Vriesendorp, Alverson, del Campo, Von May, López Wong and Ríos Ochoa2010). Only men hunt in Sucusari (Roncal et al. Reference Roncal, Bowler and Gilmore2018), hunting from canoes and on foot and at mineral lick sites located across the river basin (Gilmore et al. Reference Gilmore, Griffiths and Bowler2020). Hunters also hunt opportunistically from boats during travel when the hunter was not actively searching for prey but had a weapon available to them in the boat in case of a chance encounter with a game animal. All hunters hunt with shotguns or with machetes for slow-moving game species.

Data collection and analysis

We conducted weekly semi-structured interviews (Berg & Lune Reference Berg and Lune2014) with 19 hunters (90.48% of all active hunters) in Sucusari to capture hunter territoriality and decision-making processes on prey selection and the economics of hunting for a 10-month period from September 2018 to June 2019. The mean age of hunters interviewed was 41 years old (range: 22–68 years). Of the 19 hunters interviewed, 10 were ethnically Maijuna, 8 were mestizo and 1 was Kichwa. If a hunter was not home when we visited his house, we made at least two more attempts to contact them during the days immediately following the initial visit. If a hunter was still not at home after three visit attempts, we gathered the data for that week during the following week’s interview. During the interview, for each hunt we asked the hunter to draw the route they took on a provided base map, which included relevant landmarks such as rivers, streams, hunting camps and mineral licks, and to indicate which animals they encountered and if they had attempted to kill the animal (see Griffiths & Gilmore Reference Griffiths and Gilmore2022, Griffiths et al. Reference Griffiths, Bowler, Kolowski, Stabach, Benson and Gilmore2022). If they did not try to kill the animal, we asked why they chose not to. At the end of the study, to evaluate the degree to which hunters recognize differences in spatial use of the landscape, we asked questions about their preferred hunting locations and zones and why those locations were preferred. We also asked each hunter to indicate their hunting zone on the base map. Finally, we asked scenario-based questions about hunting zones, such as: ‘If you wanted to, could you hunt in someone else’s zone?’.

We digitized all hunter tracks in ArcGIS (ESRI 2018). To analyse the hunting zones of different families, we first grouped hunters into family groups. Only hunters who hunted more than 10 times during the study period were considered in the territory analyses (n = 16). Hunters that were linked by direct family ties (e.g., brothers or father/son) or direct marriage ties (e.g., married into a hunting family) were grouped together for family-level analyses based on data gathered from interviews showing that hunters in the same family hunted in the same zone. We used track data from interviews to calculate the zone of each family, excluding opportunistic hunts. Hunter tracks were converted to raster data in a 1 km × 1 km grid, a resolution chosen based on measured error in hunter reporting (Griffiths et al. Reference Griffiths, Kolowski, Bowler, Gilmore, Benson, Lewis and Stabach2021, Reference Griffiths, Gonzales and Gilmore2023), where the value in each cell corresponded to the total number of kilometres walked by the family in that cell – a measure of hunting effort (Sirén et al. Reference Sirén, Cardenas, Hambäck and Parvinen2013). Hunting effort is used as a proxy for hunting pressure exerted by hunters on the grid cell. We summarized the overlap of territories by counting the proportion of cells that were only visited by one family throughout the study period. We then assessed the prevalence of common hunting grounds surrounding the community and along the two major rivers in the basin using a generalized linear model framework. We applied a generalized linear model with a Poisson distribution and response variable as the number of families that used each cell (n = 302). We used the distance from the centre of the cell to the community and whether or not the cell was located on one of the two major rivers (binary) as covariates, as well as the interaction between them. We used an information-theoretic framework for model selection (Burnham & Anderson Reference Burnham and Anderson2002), comparing the Akaike information criterion (AIC) values of each unique covariate combination and choosing the model with the lowest AIC value as the optimal model. Covariates were checked for collinearity (Dormann et al. Reference Dormann, Elith, Bacher, Buchmann, Carl and Carré2013) and full models were checked for overdispersion before proceeding with model selection.

As many hunters in Sucusari reported during interviews that they do not kill primates (Griffiths et al. Reference Griffiths, Bowler, Kolowski, Stabach, Benson and Gilmore2022), we assessed the prevalence of primate refuges in the river basin. We calculated the proportion of the total hunting pressure in each grid cell that was contributed by hunters who do not kill primates. Cells that were only visited by hunters who do not kill primates (100% of contributed hunting pressure) were considered hunted primate refuges. We compared the area of primate refuge space and areas with relatively little hunting pressure on primates (<20% of total hunting pressure of the cell) to the total hunted area in the river basin.

To assess how the movement of primate hunters in Sucusari aligned with central foraging theory, we calculated predicted hunting pressure on primates using the distance-based model reported by Griffiths et al. (Reference Griffiths, Kolowski, Bowler, Gilmore, Benson, Lewis and Stabach2021, Reference Griffiths, Gonzales and Gilmore2023; cell size 1 km × 1 km). This generalized linear model assumes that hunters may radiate from four potential access points – the community, hunting camps, major rivers and major hunting trails – and that hunting pressure decreases with distance from these points. The model also takes into account movement cost associated with landscape factors such as elevation, slope and surface roughness (Griffiths et al. Reference Griffiths, Kolowski, Bowler, Gilmore, Benson, Lewis and Stabach2021, Reference Griffiths, Gonzales and Gilmore2023). We divided each cell by the total predicted hunting pressure of all cells to yield a proportion of the total predicted hunting pressure in each cell. We then multiplied the predicted proportion of hunting pressure in each cell by 0.459 – the proportion of measured hunting pressure that came from primate hunters – to yield a predicted spatial spread of hunting pressure by primate hunters. We calculated the actual spatial spread of hunting pressure on primates by dividing the measured hunting pressure in each cell by the total hunting pressure exerted by primate hunters (total kilometres walked), so that each cell held a proportion of the total measured hunting pressure value. We visually compared maps of the predicted spread of hunting pressure on primates and the actual spread of hunting pressure on primates to examine how primate hunters conformed to central foraging theory.

Results

We conducted 780 interviews capturing data on 671 unique hunts, then another set of interviews with 19 hunters at the end of the study. All hunters stated in their interviews that they each had a hunting zone in the river basin where they would go to hunt and rarely entered others’ zones, although they could if they chose to. Overall, 302 grid cells, or an area of 302 km2, were hunted by the 16 hunters whose territories were assessed, who were grouped into six families. Upon calculating hunters’ territories, 46% of cells were visited by only one family, 28% of cells were visited by two families and 12% of cells were visited by three families during the study period (Fig. 2). When cells along the major rivers were excluded, 53% of cells were visited by only one family. The mean territory size for individual families was 103.83 km2 (SD = 74.57, range = 38–224). The optimal model (df = 3, w = 0.59, explained deviance = 0.41) for predicting the number of families that visited each cell included covariates of distance from the community (coefficient = –0.039, SE = 0.0059) and the presence of a major river (coefficient = 0.53, SE = 0.091). One alternative model fell within 2 AIC points of the optimal model (ΔAIC = 0.76), which included river presence, distance from the community and their interaction; however, this model yielded the same trends as the optimal model. Model results showed that cells closer to the community and along rivers were visited by more families than those farther from the community and on land (Fig. S1).

Fig. 2. Diagrammatic map showing the number of families that visited each hunted cell in the Sucusari river basin.

We examined the spatial use of the landscape of primate hunters versus non-primate hunters. Overall, eight hunters out of the 16 analysed indicated that they do not hunt primates. On a family level, two families hunted primates, two families avoided primates and two families had individual hunters who either hunted or avoided primates. The map of the predicted spread of hunting pressure on primates differed from the measured spread of hunting pressure on primates (Fig. S2). Following central foraging theory and cost surfaces, hunting pressure on primates was predicted to be the heaviest immediately surrounding the community, hunting camps and major rivers and hunting trails. Hunting camps and points along rivers and trails that were closer to the community were predicted to experience heavier hunting pressure than those further from the community. The map of actual hunting pressure on primates showed distinct areas in the eastern, north-western and north-eastern parts of the river basin, which experienced relatively little hunting pressure on primates in comparison (Fig. S2).

We examined these areas for the prevalence of primate refuges. Of the 302 km2 that were hunted, 68 km2 (23%) were hunted exclusively by hunters who do not kill primates (Fig 3). Within this area, 14 km2 were constituted by fragments 1–5 km2 in size. The mean refuge area was 6.18 km2 (SD = 6.42) and the largest contiguous fragment was 22 km2 (Fig. 3). Overall, 234 km2 of the catchment area were hunted either exclusively by hunters who do kill primates or by both groups of hunters (Fig. 3). Within this area, 38 km2 (16% of cells) had at least 80% of their hunting pressure attributed to hunters who do not kill primates, and 155 km2 (66% of cells) had at least 50% of their hunting pressure attributed to hunters who do not kill primates. Hotspots of hunting pressure on primates also occurred, with several cells only being hunted by hunters who target primates.

Fig. 3. Diagrammatic map of potential primate refuges in the Sucusari river basin, showing the cells that experienced no hunting (unhunted refuges, green), those that were hunted by non-primate hunters only (hunted primate refuges) and the proportion of each cell’s total hunting pressure (HP) that came from primate hunters in every other hunted cell.

Discussion

All hunters recognized a distinct zone as their own hunting territory and indicated that they do not enter others’ zones, although they could if they chose to. The high level of agreement on these questions indicated that there was territoriality (Berkes et al. Reference Berkes, Colding and Folke2000) surrounding resource use. Areas near to the community and along the two major rivers in the Sucusari basin acted as common areas and were visited by almost all hunters, while areas farther from the community formed more distinct territories between families. It is probable that the use of hunter tracks to calculate territories overestimates the overlap among families, as hunters frequently travel through other zones on their way to their own (e.g., using the same river as an access point; Griffiths et al. Reference Griffiths, Bowler, Kolowski, Stabach, Benson and Gilmore2022). Therefore, while our results showed c. 54% overlap among the zones hunters entered, including the common areas near to the community and rivers, it is probable that spatial partitioning of the landscape is even more distinct between families. Territoriality among hunters may also be a traditional practice for the Maijuna, resulting in differential use of resources across the landscape (Gilmore et al. Reference Gilmore, Griffiths and Bowler2020).

Even though there seemed to be consensus on how hunting zones are used, there are no formalized or written community-wide regulations surrounding their use. In this case, the consensus probably indicates the presence of social norms in the community that may be acting as a resource management system in which rules are unwritten but still very closely adhered to (St John et al. Reference St John, Edwards-Jones and Jones2011). The partitioning of space limits the hunting pressure that each area is subject to and spreads the hunting pressure more evenly across the landscape. These social norms mean that some hunters solely hunt far away from the community, challenging the idea that hunters are central-place foragers and that hunting pressure decreases continuously as hunters move farther from the community. The partial preferences for primates exhibited by hunters create an even more fractured mosaic of hunting pressure on primates, resulting in refuges. Over 22% of the hunted area of the river basin could be considered refugia for primates, with another 16% of the remaining area being hunted for primates relatively little (<20% of the area’s total hunting pressure). In the northern Congo, heterogeneity in landscape features created small sources scattered throughout the landscape that distributed animals into sink areas (Mockrin et al. Reference Mockrin, Rockwell, Redford and Keuler2011). In Sucusari, similar heterogeneity is created by hunter territoriality and prey preferences, and the effects on species distributions are probably similar. In this case, the partitioning of space by social and cultural norms could have effects similar to changes in habitat and accessibility. This may be particularly true for species that have a small home range, such as the red howler monkey (Crockett & Eisenberg Reference Crockett, Eisenberg, Smuts, Cheney, Seyfarth, Wrangham and Struhsaker1987).

The conservation and sustainability implications of this adaptive management system are far-reaching. On the surface, hunters in Sucusari appear to be central-place foragers: they live in a community and travel to hunting grounds via rivers. However, a more detailed analysis of hunting reveals that hunting pressure is not only highly variable spatially, but also changes based on the game species in question. It should be noted that hunters in Sucusari hunt in both their community titled lands and in the MKRCA to the north, to which they have hunting rights. While we have never observed a difference in behaviour of hunters who are hunting in the titled lands versus the MKRCA, it is probable that hunters and communities without titled lands (and without access to a large conservation area) behave quite differently. It is possible that the continued protected status of their lands (and the food security that this status brings) is the only thing that allows these passive management practices to flourish. Hunters who are more food insecure (without access to large source areas) are likely to be less selective about prey items and about hunting zones.

This study provides evidence that an understanding of local norms and hunter preferences is essential when assessing the impacts of hunting on mammal populations, and that these social and cultural conventions are critical to providing an accurate understanding of hunting in local communities. The presence of local refuges may impact the overall sustainability of hunting and contribute to the conservation of ecologically important species (Gadgil et al. Reference Gadgil, Hemam, Reddy, Berkes and Folke1998). These refuges create resilience in the hunting system, ensuring the conservation of key seed-dispersing primates that maintain diversity and ensure the survival of even more species of plants and animals. Our results demonstrate the complexity of wild game management in Amazonia, where sociocultural and ecological factors interact to influence conservation and sustainability, and these factors should be considered before management decisions are made. Furthermore, the erosion of traditional adaptive management practices could be detrimental to the sustainability of hunting in rural areas.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0376892923000061.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the Maijuna community of Sucusari for collaborating on this project and OnePlanet, Inc. and Explorama Lodges for providing in-kind support to the team in the field. We thank the Fulbright Association for awarding a Fulbright US Student Grant to BMG, which funded this project.

Financial support

This work was funded by a Fulbright US Student Grant awarded to BMG.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Ethical standards

All aspects of this study were approved by George Mason University’s institutional review board, project #1288488-1. Prior informed consent was obtained from individual research participants before beginning this study.

References

Alvard, M (1995) Shotguns and sustainable hunting in the Neotropics. Oryx 29: 5866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arévalo-Sandi, A, Bobrowiec, PED, Chuma, VJUR, Norris, D (2018) Diversity of terrestrial mammal seed dispersers along a lowland Amazon forest regrowth gradient. PLoS ONE 13: e0193752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrera Zambrano, VA, Zambrano Moncada, J, Stevenson, PR (2008) Diversity of regenerating plants and seed dispersal in two canopy trees from Colombian Amazon forests with different hunting pressure. Revista de Biologia Tropical 56: 15311542.Google ScholarPubMed
Beer, C, Reichstein, M, Tomelleri, E, Ciais, P, Jung, M, Carvalhais, N et al. (2010) Terrestrial gross carbon dioxide uptake: global distribution and covariation with climate. Science 329: 834838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, BL, Lune, H (2014) Qualitative Research Methods for the Social Sciences. Harlow, UK: Pearson.Google Scholar
Berkes, F, Colding, J, Folke, C (2000) Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. Ecological Applications 10: 12511262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brodie, JF, Helmy, OE, Brockelman, WY, Maron, JL (2009) Bushmeat poaching reduces the seed dispersal and population growth rate of a mammal-dispersed tree. Ecological Applications 19: 854863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, JH (2014) Why are there so many species in the tropics? Journal of Biogeography 41: 822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnham, KP, Anderson, DR (2002) Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach, 2nd edition. New York, NY, USA: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Charnov, EL (1976) Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical population Biology 9: 129136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colding, J, Folke, C (2001) Social taboos: ‘invisible’ systems of local resource management and biological conservation. Ecological Applications 11: 584600.Google Scholar
Crockett, CM, Eisenberg, J (1987) Howlers: variations in group size and demography. In: Smuts, B, Cheney, D, Seyfarth, R, Wrangham, R, Struhsaker, T (eds), Primate Societies (pp. 5468). Chicago, IL, USA: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Dormann, CF, Elith, J, Bacher, S, Buchmann, C, Carl, G, Carré, G et al. (2013) Collinearity: a review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography 36: 2746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyson-Hudson, R, Smith, EA (1978) Human territoriality: an ecological reassessment. American Anthropologist 80: 2141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peruano, El (2015) Establecen área de conservación regional Maijuna Kichwa, ubicada en el departamento de Loreto. Decreto No. 008-2015 [www document]. URL http://www.elperuano.com.pe/NormasElperuano/2015/06/17/1252025-2.html Google Scholar
ESRI (2018) ArcGIS release 10.6. Redlands, CA, USA: ESRI.Google Scholar
Estrada, A, Garber, PA, Gouveia, S, Fernández-Llamazares, Á, Ascensão, F, Fuentes, A, et al. (2022) Global importance of Indigenous peoples, their lands, and knowledge systems for saving the world’s primates from extinction. Science Advances 8: eabn2927 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gadgil, M (1998) Traditional resource management systems. In: Sarawathi, B (ed.), Life Style and Ecology (pp. 526). New Delhi, India: Indira Gandhi National Centre for the Arts.Google Scholar
Gadgil, M, Hemam, NS, Reddy, BM (1998) People, refugia and resilience. In: Berkes, F, Folke, C (eds), Linking Social and Ecological Systems: Management Practices and Social Mechanisms for Building Resilience (pp. 3047). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gilmore, MP (2010) The Maijuna: past, present, and future. In: Gilmore, MP, Vriesendorp, C, Alverson, WS, del Campo, Á, Von May, R, López Wong, C, Ríos Ochoa, S (eds), Perú: Maijuna. Rapid Biological and Social Inventories Report 22. Chicago, IL, USA: The Field Museum (p. 226).Google Scholar
Gilmore, MP, Griffiths, BM, Bowler, M (2020) The socio-cultural significance of mineral licks to the Maijuna of the Peruvian Amazon: implications for the sustainable management of hunting. Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine 16: 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilmore, MP, Vriesendorp, C, Alverson, WS, del Campo, Á, Von May, R, López Wong, C, Ríos Ochoa, S (2010) Perú: Maijuna. Rapid Biological and Social Inventories Report 22. Chicago, IL, USA: The Field Museum.Google Scholar
Griffiths, BM (2020) Assessing the Drivers, Magnitude, and Implications of Hunting Pressure in an Indigenous Amazonian Community. PhD thesis. Fairfax, VA, USA: George Mason University.Google Scholar
Griffiths, BM, Bowler, M, Kolowski, J, Stabach, J, Benson, EL, Gilmore, MP (2022) Revisiting optimal foraging theory (OFT) in a changing Amazon: implications for conservation and management. Human Ecology 50: 545558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, BM, Gilmore, MP (2022) Differential use of game species in an Amazonian Indigenous community: navigating economics, subsistence, and social norms. Journal of Ethnobiology 42: 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, BM, Gonzales, T, Gilmore, MP (2023) Spatiotemporal variation in hunting in a riverine Indigenous community in the Amazon. Biodiversity and Conservation. Epub ahead of print. DOI: 10.1007/s10531-022-02535-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, BM, Kolowski, J, Bowler, M, Gilmore, MP, Benson, E, Lewis, F, Stabach, J (2021) Assessing the accuracy of distance- and interview-based measures of hunting pressure. Conservation Science and Practice 4: e592.Google Scholar
Levi, T, Lu, F, Yu, DW, Mangel, M (2011) The behaviour and diet breadth of central-place foragers: an application to human hunters and Neotropical game management. Evolutionary Ecology Research 13: 171185.Google Scholar
Levi, T, Shepard, GH, Ohl-Schacherer, J, Peres, CA, Yu, DW (2009) Modelling the long-term sustainability of Indigenous hunting in Manu National Park, Peru: landscape-scale management implications for Amazonia. Journal of Applied Ecology 46: 804814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Link, A, Fiore, AD (2006) Seed dispersal by spider monkeys and its importance in the maintenance of neotropical rain-forest diversity. Journal of Tropical Ecology 22: 235246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayor, P, El Bizri, H, Bodmer, RE, Bowler, M (2017) Assessment of mammal reproduction for hunting sustainability through community-based sampling of species in the wild. Conservation Biology 31: 912923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mockrin, MH, Rockwell, RF, Redford, KH, Keuler, NS (2011) Effects of landscape features on the distribution and sustainability of ungulate hunting in northern Congo. Efectos de las Características del Paisaje sobre la Distribución y Sustentabilidad de la Cacería de Ungulados en el Norte de Congo 25: 514525.Google ScholarPubMed
Orians, G, Pearson, N (1979) On the theory of central place foraging. In: Horn, D, Mitchell, R, Stairs, G (eds), Analysis of Ecological Systems (pp. 157177). Columbus, OH, USA: Ohio State University Press.Google Scholar
Roncal, CM, Bowler, M, Gilmore, MP (2018) The ethnoprimatology of the Maijuna of the Peruvian Amazon and implications for primate conservation. Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine 14: 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirén, AH, Cardenas, J-C, Hambäck, P, Parvinen, K (2013) Distance friction and the cost of hunting in tropical forest. Land Economics 89: 558574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St John, FA, Edwards-Jones, G, Jones, JP (2011) Conservation and human behaviour: lessons from social psychology. Wildlife Research 37: 658667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venkataraman, VV, Kraft, TS, Dominy, NJ, Endicott, KM (2017) Hunter-gatherer residential mobility and the marginal value of rainforest patches. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114: 30973102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winterhalder, B (2001) The behavioural ecology of hunter-gatherers. In: Panter-Brick, C, Layton, RH, Rowley-Conwy, P (eds), Hunter-Gatherers: An Interdisciplinary Perspective (pp. 1238). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Diagrammatic map of the study site, the Maijuna community of Sucusari, the titled lands of the Sucusari community and the Maijuna–Kichwa Regional Conservation Area (MKRCA) in the north-eastern Peruvian Amazon.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Diagrammatic map showing the number of families that visited each hunted cell in the Sucusari river basin.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Diagrammatic map of potential primate refuges in the Sucusari river basin, showing the cells that experienced no hunting (unhunted refuges, green), those that were hunted by non-primate hunters only (hunted primate refuges) and the proportion of each cell’s total hunting pressure (HP) that came from primate hunters in every other hunted cell.

Supplementary material: File

Griffiths et al. supplementary material

Griffiths et al. supplementary material

Download Griffiths et al. supplementary material(File)
File 3.8 MB