Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:05:15.895Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Working Memory, Test Anxiety and Effective Interventions: A Review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2012

Tony Mowbray*
Affiliation:
Monash University and Pinelodge Clinic, Australia
*
Address for correspondence: Tony Mowbray, Pinelodge Clinic, 1480 Heatherton Road, Dandenong VIC 3175, Australia. Email: [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

Test anxiety (TA) affects exam performance and has been found to be composed of a cognitive (worry) and emotional component. The cognitive component has been implicated in the performance decrements seen in individuals with high TA; however, interventions involving cognitive therapies have provided mixed and limited outcomes with regard to improvement in performance. This article explores TA in relation to attentional control theory, which provides a framework for the impact of anxiety on working memory and cognitive performance. Possible interventions for the treatment of TA guided by attentional control theory will then be discussed and the need for further research to explore the efficacy of these interventions established.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Australian Psychological Society Ltd 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 31, 3060. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P. (2011). The benefits of computerised working memory assessment. Educational and Child Psychology, 28 (2), 817. http://www.bpsshop.org.uk/Educational-Child-Psychology-Vol-28-No-2-June-2011-Computerised-approaches-to-assessment-P1457.aspx.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417423. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 4, 829839. doi:10.1038/nrn1201Google Scholar
Bandalos, D. L., Yates, K., & Thorndike-Christ, T. (1995). The effects of math self-concept, perceived self-efficacy, and attributions for success and failure on test anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87 (4), 611624. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.87.4.611Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L., Kulp, C. A., Holt, L. E., & Carr, T. H. (2004). More on the fragility of performance: Choking under pressure in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133 (4), 584600. doi:10.1037/00963445.133.4.584Google Scholar
Benjamin, M., McKeachie, W. J., Lin, Y., & Holinger, D. P. (1981). Test anxiety: Deficits in information processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73 (6), 816824. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.73.6.816Google Scholar
Birenbaum, M., & Pinku, P. (1997). Effects of test anxiety, information organization, and testing situation on performance on two test formats. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 2338. doi:10.1006/ceps.1997.0923CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodas, J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). Test anxiety: A cross-cultural perspective. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8 (1), 6588. doi:10.1007/s10567-005-2342-xGoogle Scholar
Brown, L. A., Forman, E. M., Herbert, J. D., Hoffman, K. L., Yuen, E. K., & Goetter, E. M. (2011). A randomized controlled trial of acceptance-based behaviour therapy and cognitive therapy for test anxiety: A pilot study. Journal of Behavior Modification, 35 (1), 3153. doi:10.1177/01454455103-90930Google Scholar
Bulevich, J. B., Roediger, H. L., Balota, D. A., & Butler, A. C. (2006). Failures to find suppression of episodic memories in the think/no-think paradigm. Memory and Cognition, 34, 15691577. doi:10.3758/BF03195920Google Scholar
Calvo, M. G., & Eysenck, M. W. (1996). Phonological working memory and reading in test anxiety. Memory, 4, 289305. doi:10.1080/096582196388960Google Scholar
Cassady, C. J. (2004). The influence of cognitive test anxiety across the learning-test cycle. Journal of Learning and Instruction, 14, 569592. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.09.002Google Scholar
Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 270295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2001.1094Google Scholar
Chapell, M. S., Blanding, B. Z., Silverstein, M. E., Takahashi, M., Newman, B., Gubi, A., & McCann, N. (2005). Test anxiety and academic performance in undergraduate and graduate students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97 (2), 268274. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chein, J., & Morrison, A. (2010). Expanding the mind's workspace: Training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17 (2), 193199. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.2.193Google Scholar
Covington, M. (2000). Goal theory, motivation, and school achievement: An integrative review. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 141156. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.171Google Scholar
Coy, W. B., O'Brien, W. H., Tabaczynski, T., Northern, J. J., & Carels, R. C. (2011). Associations between evaluation anxiety, cognitive interference, and performance on working memory tasks. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 823832. doi:10.1002/acp.1765Google Scholar
Culler, R. E., & Holohan, C. J. (1980). Test anxiety and academic performance: the effects of study-related behaviors. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 1626. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.72.1.16Google Scholar
D'Alelio, W. A., & Murray, A. J. (1981). Cognitive therapy for test anxiety. Cognitive therapy and research, 5, 299307. doi:10.1007/BF01193413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darke, S. (1988). Effects of anxiety on inferential reasoning task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 499505. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.55.3.499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dendato, K. M., & Diener, D. (1986). Effectiveness of cognitive/relaxation therapy and studyskills training in reducing self-reported anxiety and improving the academic performance of test-anxious students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33 (2), 131135. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.33.2.131Google Scholar
Derakshan, N., Ansari, T. L., Hansard, M., Shoker, L., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety inhibition, efficiency, and effectiveness: An investigation using the antisaccade task. Experimental Psychology, 56, 4855. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.56.1.48Google Scholar
Derakhshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety, processing efficiency, and cognitive performance new developments from attentional control theory. European Psychologist, 14 (2), 168176. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.14.2.168Google Scholar
Diaz, R. J., Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., & Tanofsky-Kraff, M. (2001). Cognition, anxiety, and prediction of performance in 1st-year law students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93 (2), 420429. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.93.2.420Google Scholar
Doughty, A. H., Kastner, R. M., & Bismark, B. D. (2011). Resurgence of derived stimulus relations: Replication and extensions. Behavioural Processes, 86 (1), 152155. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliman, N. A., Green, M. W., Rogers, P. J., & Finch, G. M. (1997). Processing-efficiency theory and the working-memory system: Impairments associated with sub-clinical anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 3135. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00016-0Google Scholar
Entwistle, N., Tait, H., & McCune, V. (2000). Patterns of response to an approaches to studying inventory across contrasting groups and contexts. European Journal of the Psychology of Education, 15, 3348. doi:10.1007/BF03173165Google Scholar
Ergene, T. (2003). Effective interventions on test anxiety reduction: A meta-analysis. School Psychology International, 24 (3), 313328. doi:10.1177/01430343030243004Google Scholar
Erskine, J. A. K., Kvavilashvili, L., & Kornbrot, D. E. (2007). The predictors of thought suppression in young and old adults: Effects of anxiety, rumination and other variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 10471057. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.016Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7 (2), 336353. doi:10.1037/15283542.7.2.336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, M. W., Payne, S., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Trait anxiety, visuospatial processing, and working memory. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 12141228. doi:10.1080/02699930500260245Google Scholar
Gray, J. R. (2001). Emotional modulation of cognitive control: Approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 436452. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.436Google Scholar
Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., . . . Pine, D. S. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: A meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 982990. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021Google Scholar
Hallet, P. E. (1978). Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research, 18, 12791296.Google Scholar
Harris, R. (2006). Embracing your demons: An overview of acceptance and commitment therapy. Psychotherapy in Australia, 12 (4), 28.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (Eds.). (2001). Relational Frame Theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, S., Hirsch, C. R., & Mathews, A. (2010). Facilitating a benign attentional bias reduces negative thought intrusions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 235240. doi:10.1037/a0018264Google Scholar
Hayes, S., MacLeod, C., & Hammond, G. (2009). Anxiety-linked task performance: Dissociating the influence of restricted working memory capacity and increased investment of effort. Cognition & Emotion, 23 (4), 753781. doi:10.1080/02699930802131078Google Scholar
Hazen, R. A., Vasey, M. W., & Schmidt, N. B. (2009). Attentional retraining: A randomized clinical trial for pathological worry. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 627633. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.07.004Google Scholar
Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, and treatment of test anxiety. Review of Educational Research, 58, 4777. doi:10.3102/00346543058001047CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsch, C. R., MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., Sandher, O., Siyani, A., & Hayes, S. (2011). The contribution of attentional bias to worry: Distinguishing the roles of selective engagement and disengagement. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 272277. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.09.013Google Scholar
Ikeda, M., Iwanaga, M., & Seiwa, H. (1996). Test anxiety and working memory system. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82 (3), 12231231. doi:10.2466/pms.1996.82.3c.1223Google Scholar
Johnson, D. R., & Gronlund, D. S. (2009). Individuals lower in working memory capacity are particularly vulnerable to anxiety's disruptive effect on performance. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 22 (2), 201213. doi:10.1080/10615800802291277Google Scholar
Joiner, T. E., Steer, R. A., Beck, A. T., Schmidt, N. B., Rudd, M. D., & Catanzaro, S. J. (1999). Physiological hyperarousal: Construct validity of a central aspect of the tripartite model of depression and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 290298. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.108.2.290Google Scholar
Keogh, E., & French, C. C. (2001). Test anxiety, evaluative stress and susceptibility to distraction from threat. European Journal of Personality, 15, 123141. doi:10.1002/per.400Google Scholar
Lee, J. H. (1999). Test anxiety and working memory. Journal of Experimental Education, 67 (3), 218225. doi:10.1080/00220979909598354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W., (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 20, 975978. doi:10.2466/pr0.1967.20.3.975Google Scholar
Lowe, P. A., Lee, S. W., Witteborg, K. M., Prichard, K. W., Luhr, M. E., Cullinan, C. M., . . . Janik, M. (2008). The Test Anxiety Inventory for Children and Adolescents (TAICA): Examination of the psychometric properties of a new multidimensional measure of test anxiety among elementary and secondary school students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 26 (3), 215230. doi:10.1177/0734282907303760CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 189217. doi:10.1146/annurevclinpsy032511-143052Google Scholar
MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111 (1), 107123. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.1.107Google Scholar
McIlroy, D., Bunting, B., & Adamson, G. (2000). An evaluation of the factor structure and predictive validity of a test anxiety scale with reference to students’ past performance and personality indices. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 1732. doi:10.1348/000709900157949Google Scholar
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? on the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141 (2), 302320. doi:10.1037/a0025250Google Scholar
Mecklinger, A., Parra, M., & Waldhauser, G. T. (2009). ERP correlates of intentional forgetting. Brain Research, 1255, 132147. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.073Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734Google Scholar
Najmi, S., & Amir, N. (2010). The effect of attention training on a behavioral test of contamination fears in individuals with subclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 136142. doi:10.1037/a0017549CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naveh-Benjamin, M., McKeachie, W. J., & Lin, Y. G. (1991). A comparison of training programs intended for different types of test anxious students: Further support for an information-processing model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 134139. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.1.134Google Scholar
Newbegin, I., & Owens, A. (1996). Self-esteem and anxiety in secondary school achievement. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11, 521530. http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1996-06041-008Google Scholar
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2005). Academic examination stress impairs self-control. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 276. doi:10.1521/jscp.24.2.254.62276Google Scholar
Onyeizugbo, E. (2010). Self-efficacy, gender and trait anxiety as moderators of test anxiety. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 8 (1), 299312. http://www.investigacion-psicopedagogica.org/revista/new/english/index.php?n=20Google Scholar
Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., . . . & Ballard, C. G. (2010). Putting brain testing to the test. Nature, 10 (465), 775779. doi:10.1038/nature09042Google Scholar
Owens, M., Stevenson, J., Norgate, R., & Hadwin, J. A. (2008). Processing efficiency theory in children: Working memory as a mediator between trait anxiety and academic performance. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 21, 417430. doi:10.1080/10615800701847823Google Scholar
Peckham, A. D., McHugh, R. K., & Otto, M. W. (2010). A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depression and Anxiety, 27, 11351142. doi:10.1002/da.20755CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putwain, D. W., Connors, L., & Symes, W. (2010). Do cognitive distortions mediate the test anxiety–examination performance relationship? Educational Psychology, 30 (1), 1126. doi:10.1080/01443410903328866Google Scholar
Raju, M. P., & Asfaw, A. (2009). Recalled test anxiety in relation to achievement, in the context of general academic self-concept, study habits, parental involvement and socio-economic status among grade 6 Ethiopian students. International Journal of Primary, elementary and Early Years Education, 37 (3), 269285. doi:10.1080/03004270902734085Google Scholar
Richmond, L. L., Morrison, A. B., Chein, J. M., & Olson, I. R. (2011). Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 26, 813822. doi:10.1037/a0023631Google Scholar
Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2005). A comparison of attentional biases and memory biases in women with social phobia and major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 6274. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.62Google Scholar
Rosen, V. M., & Engle, R. W. (1998). Working memory capacity and suppression. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 418436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2590Google Scholar
Rozenman, M., Weersing, V., & Amir, N. (2011). A case series of attention modification in clinically anxious youths. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 324330. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2011.02.007Google Scholar
Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: Findings from the COGITO study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2, 110. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2010.00027Google Scholar
Shackman, A. J., Sarinopoulos, I., Maxwell, J. S., Pizzagali, D. A., Lavric, A., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Anxiety selectively disrupts visuospatial working memory. Emotion, 6, 061. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.40Google Scholar
Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind. Memory & Cognition, 36, 11441150. doi:10.3758/MC.36.6.1144Google Scholar
Spada, M. M., Nikcevic, A., Moneta, G. B., & Ireson, J. (2006) Metacognition as a mediator of the effect of test anxiety on a surface approach to studying. Educational Psychology, 26 (5), 615624. doi:10.1080/01443410500390673Google Scholar
Spangler, G., Pekrun, R., Kramer, K., & Hofmann, H. (2002). Students’ emotions, physiological reactions, and coping in academic exams. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 15 (4), 413. doi:10.1080/1061580021000056555Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Gonzales, H. P., Taylor, C. J., Anton, W. D., Algaze, B., Ross, G. K., & Westberry, L. G. (1980). Test Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135, 7799.Google Scholar
Tohill, J., & Holyoak, K. (2000). The impact of anxiety on analogical reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 6 (1), 2740. doi:10.1080/135467800393911Google Scholar
Waldhauser, G. T., Johansson, M., Bäckström, M., & Mecklinger, A. (2011). Trait anxiety, working memory capacity, and the effectiveness of memory suppression. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 52, 2127. doi:10.1111/j.14679450.2010.00845.xGoogle Scholar
Wilson, K. G., & Hayes, S. C. (1996). Resurgence of derived stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 66 (3), 267281. doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.66-267Google Scholar
Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Zettle, R. D. (2003). Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) Vs. Systematic desensitization in treatment of mathematics anxiety. The Psychological Record, 53, 197215. http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol53/iss2/3Google Scholar