Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T05:06:46.687Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fluid and crystallised intelligence and their relationship to school outcome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2015

Mary E. Marriott
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Esther Care*
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
*
Department of Learning and Educational Development, Faculty of Education, University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia, Fax: 61 8344 0975, E-mail: [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

Few studies exist that address the relationship between the higher order factors of fluid (Go and crystallised (Gc) intelligence and Victorian Certificate of Education (VCE) outcomes. In this study, for each of five cohorts, longitudinal data have been collected over a six-year span, consisting of results from standardised achievement and ability tests and ending with achievement outcomes.Two factors, interpreted as Gf and Gc, are derived from the standardised measures. In Year 7, these measures were the Progressive Achievement Test Mathematics (PAT Moths), Progressive Achievement Test Reading Comprehension (PAT Comprehension), and the jenkins Test of Nonverbal Ability. In Year 10, the measures were the subtests of the Differential Aptitude Test (DAT).These factors are investigated in order to identify whether they are differentially important in regard to VCE Performance. The standardised measures and final VCE results were collected at a Melbourne independent girls' school from 414 exiting Year 12 students, in 1998, 1999, 2000, 2001, and 2002. Both Gf and Gc accounted for a significant amount of the variance in VCE performance, with Gc being the most significant indicator.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Australian Psychological Society 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P.L., Beier, M.E., & Bowen, K.R. (2000). Explorations of crystallized intelligence completion tests, cloze tests, and knowledge. Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 105121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anastasi, A. (1990). Psychological testing. (6th ed.) New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bachman, J. & O’Malley, P. (1986). Self-concepts, self-esteem, and educational experiences: The frog pond revisited (again). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(1), 3546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauducel, A., Brocke, B., & Liepmann, D. (2001). Perspectives on fluid and crystallized intelligence: facets for verbal, numerical, and figurai intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(6), 977994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. (1985). Type of school and academic achievement. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Sociology, 21, 219236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J.B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: Press Syndicate of the University of Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R.B. (1980). The heritability of fluid, gf, and crystallized, gc, intelligence, estimated by a least squares use of the mava method. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 50, 253265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cool, V.A., & Keith, T. (1991). Testing a model of school learning: Direct and indirect effects on academic achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 16(1), 2844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
deLemos, M.M. (1989). Differential aptitude tests–Australian manual. Melbourne, Victoria: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Australian Council for Educational Research.Google Scholar
Edwards, R. (1994). Predicting academic achievement and counselling for course selection: an examination of the effectiveness of the differential tests in forecasting year 12 success. Australian Journal of Career Development, 3(2), 3136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, D.P., Genshaft, J.L., & Harrison, P.L. (1997). Contemporary intellectual assessment, theories, tests and issues. London: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Glossop, J.A., Appleyard, R., & Roberts, C. (1980). Achievement relative to a measure of general intelligence. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 49(3), 249257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graetz, B. (1990). Private schools and educational attainment: cohort and generational effects. Australian Journal of Education, 34(2), 174191.Google Scholar
Horn, J.L., & Cattell, R.B. (1968). Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57(5), 253270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. (2000). Let’s hear it for crystallized intelligence. Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 123129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. (1992). Commentary: vehicles of g. Psychological Science, 3(5), 275278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le, A.T. (2002). The rising education levels of females in Australia. Education Economics, 10(1), 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marksq, G.McMillan, J., & Hillman, K. (2001). Tertiary Entrance Performance: The role of student background and school factors. Longitudinal Surveys of Australian Youth, 22, 1102.Google Scholar
McGraw, B. (1985). Combining school based and external assessments of performance to determine university admissions. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 11, 303314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNemar, Q. (1964). Lost: Our intelligence? Why? American Psychologist, 19, 871882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, M., & Flynn, M. (1997). Quality of school life and students’ achievement in the HSC: A multilevel analysis. Australian Journal of Education, 41(2), 169188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omizo, M. (1982). The Differential Aptitude Tests as predictors of success in a high school for engineering program. Educational And Psychological Measurement, 40, 197203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, D. (1976). Differential aptitude test scores and vocational training program choice. Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance, 9(3), 101109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1904). General intelligence objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1967). The abilities of man. In Wiseman, S. (Ed.), Intelligence and ability (pp. 5868). Great Britain: Penguin Books. (Reprinted from The abilities of man, by C. Spearman, 1927, London: Macmillan)Google Scholar
Stankov, L. & Chen, K. (1988). Training and changes in fluid and crystallised intelligence. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13, 382397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stankov, L. (2000). The theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence new findings and recent developments. Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomdike, M. (1997). The early history of intelligence testing. In Flanagan, D.P.Genshaft, J.L. & Harrison, P.L. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment, theories, tests and issues (pp. 316). London: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Thomdike, R. (1986). The role of general ability in prediction. Journal of Vocational Behaviour, 29, 332339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. (1947). Multiple factor analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Undheim, J., & Gustafsson, J. (1987). The hierarchical organization of cognitive abilities: restoring general intelligence through the use of linear structural relations (LISREL). Multivariate Behavioural Research, 22(2), 149171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanderwood, M.L., McGrew, K.S., Flanagan, D.P., & Keith, T.Z. (2002). The contribution of general and specific cognitive abilities to reading achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 13, 159188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, V., (Ed). (1989). Academic achievement and aptitude testing: Practical applications and test reviews. USA: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar