Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T18:07:26.843Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Understanding emotion dysregulation from infancy to toddlerhood with a multilevel perspective: The buffering effect of maternal sensitivity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2024

Mindy A. Brown*
Affiliation:
Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA
Mengyu (Miranda) Gao
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Beijing Normal University School of Psychology, Beijing, China
Jennifer Isenhour
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Nila Shakiba
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
Sheila E. Crowell
Affiliation:
Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, USA
K. Lee Raby
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Elisabeth Conradt
Affiliation:
Psychology, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
*
Corresponding Author: Mindy A. Brown, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Challenges with childhood emotion regulation may have origins in infancy and forecast later social and cognitive developmental delays, academic difficulties, and psychopathology. This study tested whether markers of emotion dysregulation in infancy predict emotion dysregulation in toddlerhood, and whether those associations depended on maternal sensitivity. When children (N = 111) were 7 months, baseline respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), RSA withdrawal, and distress were collected during the Still Face Paradigm (SFP). Mothers’ reports of infant regulation and orientation and maternal sensitivity were also collected at that time. Mothers’ reports of toddlers’ dysregulation were collected at 18 months. A set of hierarchical regressions indicated that low baseline RSA and less change in RSA from baseline to stressor predicted greater dysregulation at 18 months, but only for infants who experienced low maternal sensitivity. Baseline RSA and RSA withdrawal were not significantly associated with later dysregulation for infants with highly sensitive mothers. Infants who exhibited low distress during the SFP and who had lower regulatory and orienting abilities at 7 months had higher dysregulation at 18 months regardless of maternal sensitivity. Altogether, these results suggest that risk for dysregulation in toddlerhood has biobehavioral origins in infancy but may be buffered by sensitive caregiving.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Development of emotion regulation is a crucial accomplishment of early childhood. Early emotional experiences and attempts to regulate those experiences serve important developmental functions for an infant (Abe & Izard, Reference Abe and Izard1999). Some of these functions are accomplished through synchronized dyadic interactions between caregivers and infants, formation of attachment bonds, and early social referencing behaviors (Abe & Izard, Reference Abe and Izard1999). Failure to develop emotion regulation abilities is associated with social and cognitive developmental challenges (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007; Penela et al., Reference Penela, Henderson, Hane, Ghera and Fox2012), academic difficulties (Graziano et al., Reference Graziano, Reavis, Keane and Calkins2007), maladaptive behavior such as aggression or withdrawal later in childhood (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Smith, Gill and Johnson1998; Hill et al., Reference Hill, Degnan, Calkins and Keane2006), and psychopathology (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010; Berking & Wupperman, Reference Berking and Wupperman2012; Keenan, Reference Keenan2000; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, Mennin and Nolen-Hoeksema2011). Current research tends to focus on single markers of emotion dysregulation at one time point. However, consistent with a developmental psychopathology perspective, the development of emotion regulation and dysregulation invariably occurs within a multilevel system (Adrian & Berk, Reference Adrian, Berk, Beauchaine and Crowell2020; Adrian et al., Reference Adrian, Zeman and Veits2011). The developmental process of acquiring emotion regulation skills is dependent on both innate infant characteristics, such as physiological arousal, behavioral affect, and an ability to orient and self-soothe (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Dedmon, Gill, Lomax and Johnson2002; Santucci et al., Reference Santucci, Silk, Shaw, Gentzler, Fox and Kovacs2008), as well as external markers, such as maternal sensitivity (Crockenberg & Leerkes, Reference Crockenberg and Leerkes2004; Fox & Calkins, Reference Fox and Calkins2003). Therefore, there is a need to better understand how both infant and caregiver influences affect child emotion dysregulation. Learning more about these processes, beginning in infancy, may increase our ability to detect risk for psychopathology much earlier in development (Mittal & Wakschlag, Reference Mittal and Wakschlag2017).

Defining emotion dysregulation in early childhood

Emotion dysregulation is not merely the absence or opposite of emotion regulation (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hahn, Crowell, Beauchaine and Crowell2020; Cole et al., Reference Cole, Martin and Dennis2004, Reference Cole, Hall, Hajal, Beauchaine, Hinshaw, Hinshaw and Beauchaine2017). Emotion regulation involves activation of a goal to influence emotions (conscious or automatic). Although some forms emotion regulation can emerge early in development (e.g., thumb sucking to self-soothe; Crockenberg & Leerkes, Reference Crockenberg and Leerkes2004; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum2010), most forms of emotion regulation are developed through socialization and coregulation, which become more sophisticated and require more cognitive ability as development progresses (Vohs & Baumeister, Reference Vohs and Baumeister2016). In contrast, early signs of emotion dysregulation may appear as simple expressions of emotion, such as distress, that is more intense than typical or age normative. Therefore, emotion dysregulation may arise and be detected earlier in development than emotion regulation (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hahn, Crowell, Beauchaine and Crowell2020). In this way, markers of emotion dysregulation may be particularly useful to observe during infancy to predict future dysregulation.

Despite the importance of studying emotion dysregulation in infancy, no consensus has been reached regarding its conceptualization (Webb et al., Reference Webb, Hawes, Campbell and Clear2020). Definitions have ranged from wide: “the failure to meet the developmental tasks of emotional development” (Cole et al., Reference Cole, Michel and Teti1994, p. 77) to more detailed: “an inability to respond to stimuli with well-maintained control” (Keenan, Reference Keenan2000, p. 420). For this paper, we define infant emotion dysregulation as the inability to maintain emotional control, engage with the environment, or recover from distress in a developmentally appropriate manner.

Infant markers of emotion dysregulation

Physiological stress response

One marker of infant emotion dysregulation may be physiological stress reactivity and regulation (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Glynn, Waffarn and Sandman2011). Parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) activity has been researched broadly among child, adolescent, and adult samples (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001, Reference Beauchaine2015b; Porges et al., Reference Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt and Maiti1994) but has received comparatively less empirical attention in infants compared to other stress response pathways (e.g., infant hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Glynn, Waffarn and Sandman2011). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) is one measure of PNS functioning (Porges, Reference Porges2007) and is an established correlate of mental health symptoms (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2001; Wagner & Waller, Reference Wagner and Waller2020). RSA is a measure of the rhythmic increases and decreases in heart rate that occur across the respiratory cycle due to vagal (i.e., parasympathetic) influences on heart rate (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Uchino and Berntson1994). RSA responses likely represent an infant’s emerging ability to cope and self-regulate in response to a stressor (Hinnant & El-Sheikh, Reference Hinnant and El-Sheikh2009; Porges, Reference Porges2007). During a threatening or challenging situation, the body withdraws vagal influence to allow for active responding. In the absence of a strong sympathetic nervous system influence, this withdrawal (leading to a decrease in RSA from baseline) often correlates with a higher heart rate. Vagal withdrawal also typically corresponds to shifting attentional and behavioral resources in response to threat or challenge and increased attention to the environment, which allow the individual to confront a threatening situation (Porges, Reference Porges2007). Vagal engagement (leading to increases in RSA from baseline) typically corresponds to slower heart rate and can help facilitate a return to homeostasis (Brooker & Buss, Reference Brooker and Buss2010; Porges, Reference Porges2007). Higher baseline RSA may reflect more positive and responsive emotional states in infancy while lower baseline RSA has been related to more adjustment and cognitive problems (Stifter & Corey, Reference Stifter and Corey2001). Decreases in RSA in response to stress (i.e., RSA withdrawal) may reflect more positive and responsive emotional states in infancy and better regulatory abilities (Calkins & Hill, Reference Calkins, Hill and Gross2007; Moore & Calkins, Reference Moore and Calkins2004). In contrast, smaller decreases in RSA from baseline to a stressor or increases in response to stress (i.e. RSA augmentation) have been related to more adjustment and cognitive problems in some studies (Graziano & Derefinko, Reference Graziano and Derefinko2013). It is thought that healthy RSA responses include returning to prestress, baseline levels after the stressor is removed (i.e., during recovery; see Porges & Furman, Reference Porges and Furman2011, for a review).

Behavioral affect

A second possible infant marker of infant emotion dysregulation is infant behavioral affect. Negative emotionality is a component of temperament that can reflect patterns of intense, distressed emotions (Buss & Plomin, Reference Buss, Plomin, Dunn and Plomin1986; Kim & Kochanska, Reference Kim and Kochanska2012). It can be used as an index of how quickly, strongly, and regularly the infant experiences negative emotions (Rothbart & Derryberry, Reference Rothbart, Derryberry, Backman and von Hofsten2002). Negative affect is hypothesized to interfere with healthy emotion regulation in two ways. First, prolonged negative emotionality overextends neural systems that underlie regulatory capacity, weakening their ultimate efficiency (Patterson et al., Reference Patterson, Lenartowicz, Berkman, Ji, Poldrack and Knowlton2016). Secondly, children who become overwhelmed by negative emotions likely have experienced failures to regulate their highly negative affect in the past, thus decreasing the chance that they will attempt to regulate in the future (Calkins & Degnan, Reference Calkins, Degnan and Ammerman2006; Petrenko et al., Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019). Empirical studies support this hypothesis. O’Connor (Reference O’Connor2001) found that negative affect in infants at 12 months was associated with the development of depressive symptoms at 6 years of age (O’Connor, Reference O’Connor2001). Similarly, Bridgett et al. (Reference Bridgett, Gartstein, Putnam, McKay, Iddins, Robertson, Ramsay and Rittmueller2009) discovered that high levels of negative affect in early infancy were associated with less ability to orient and regulate at 4–12 months (Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Gartstein, Putnam, McKay, Iddins, Robertson, Ramsay and Rittmueller2009). Finally, multiple researchers have shown that higher negative affect in infancy predicts lower effortful control in toddlers and preschoolers (for a review, see Lonigan et al., Reference Lonigan, Vasey, Phillips and Hazen2004).

Orientation and regulation

A third potential marker of emotion dysregulation in infancy concerns infant orientation and regulation, or the ability to focus attention and regulate behavior (Stifter & Dollar, Reference Stifter, Dollar and Cicchetti2016). The ability to focus may include behaviors such as sustained attention and pleasure at observing an object. Regulating may include behaviors such as enjoyment while being held by the caregiver or a reduction in crying or distress when the caregiver uses soothing techniques such a patting or rubbing their back. Because orientation and regulation may be one of the earliest manifestations of attention and regulatory functioning (Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Bridgett, Young, Panksepp and Power2013; Petrenko et al., Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019), deficits in this area could be an early marker of emotion dysregulation. Orientation and regulation in infancy provide a foundation for more effortful self-regulation later in childhood (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Murray and Harlan2000; Putnam & Rothbart, Reference Putnam and Rothbart2006). In turn, effortful control, or the child’s capacity to use attentional resources and inhibit behavioral responses in a way that can regulate their emotion (Rothbart et al., Reference Rothbart, Ahadi and Hershey1994), has been linked to later forms of emotion regulation (Petrenko et al., Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019). Low infant orientation and regulation have been associated with several maladaptive outcomes across the lifespan (Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Burt, Edwards and Deater-Deckard2015; Petrenko et al., Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019).

Caregiver associations with infant emotion regulation

During the first year of life, infants rely heavily on caregivers to aid with coregulation of their distress (Spangler et al., Reference Spangler, Schieche, Ilg, Maier and Ackermann1994). Infants can use behavioral cues such as facial expression, crying, tone of voice, and gestures to communicate (Weinberg & Tronick, Reference Weinberg and Tronick1994). Sensitive caregivers can recognize and respond to cues accurately, warmly, and in a timely manner (Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall1978, Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall2015) facilitating the infant’s ability to manage and express their emotions effectively (Landry et al., Reference Landry, Smith and Swank2006). Over time, infants and young children can learn about their emotions and how to respond to them by observing and modeling the expressiveness of a sensitive caregiver (McCoy & Raver, Reference McCoy and Raver2011). This process is central to the development of emotional regulation skills during early childhood. Infants and young children are highly attuned to the emotional cues of their caregivers from early on (Grossmann et al., Reference Grossmann, Grossmann, Fremmer-Bombik, Kindler, Scheuerer-Englisch and Zimmermann2005). Through repeated interactions, they begin to recognize patterns in facial expressions, tone of voice, and body language that convey various emotions (Thompson, Reference Thompson and Gross2014). A sensitive caregiver, who responds promptly and appropriately to the child’s emotional signals, provides a rich emotional environment for the child to learn from (Davidov & Grusec, Reference Davidov and Grusec2006). By observing how their caregiver expresses and regulates emotions in different situations, children start to internalize these strategies and develop their own repertoire of emotional regulation skills (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998). For example, if a caregiver calmly soothes a distressed child, the child may learn to self-soothe by imitating similar behaviors or seeking comfort from others when feeling upset (Denham et al., Reference Denham, Zoller and Couchoud1994). Moreover, the modeling of emotional expressiveness by a sensitive caregiver helps children to understand the range and complexity of emotions (Halberstadt et al., Reference Halberstadt, Cassidy, Stifter, Parke and Fox2001). They learn that it is normal to experience a variety of feelings and that emotions can be expressed in adaptive ways (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007). This understanding lays the foundation for developing empathy towards others and navigating social interactions effectively (Denham, Reference Denham1998). Through this attachment-mediated and social learning process of observation, imitation, and internalization, all within the context of sensitive parent-infant interactions, children gradually acquire the ability to recognize, label, and regulate their emotions more autonomously (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum2010). The presence of a securely attached, sensitive caregiver who provides a nurturing and responsive environment significantly contributes to this emotional development journey, fostering the emergence of adaptive emotional regulation skills that serve the child well into adulthood (Belsky & Fearon, Reference Belsky and Fearon2002). Sensitive caregiving in infancy has been associated with better physiological regulation (Perry & Calkins, Reference Perry, Calkins, Cole and Hollenstein2018), reduction of infant distress (Kopp, Reference Kopp1989), fewer behavioral problems, and higher levels of emotion regulation overall (Gunnar & Donzella, Reference Gunnar and Donzella2002; Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Blankson and O’Brien2009). Because of this dependence on caregivers for the regulation of infant emotional states during the first year, caregiver behaviors during this time can greatly shape the development of infant emotion regulation.

High maternal sensitivity may help reduce maladaptive emotional development. Crockenberg and Leerkes (Reference Crockenberg and Leerkes2003) found that highly sensitive mothers may buffer the risk of later anxious behavior for highly reactive infants. Sensitivity as a protective factor have been explored with infants in multiple studies (Haley & Stansbury, Reference Haley and Stansbury2003; Martinez-Torteya et al., Reference Martinez-Torteya, Dayton, Beeghly, Seng, McGinnis, Broderick, Rosenblum and Muzik2014; Porter et al., Reference Porter, Wouden-Miller, Silva and Porter2003; Schore, Reference Schore2001). Caregiver sensitivity has also been identified as a buffer for negative outcomes in toddlers and young children (Bernier et al., Reference Bernier, Carlson and Whipple2010; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Zhou, Spinrad, Valiente, Fabes and Liew2005; Gaertner et al., Reference Gaertner, Spinrad and Eisenberg2008; Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Blankson and O’Brien2009; Spinrad et al., Reference Spinrad, Stifter, Donelan-McCall and Turner2004, Reference Spinrad, Eisenberg, Gaertner, Popp, Smith, Kupfer, Greving, Liew and Hofer2007). For example, Penela et al. (Reference Penela, Henderson, Hane, Ghera and Fox2012) found that temperamental fear and poor socioemotional development was reduced with high-quality caregiving. Additionally, children whose mothers were high in sensitivity in the first 2 years of life were less likely to demonstrate behavior problems - a correlate of adaptive emotion regulation - at ages 2 and 3 years (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1998).

Conversely, children who experience less sensitive caregiving may be at greater risk of maladaptive development. Parents who are less responsive to infants’ distress may have infants who experience prolonged distress, which may compromise any attempt the infant makes at regulating their own emotions (e.g., Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Gartstein, Putnam, McKay, Iddins, Robertson, Ramsay and Rittmueller2009; Patterson et al., Reference Patterson, Lenartowicz, Berkman, Ji, Poldrack and Knowlton2016; Stifter & Spinrad, Reference Stifter and Spinrad2002). This prolonged distress may weaken the connectivity of neural systems that support response inhibition, such as the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, medial frontal cortex, and parietal cortex (Patterson et al., Reference Patterson, Lenartowicz, Berkman, Ji, Poldrack and Knowlton2016). Additionally, infants who do not expect responsive caregiving based on past experiences may experience excessive activation of other stress response systems, such as the sympathetic nervous system, HPA axis, and cardiovascular system, which can contribute to greater wear and tear on the body and worse health (Enlow et al., Reference Enlow, King, Schreier, Howard, Rosenfield, Ritz and Wright2014; Obrist, Reference Obrist1976). These studies suggest that when examining the development of infant emotion regulation, it is crucial to examine caregiver influences (Petrenko et al., Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019).

Interactions between infant markers and caregiver sensitivity

Interactions between infant characteristics and maternal sensitivity may be associated with differences in emotion regulation (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Letourneau, Campbell, Tomfohr-Madsen and Giesbrecht2017). High caregiver sensitivity can buffer the risks of high emotional reactivity. For example, among infants with greater negative temperament, those who experienced higher maternal sensitivity had a decreased chance that they would be emotionally dysregulated in later childhood (Crockenberg & Leerkes, Reference Crockenberg and Leerkes2006). Additionally, infants who had difficulty regulating fear displayed internalizing behavior as toddlers, but only if they experienced low caregiver sensitivity. High caregiver sensitivity functioned to buffer that link (Early et al., Reference Early, Rimm-Kaufman, Cox, Saluja, Pianta, Bradley and Payne2002; Penela et al., Reference Penela, Henderson, Hane, Ghera and Fox2012). Multiple studies have demonstrated that, among children with high emotionality, those whose caregivers were less sensitive exhibited poorer regulation, and those whose caregivers had high sensitivity displayed the best regulation skills (Kim & Kochanska, Reference Kim and Kochanska2012; Pluess & Belsky, Reference Pluess and Belsky2011; Roisman et al., Reference Roisman, Newman, Fraley, Haltigan, Groh and Haydon2012).

In contrast, low caregiver sensitivity may serve as a particular risk factor for infants who lack rudimentary emotion regulation skills. For example, among infants who demonstrated higher levels of negative affect, low caregiver sensitivity was related to less attention regulation and more avoidant behaviors (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Letourneau, Campbell, Tomfohr-Madsen and Giesbrecht2017). Lower levels of caregiving may also increase the risk of maladjustment, particularly when the infant’s regulatory skills are low (Wu & Feng, Reference Wu and Feng2020).

Developmental psychopathology perspective

Several researchers in the field agree that emotion dysregulation, being a complex, multifaceted, and dynamic construct, should be studied using a multimethod approach in order to capture a complete understanding of its development and the mechanisms involved (Adrian & Berk, Reference Adrian, Berk, Beauchaine and Crowell2020; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hahn, Crowell, Beauchaine and Crowell2020; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Vlisides-Henry, Kaliush, Beauchaine, Beauchaine and Crowell2020; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Schröder and von Scheve2014; Strauman, Reference Strauman2017; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Tolan and Leventhal2010; Webb et al., Reference Webb, Hawes, Campbell and Clear2020). Rather than resulting from one discrete emotional component, emotion regulation is the reciprocal interaction between multiple influences that creates emotional activation and regulation (Thompson, Reference Thompson2011a). These different systems, rather than always converging, may be continually and mutually interacting, becoming more integrated as the individual matures. Therefore, rather than studying infant emotion dysregulation using one method in isolation, researchers using multiple methods within the same sample would be in a better position to capture the complexity of the construct (Adrian & Berk, Reference Adrian, Berk, Beauchaine and Crowell2020).

However, very few studies to date have used more than one method to assess emotion dysregulation in young children (Webb et al., Reference Webb, Hawes, Campbell and Clear2020). Adrian et al. (Reference Adrian, Zeman and Veits2011) conducted a 35-year review of emotion regulation assessment, including those used with infants. Out of the 157 studies in 42 major journals measuring emotion regulation, 61.1% used only one method of measurement, 23.6% used two methods, 10.8% used three methods, and 4.5% used four methods (Adrian et al., Reference Adrian, Zeman and Veits2011). Webb et al. (Reference Webb, Hawes, Campbell and Clear2020) argued that each measurement category has its own unique strengths and limitations. Given this, as emotion regulation is defined by the methods used to assess it, they recommended using a multimethod approach, which will provide a more comprehensive view of the construct. Furthermore, the majority of these and other emotion regulation and dysregulation studies focused on toddlers and older children rather than infants (Adrian et al., Reference Adrian, Zeman and Veits2011; Petrenko et al., Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019). Petrenko et al. (Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019) observed that most research focuses on the development of regulatory capacity in toddlers and older children. However, many regulatory behaviors in infancy precede those in toddlerhood (such as orienting and regulation preceding effortful control). Thus, they contended that it is crucial to study these key regulatory behaviors in infancy, as those behaviors are foundational to the development of later regulation.

Current study

Embracing a developmental psychopathology perspective, the goal of this study was to explore the development of infant emotion dysregulation by using a multidimensional approach. Three major markers of emotion dysregulation in infancy were explored to discover which markers contributed to the development of emotion dysregulation in toddlerhood. Although dysregulation changes across development, research has shown stability between early-life regulatory problems (e.g., excessive crying, sleep and feeding difficulties) and later childhood dysregulation (e.g., negative emotionality, conduct problems, hyperactivity; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Puzia and Yaptangco2015; Winsper & Wolke, Reference Winsper and Wolke2014). The presence of multiple regulatory problems, including excessive crying, sleep and feeding difficulties, is conceptualized as “dysregulation” in toddlerhood and is linked to negative outcomes (Hemmi et al., Reference Hemmi, Wolke and Schneider2011; Winsper & Wolke, Reference Winsper and Wolke2014). The current study also investigated which of these markers may be moderated by maternal sensitivity, indicating possible intervention opportunities.

Our first aim was to examine whether biobehavioral markers of emotion dysregulation during infancy predict emotion dysregulation a year later when children are toddler. We hypothesized that lower baseline RSA during infancy would predict higher levels of toddler emotion dysregulation (Hypothesis 1a), less RSA withdrawal during infancy would predict greater toddler emotion dysregulation (Hypothesis 1b), higher levels of distress during infancy would predict greater toddler emotion dysregulation (Hypothesis 1c), and lower levels of orienting and regulation during infancy would predict greater toddler emotion dysregulation (Hypothesis 1d).

Our second aim was to test whether the associations between these four markers of emotion dysregulation during infancy and toddlers’ emotion dysregulation are moderated by maternal sensitivity. We hypothesized that maternal sensitivity would buffer infants who exhibited risk for emotion dysregulation at age 7 months (Hypothesis 2). In other words, the associations articulated in Hypotheses 1a-1d would only be significant for infants who experienced low sensitivity. In contrast, markers of emotion dysregulation during infancy were not expected to be associated with toddler emotion dysregulation when mothers were highly sensitive.

Methods

Participants

Data for this study were drawn from a prospective, longitudinal study exploring the intergenerational transmission of emotion dysregulation during the prenatal and early postnatal periods. For the present study, data were drawn from 111 mother-infant dyads during 7- and 18-month assessments. Infants were 47% female (53% male). The majority (65.5%) of infants were White, 34.9% Hispanic/Latine, with 10% or less of other racial/ethnic groups. Fifty-nine percent of women had a household income of $50,000 or more per year, 17% were between $30,000 and $49,000, and 22% made $29,000 or less per year in their household. Fifty-six percent of women were college graduates or had post graduate education, 42% were high school graduates and may have had some college experience, and 2 percent had not graduated high school. Most women were married (79%), 15% were single, never married, and 6% were separated or divorced.

Procedures

All women provided a written informed consent before every assessment and digitally before completing online questionnaires. Women were oversampled for emotion dysregulation. (More details about recruitment, study design, eligibility criteria, and assessment procedures can be found in Lin et al., Reference Lin, Kaliush, Conradt, Terrell, Neff, Allen, Smid, Monk and Crowell2019.) All study procedures were approved by the University of Utah and Intermountain Medical Center Institutional Review Board.

We obtained RSA data from dyads during the still-face paradigm at 7 months. Infant regulation/orientation data from mother report were obtained from online questionnaires. We were also able to code behavioral distress from these infants, and maternal sensitivity data from these mothers. Missing data were due to video being unscorable (e.g., poor camera angle) or video or software malfunction. Of these numbers, 86% completed follow-up questionnaires when their infants neared 18 months of age. Attrition occurred due to families moving, mothers declining to continue, or being unable to reach the family after repeated attempts.

The 7-month and 18-month timepoints were chosen because at these ages infants and toddlers are learning important regulatory skills, in part via interactions with their primary caregivers. Furthermore, these timepoints align with existing research, which allows us to more easily compare findings across studies and build upon previous knowledge (Braungart-Rieker et al., Reference Braungart-Rieker, Zentall, Lickenbrock, Ekas, Oshio and Planalp2014; Kiel & Kalomiris, Reference Kiel and Kalomiris2015; Peltola et al., Reference Peltola, Forssman, Puura, van IJzendoorn and Leppänen2015; Aktar & Pérez-Edgar, Reference Aktar, Pérez-Edgar, Lockman and Tamis-LeMonda2020; Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Gartstein, Putnam, Lance, Iddins, Waits and Lee2011; Mangelsdorf et al., Reference Mangelsdorf, Shapiro and Marzolf1995). By aligning with existing research, we felt it would be easier to compare findings across studies and build upon previous knowledge.

The most prominent was the cognitive and emotional development of the young children. By 18 months, the child has undergone significant developmental changes, including language acquisition, social cognition, and self-regulation. We wanted to capture these developmental changes and their influence on other variables of interest to us in the study.

Prior to the 7-month visit, women completed a series of questionnaires online (mean infant age = 6.61 months, SD = .93). The survey included questions regarding the participant’s age, socioeconomic status, educational background, and both mother’s and baby’s race and ethnicity.

Mothers also completed the Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ; Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003; Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Helbig, Gartstein, Rothbart and Leerkes2014), which is a questionnaire measure of infant temperament, at this time. Mothers received a link to the measures via Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap) email before attending the 7-month lab visit. Researchers attached heart rate and respiration monitoring equipment on both mothers and infants during lab visits.

Still-face paradigm and free play

Following a baseline assessment, infants were placed in a highchair and the experimenters introduced the Still-Face Paradigm (SFP; Haley & Stansbury, Reference Haley and Stansbury2003; Tronick et al., Reference Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise and Brazelton1978), which is a validated experimental procedure developed to assess socioemotional regulation in infants (Giusti et al., Reference Giusti, Provenzi and Montirosso2018). During the SFP, mothers interact with their infants in a play, still-face, and recovery episode (Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, van Ijzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg2009; Moore & Calkins, Reference Moore and Calkins2004; Tronick et al., Reference Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise and Brazelton1978).

The procedure was stopped if the infant became too upset, such as uninterrupted crying for 10–15 s, or if the mother requested a stop on the procedure. Continuous measures of heart rate and RSA were collected during the still-face paradigm. At the end of the 7-month laboratory visit, mother-infant dyads engaged in a free play interaction with their infants. Mothers were instructed to play with their infants as they normally would on a carpet for five minutes without toys and 10 minutes with age-appropriate toys that were provided.

Follow-up assessment

When infants neared 18 months old, their mothers (regardless of whether they attended the 7-month visit) were contacted via email or phone and invited to complete online questionnaires (via RedCap software) prior to a lab visit. From the 18-month visit, only data from the online questionnaires were used for this analysis. Among these online questionnaires was the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA).

Measures

Infant respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA)

Electrocardiogram data were collected from infants using a two-lead configuration with spot electrodes placed on the right clavicle and left ribcage (Qu et al., Reference Qu, Zhang, Webster and Tompkins1986) using MindWare mobile devices (MindWare Technologies Ltd., Gahanna, OH; Biolab software version 3.1). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) was defined as the high frequency band of the power spectrum waveform (0.24–1.04 Hz for infants) and was scored in 30-second epochs by trained research assistants using Mindware’s heart rate variability analysis software. This software flags peaks of the R wave within each QRS complex (the graphical depiction of a heartbeat) and records the interval between adjacent R peaks (i.e., interbeat intervals). The software identifies whether the interbeat intervals are within an expected range for the time series, as well as whether they are within expected deviations considering surrounding data. Trained research assistants reviewed the flagged R peaks and made corrections when necessary (i.e., misidentified R peak). Epochs were considered missing if: (1) there were ≥ 5 s of missing or unusable data or (2) RSA values fell outside of the expected range of 1–10. RSA data were double-checked by a senior investigator when necessary. Infant RSA was collected during the SFP. Baseline scores were calculated by averaging the RSA values during the first episode of the SFP (i.e., baseline play episode). Change scores were calculated by subtracting the mean RSA of the first episode (baseline play) from that of the second episode (still-face). Therefore, lower scores reflect greater withdrawal (more decrease in RSA from baseline to task, i.e., lower scores reflect lower values).

Infant behavioral distress

Infants’ affect and attention during the still-face episode of the SFP were coded in 1-second intervals by trained research assistants using the Infant and Caregiver Engagement Phases coding (Weinberg & Tronick, Reference Weinberg and Tronick1999). Infants’ behavior was assigned one of seven mutually exclusive codes using Noldus Observer: protest, withdrawal, negative engagement, engagement with an object or the environment, social monitor, positive social engagement, or unscorable. For the purposes of predicting emotion dysregulation, three codes: negative engagement, protest, and withdrawal were added to create a measure of “infant distress,” which was used in this analysis. Coders were blind to all other mother-infant data.

Proportion scores for each of the codes were calculated and ranged from 1.00 (indicating the infant engaged in that behavior 100% of the time) and 0 (indicating the infant did not engage in that behavior). Thirty-one of the observations were double-coded, and the interrater reliability (intraclass correlation) for the measure of infant distress during the still-face episode was .996.

Infant behavior questionnaire (IBQ): regulatory control/orienting

Mothers completed the Infant Behavior Questionnaire Revised Short Form (IBQ-R; Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003; Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Helbig, Gartstein, Rothbart and Leerkes2014) online with other questionnaires at the 7-month time point. The IBQ-R Short Form consists of 91 items and 14 scales and is a parent-report measure of infant temperament, based on the IBQ developed by Rothbart (1981). Mothers rated the frequency that their infant engaged in specific day-to-day behaviors in the prior one to two weeks using a 7-point scale, with responses ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always). The Orienting/Regulation dimension, which is used in this analysis, includes the subscale scores of low intensity pleasure, cuddliness, duration of orienting, and soothability. Each of these subscales includes 6-7 items, such as “When rocking your baby, how often did s/he take more than 10 minutes to soothe?” This dimension had a good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .75; Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Helbig, Gartstein, Rothbart and Leerkes2014).

Maternal sensitivity at 7 months

Video-recorded interactions during the two nondistressing play episodes were coded using five-point scales capturing sensitivity, intrusiveness, detachment, and positive regard. These scales were adapted from the Observational Record of the Caregiving Environment and (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1998.) The sensitivity scale assessed the mother’s ability to follow her infant’s lead by responding appropriately to the infant’s signals. The intrusiveness scale assessed the mother’s engagement in psychologically or physically directive behaviors. The detachment scale assessed mothers’ lack of emotional engagement and awareness of infants’ needs. The positive regard scale assessed mothers’ expressions of positive affect and delight directed toward the infant during the interaction. Researchers assigned ratings based on both quality and quantity of mother behavior using a 1-5 scale with: 1 = Not at all characteristic, to 5 = Highly characteristic. All videos were double coded by trained coders, and disagreements greater than one scale point were conferenced. For cases with disagreements that were less than one scale point, the average scores were used in the analyses. Intraclass correlations for the ratings of caregiver behavior were between .56 and .83. A composite measure of overall maternal sensitivity during the play interactions was created by averaging the four ratings (after reverse scoring maternal intrusiveness and detachment; α = 0.61).

Emotion dysregulation in toddlers

Emotion dysregulation in this study was measured at 18 months using the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA; Carter et al., Reference Carter, Briggs-Gowan, Jones and Little2003). The ITSEA is a caregiver-report tool that measures social-emotional problems and competencies in children 12–35 months old (Carter et al., Reference Carter, Briggs-Gowan, Jones and Little2003). Mothers completed this form when their infants neared 18 months of age. For this analysis we used the Dysregulation domain, which consists of 4 subscales (Negative Emotionality, Sleep, Eating, and Sensory Sensitivity) and 34 items. Some example items include: “Cries or has tantrums until he or she is exhausted,” “Is impatient or easily frustrated,” and “Is whiny or fussy when he or she is NOT tired.” The dysregulation domain had an acceptable Cronbach’s alpha of .66.

Analytic plan

The percentage of missing values ranged from 0 for some variables to as high as 21.62% for 7-month observed distress, and only about 78% of the participants in the sample would have been available for analysis under the listwise deletion method. Data are missing due to non-response, attrition, equipment failure, infant distress, or family move. No discernible pattern appeared after checking for missing data patterns via SPSS. Those who atritted did not differ in demographics or key variables assessed at 7 months. We addressed the problem of missing data using the multiple imputation technique including all analysis variables under the assumption that variables are missing at random (Schafer & Graham, Reference Schafer and Graham2002). SPSS’s multiple imputation command generated 80 imputed datasets and 500 iterations (Van Buuren, Reference Van Buuren2018). Datasets were pooled and analysis run on imputed dyadic data (N = 111). Imputed values compare reasonably to observed values, and results using listwise deletion are similar to multiple imputation, so imputed regression results are presented (Manly & Wells, Reference Manly and Wells2015). For ease of investigating simple slopes, the bar procedure was used as pooling technique. This procedure was created by Baranzini (Reference Baranzini2018) and facilitates the compression of several imputed data frame outputs into a single imputed data frame. The bar procedure pools and creates a mean of the multiply-imputed values as they are generated by the algorithm, retaining between-sample variability in the estimates. This procedure has also been used by other researchers (Orkorn, Reference Okorn, Verhoeven and Van Baar2022; van der Kamp et al., Reference van der Kamp, Klaver, Thio, Driessen, de Jongh, Tabak and Hermens2020).

Five hierarchical linear regressions were conducted. Each of the first four regressions focused on one of the four measures of early emotion dysregulation. A fifth regression was conducted to determine whether significant effects held independently of one another. This fifth regression included all significant main effects and interactions, which included observed infant distress, mother-reported orienting and regulatory control, baseline RSA-maternal sensitivity interaction, and RSA withdrawal-maternal sensitivity interaction. Demographic variables were screened for possible inclusion in Step 1 of the hierarchical regression as covariates but were ultimately not included because of non-correlation with study variables. Step 2, testing for main effects, included the individual infant predictor (baseline RSA, RSA withdrawal, infant distress, and infant orienting and regulation), mean-centered. Step 3 included maternal sensitivity, mean-centered. Step 4, testing for an interaction between the predictor and maternal sensitivity, included a variable created by multiplying the predictor and the maternal sensitivity variable. Significant interactions were probed at three levels: -1 standard deviation (SD), mean, and + 1SD, using an online computational tool (Preacher et al., Reference Preacher, Curran and Bauer2006), to determine simple slopes.

Results

Prior to conducting primary analyses, demographic characteristics of participants were assessed. Correlations and descriptive statistics for predictor and outcome variables Table 1 and Table 2.

Table 1. Correlations

Note. *p < .05, **p < .01, (two = tailed.).

Table 2. Measures’ descriptive statistics

Note. N = sample size, M = mean, SD = standard deviation, RSA = respiratory sinus arrhythmia, IBQ = Infant Behavior Questionnaire, ITSEA = Infant-Toddler Social Emotional Assessment.

Model 1: Infant baseline RSA predicting dysregulation

Results of the first linear regression model indicated that neither baseline RSA nor maternal sensitivity were significantly associated with toddler emotion dysregulation as measured by the ITSEA (see Table 3). However, there was a significant interaction between baseline RSA and maternal sensitivity at 7 months (β = .26, t = 2.69, p = .008; See Table 3). Simple slopes analyses showed low baseline RSA at 7 months predicted higher dysregulation at 18 months, but only for infants whose mothers had low sensitivity (B = −.09, t = −2.26, p = .03; Fig. 1). The associations between baseline RSA and toddler emotion dysregulation were not significant for infants with average and high maternal sensitivity (average: B = −0.05, t = −1.61, p = .11; high: B = .05, t = 1.30, p = .20).

Figure 1. Baseline RSA and maternal sensitivity interaction plot. Independent variables were centered for graphical representation. Results from Model 1 show significance only for infants who have experienced low maternal sensitivity; ***p < .001 (two-tailed).

Table 3. Regression results

Note. N = 111; B = unstandardized coefficient, SE = standard error, β = standardized coefficient, Beta value, t = t value, p = p value/2-tailed significance, R 2 = goodness-of-fit measure; *p < .05,.

**p < .01, ***p < .001, (two-tailed.).

Model 2: Infant RSA withdrawal predicting dysregulation

Results of the second linear regression model indicated that neither RSA withdrawal nor maternal sensitivity at 7 months were significantly associated with toddler emotion dysregulation as measured by the ITSEA. However, there was a significant interaction between RSA withdrawal to the still face and maternal sensitivity (β = −.26, t = −2.88, p = .005; see Table 3). Simple slopes analyses showed that lower RSA withdrawal (less change in RSA from baseline to the still-face episode) predicted higher dysregulation at 18 months, but only among infants of mothers with low (B = .07, t = 3.67, p = .0004) or average sensitivity (B = .01, t = −0.29, p = .02; see Fig. 2.) The association between RSA withdrawal and toddler emotion dysregulation was not significant for infants of mothers with high sensitivity (B = .05, t = −0.92, p = .36).

Figure 2. RSA withdrawal and maternal sensitivity interaction plot. Independent variables were centered for graphical representation. RSA withdrawal = still-face task mean minus baseline play mean. Lower scores represent greater withdrawal, or more decrease from baseline to task. Higher scores represent less withdrawal from baseline to task. Because the mean value of RSA change was negative (see Table 3), positive values on the mean-centered x-axis reflect actual RSA withdrawal values that are close to zero (reflecting a lack of change in RSA levels from play to still-face episodes) and not positive RSA changes (which would indicate that RSA levels increased from baseline to still-face). Results from Model 2 show significance only for infants who have experienced low maternal sensitivity; *p < .05, **p < .01 (two-tailed). RSA withdrawal = still-face task mean minus baseline play mean. Lower scores represent greater withdrawal, or more decrease from baseline to task. Higher scores represent less withdrawal or increase from baseline to task.

Model 3: Infant behavioral distress predicting dysregulation

Results of the third linear regression model indicated that higher observed distress during the still-face paradigm at 7 months predicted lower toddler dysregulation at 18 months, contrary to hypothesis 1c (β = −.20, t = −2.16, p = .03). This association was not significantly moderated by maternal sensitivity (see Table 3).

Model 4: Infant orientation and regulation predicting dysregulation

Results of the third linear regression model indicated that higher levels of mother-reported infant regulation and orientation at 7 months predicted lower levels of toddler dysregulation at 18 months, supporting hypothesis 1d (β = −.22, t = −2.30, p = .02). This association was not significantly moderated by maternal sensitivity (see Table 3).

Model 5: Including all significant variables in the model

Results of the fifth linear regression model indicated that when the significant main effects of infant dysregulation and interactions with maternal sensitivity were included in the same model predicting toddler dysregulation, only observed infant distress (B = −.07, p = .04) and RSA withdrawal-maternal sensitivity interaction (B = −.08, p = .03) remained significant (see Table 3).

Discussion

Emotion dysregulation in childhood may portend problems with social and cognitive development, academic difficulties, maladaptive behavior such as aggression or withdrawal, and psychopathology later in life (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010; Berking & Wupperman, Reference Berking and Wupperman2012; Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Smith, Gill and Johnson1998; Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Graziano et al., Reference Graziano, Reavis, Keane and Calkins2007; Hill et al., Reference Hill, Degnan, Calkins and Keane2006; Keenan, Reference Keenan2000; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, Mennin and Nolen-Hoeksema2011; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007; Penela et al., Reference Penela, Henderson, Hane, Ghera and Fox2012). This study was designed to test whether markers of risk for emotion dysregulation in infancy could be used to predict risk for emotion dysregulation in toddlers. Because of the coregulation that occurs between mother and infant, we tested whether the effect of infant emotion dysregulation on child emotion dysregulation was moderated by maternal sensitivity. Since emotion dysregulation is a multidimensional process, studying it using only one level of analysis may not capture its complexity. Few studies to our knowledge use more than one method of assessing emotion dysregulation, especially in infancy, a time when regulatory processes develop rapidly (Kopp, Reference Kopp1982). Currently, the field is limited by single-variable studies that do not consider the contributions of multiple variables, thereby limiting interpretability and applicability to intervention. Using a multilevel, longitudinal dataset, our study aimed to address these issues. Overall, our results showed that multiple biobehavioral markers in infancy can predict risk for dysregulation in toddlerhood. Our results also suggest that maternal sensitivity may serve as a buffer for infants with certain RSA responses.

Baseline RSA and maternal sensitivity

We hypothesized that the association between infant baseline RSA and toddler dysregulation would only vary for infants of mothers who displayed lower levels of sensitivity. This hypothesis was supported. Among infants who experienced lower levels of caregiving sensitivity at 7 months, lower baseline RSA predicted higher levels of dysregulation. For infants who experienced average or high caregiving sensitivity at 7 months, baseline RSA was not predictive of dysregulation in toddlerhood. These results suggest that low caregiver sensitivity may be a risk factor for infants with low baseline RSA. Baseline RSA is a marker of infant stress withdrawal and capacity for regulation, and infants with a lower baseline RSA may struggle with physiological aspects of emotion regulation, thus predisposing them to more negative and less responsive emotional states during times of stress (Stifter & Corey, Reference Stifter and Corey2001).

Researchers suggest that the level of sensitivity in caregiving may affect children’s regulatory development. Kim and Kochanska (Reference Kim and Kochanska2012) found that infants high in negative affect were more likely to lack regulatory skills when they had less responsive relationships with their mothers (Kim & Kochanska, Reference Kim and Kochanska2012). Conversely, Spangler (Reference Spangler1990) showed that high maternal responsiveness was associated with less “difficult temperament” (e.g., approachability, adaptability, intensity, mood) at 12 months and higher social competence at 2 years (Spangler, Reference Spangler1990). In sum, in the absence of a sensitive caregiver, some infants may struggle to develop effective self-regulatory strategies.

RSA withdrawal and maternal sensitivity

Like baseline RSA, we hypothesized that the association between RSA withdrawal and later dysregulation would only be significant for infants who experienced lower levels of maternal sensitivity. This hypothesis was also supported by our results. For infants whose mothers displayed average or low sensitivity, lower RSA withdrawal (i.e., a more blunted response) to distress predicted higher dysregulation. As expected, among infants whose mothers were more sensitive, levels of RSA withdrawal were not predictive of later dysregulation.

During the still-face paradigm, infants are expected to show vagal withdrawal leading to lower RSA, and many researchers have established this pattern (Bazhenova et al., Reference Bazhenova, Plonskaia and Porges2001; Moore & Calkins, Reference Moore and Calkins2004; Weinberg & Tronick, Reference Weinberg and Tronick1996). Higher RSA withdrawal in infants and children has been linked to better soothability, attentional control (Huffman et al., Reference Huffman, Bryan, del Carmen, Pedersen, Doussard-Roosevelt and Forges1998), behavioral distraction (Calkins, Reference Calkins1997), and lower levels of aggression, reactivity, and depression (Perry et al., Reference Perry, Calkins and Bell2016; Porges et al., Reference Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, Portales and Greenspan1996). In contrast, less RSA withdrawal has been empirically linked with more dysregulated behavior in infants, particularly in the context of a less sensitive caregiving relationship. Moore and Calkins (Reference Moore and Calkins2004) showed that less RSA withdrawal was linked to less positive affect and less synchrony in play with their mothers (Moore & Calkins, Reference Moore and Calkins2004). As the interaction results in this study suggest, infants who have experienced low or average maternal sensitivity are at particular risk for later dysregulation if they demonstrate less RSA withdrawal. In the context of the still-face, if we assume that less RSA withdrawal in an infant indicates more difficulty regulating emotions, then it follows that those same infants, particularly those whose caregiver was not responsive, would also be more likely to struggle with dysregulation in toddlerhood. Conversely, those infants who display more withdrawal may be demonstrating their ability to effectively respond to a stressor and regulate their emotions in the absence of a sensitive caregiver, making it more likely that they are also able to do this in toddlerhood.

For both baseline RSA and RSA withdrawal, it is possible that higher caregiver sensitivity buffered the effects of physiological dysregulation (i.e., low baseline RSA and low RSA withdrawal). Based on the quality of their past interactions, infants who do not expect or receive sensitive caregiver responses to distress may also experience activation of other stress response systems, such as the sympathetic nervous system (Enlow et al., Reference Enlow, King, Schreier, Howard, Rosenfield, Ritz and Wright2014). This repeated stress response system activation may contribute to “wear and tear” on the body, poor health, and lower ability to effectively cope with stress in the future (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Hill-Soderlund, Propper, Calkins, Mills-Koonce and Cox2009). The buffering effect of maternal sensitivity may therefore play an important protective role for infants at risk for maladaptive stress responses. Infants who initially had poorer RSA functioning, but were exposed to high levels of maternal sensitivity, may have demonstrated greater growth in baseline RSA and withdrawal over time, leading to more adaptive emotion regulation.

Infant behavioral distress

While it is normative and expected for infants to become distressed during the still-face episode, we expected that the infants who become distressed more quickly, more intensely, and for a higher percentage of time, would be more likely to have a higher dysregulation score at 18 months. Instead, we found that higher levels of infant distress in response to maternal unavailability was associated with lower dysregulation at 18 months. For this analysis, only the distress data that were coded during the still-face episode were used. This was the period that the infants’ mothers were not providing their infants with social feedback. We did not analyze distress that was displayed during the reunion period, when mother and infant were given the opportunity to resolve this social stressor. It is possible that, even if the infant was highly distressed, their ability to later regulate and recover during a reunion period may be more predictive of future regulation than the initial reaction to the stressor.

It is also possible that exhibiting distress is a form of regulation that infants use at this developmental stage (Bell & Ainsworth, Reference Bell and Ainsworth1972). Perhaps infants who were able to express their needs (i.e., by communicating distress) were more likely to receive appropriate attention from their caregiver. On the other hand, it is possible that exhibiting low distress in response to the stress of maternal unavailability is a risk factor for later problems with emotion regulation. Infants who displayed less distress during this stressor may have been displaying blunted affect, indicative of withdrawn behavior that is not considered a typical response to the still-face episode in infancy (Guedeney et al., Reference Guedeney, Matthey and Puura2013; Tronick & Cohn, Reference Tronick and Cohn1989). Low distress during the still face is non-normative and may reflect early adversity or poor relational history between mothers and infants.

Mother-reported infant orienting and regulatory control

As expected, infants whose mothers reported higher levels of orienting and regulatory control at 7 months were less likely to be dysregulated at 18 months. These skills are believed to be one of the earliest types of regulatory functioning that manifests in the infant (Gartstein & Rothbart, Reference Gartstein and Rothbart2003; Petrenko et al., Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019; Rothbart & Derryberry, Reference Rothbart, Derryberry, Lamb and Brown1981). Regulatory control and orientation are foundational for developing effortful control, which aids the child in using their own attentional resources and inhibit their behaviors in ways that regulate emotions (Rothbart et al., Reference Rothbart, Ahadi and Hershey1994). Infants who are developing these foundational skills by 7 months are more likely to continue an adaptive development in regulatory abilities, thereby being less likely to exhibit symptoms of dysregulation at 18 months. Past research has shown that low regulatory control and orientation early in life have been associated with several maladaptive outcomes across the lifespan (Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Burt, Edwards and Deater-Deckard2015; Petrenko et al., Reference Petrenko, Kanya, Rosinski, McKay and Bridgett2019).

Contrary to our predictions, the behavioral markers of risk for emotion dysregulation did not significantly interact with caregiving sensitivity to predict emotion dysregulation in toddlers. It may be that these behavioral measures were more robust predictors of emotion dysregulation than the physiological predictors. Physiological markers can be measured at birth or even prenatally, yet behavioral markers, (while they still may appear early in infancy), may be easier to assess in a later stage of regulatory development, reflecting a more embedded part of the infant’s regulatory repertoire. Although physiological traits may be buffered by high maternal sensitivity, it is possible that behavioral traits may persist even when a sensitive caregiver is present.

It is also possible that, since RSA was still maturing at the developmental stage that we were observing, it was particularly amenable to caregiver influence. Polyvagal theory may provide an explanation for this link between autonomic regulation and maternal sensitivity (Porges, Reference Porges2007; Porges et al., Reference Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt and Maiti1994, Reference Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, Portales and Greenspan1996, Reference Porges, Davila, Lewis, Kolacz, Okonmah‐Obazee, Hane, Kwon, Ludwig, Myers and Welch2019; Porges & Furman, Reference Porges and Furman2011). This theory describes neural pathways that regulate autonomic reactivity and show that autonomic state provides a platform for infant-caregiver coregulation. Vagal pathways are bidirectionally linked with areas of the brain that control aspects of social engagement (e.g., facial expression, gaze, vocalizations). This connection has been demonstrated in an intervention study with preterm infants and their mothers. Infants of mothers who engaged in this intervention, which included enhanced maternal care and sensitivity, exhibited enhanced autonomic regulation (Porges et al., Reference Porges, Davila, Lewis, Kolacz, Okonmah‐Obazee, Hane, Kwon, Ludwig, Myers and Welch2019). Additionally, improved emotional connection between caregiver and infants may increase the maturity of the vagal regulation system through increased production of oxytocin that comes with increased social interaction (Welch & Ludwig, Reference Welch and Ludwig2017).

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

Our study has several strengths, including a longitudinal design and the use of multiple markers of infant emotion dysregulation. However, our findings should be interpreted in the context of limitations. Firstly, 7-month infant orientation and regulation was assessed via caregiver-report. Although the other three measures of infant emotion dysregulation at the 7-month timepoint were either physiological measures or coded by trained lab experimenters, this measure may have been biased or influenced by other factors, including mother’s mood and/or her own level of emotion dysregulation. However, a strength of this mother-report measure is that mothers report on infant behavior across multiple contexts and over time. Secondly, the 18-month measure of infant dysregulation was also a caregiver-report measure. While this measure has been shown to be a reliable measure and predictive of childhood outcomes, it may be more highly correlated with the mother-report of infant orientation and regulation at 7 months because of the effects of common-method variance. Thirdly, we chose to use baseline RSA and RSA withdrawal as our physiological predictors, given that RSA appears to be a reliable and valid peripheral biomarker of emotion dysregulation. However, as some scholars suggest, it is important for future research to include markers from multiple stress response systems, such as pre-ejection period, electrodermal activity, and cortisol regulation patterns, to further examine how complex multisystem activity contributes to the development of emotion dysregulation (Rash et al., Reference Rash, Thomas, Campbell, Letourneau, Granger and Giesbrecht2016; Suurland et al., Reference Suurland, van der Heijden, Huijbregts, van Goozen and Swaab2018). Additionally, our sample size is relatively small. Furthermore, although our sample included a sizable proportion of Hispanic/Latine infants, (34.1%), the largest subgroup was White and non-Hispanic/Latine (68.9%). It is unclear whether our findings can be generalized to other samples with different racial and ethnic identities. Lastly, maternal sensitivity was assessed during a play period rather than during a distress episode. This may be a limitation, as some researchers have found maternal sensitivity during distress to be a more salient predictor of affective dysregulation (Leerkes et al., Reference Leerkes, Blankson and O’Brien2009). We noticed that many of our pregnant participants found watching an infant play to be more stressful (as indicated by maternal prenatal sympathetic nervous system responses) than watching an infant cry. We therefore decided to examine maternal sensitivity during the free play episode, rather than sensitivity to infant distress.

Conclusions

Knowledge of how specific indicators of infant dysregulation in early life confer subsequent risk for later dysregulation is lacking, as is a deep understanding of how caregiving may operate as a moderator in relation to these indicators. This study used a rich, longitudinal dataset that explored the risk of four different indicators of infant dysregulation and the possible moderation of maternal sensitivity. This study elucidates the significance of exploring infancy in the development of emotion dysregulation. We demonstrated that risk for toddler emotion dysregulation can be predicted as early as 7 months. Infants with a certain RSA pattern (low baseline RSA and less RSA withdrawal) may be at risk for toddler dysregulation, but only in the context of low caregiver sensitivity. Higher caregiver sensitivity appeared to buffer this risk. These findings provide guidance on for whom early intervention may be most beneficial. Implementing interventions focused on increasing caregiver sensitivity, especially for infants with specific parasympathetic nervous system responses to stress, may reduce risk for the emergence of behavior dysregulation in toddlers. Measurement of multiple markers of emotion regulation and dysregulation in infancy may be important to fully understanding the development of toddler emotion dysregulation.

References

Abe, J. A. A., & Izard, C. E. (1999). The developmental functions of emotions: An analysis in terms of differential emotions theory. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 523549. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379177 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adrian, M., & Berk, M. (2020). Behavioral assessment of emotion dysregulation. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Crowell, S. E. (Ed.), The oxford handbook of emotion dysregulation (pp. 377394). Oxford University Press, https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190689285.001.0001 Google Scholar
Adrian, M., Zeman, J., & Veits, G. (2011). Methodological implications of the affect revolution: A 35-year review of emotion regulation assessment in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(2), 171197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: Assessed in the strange situation and at home. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (2015). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aktar, E., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2020). Infant emotion development and temperament. In Lockman, J. J., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (Ed.), The cambridge handbook of infant development: Brain, behavior, and cultural context (pp. 715741). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108351959 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baranzini, D. (2018). SPSS single dataframe aggregating SPSS multiply imputed split files.Google Scholar
Bazhenova, O. V., Plonskaia, O., & Porges, S. W. (2001). Vagal reactivity and affective adjustment in infants during interaction challenges. Child Development, 72(5), 13141326. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00350 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and gray’s motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13(2), 183214. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401002012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P. (2015). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44(5), 875896. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1038827 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., Hahn, H., & Crowell, S. E. (2020). Future directions in research and treatment of emotion dysregulation. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Crowell, S. E. (Ed.), The oxford handbook of emotion dysregulation (pp. 477486). Oxford University Press, https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190689285.001.0001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43(4), 11711190. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127506 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Fearon, R. M. (2002). Early attachment security, subsequent maternal sensitivity, and later child development: Does continuity in development depend upon continuity of caregiving? Attachment & Human Development, 4(3), 361387. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730210167267 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regulation and mental health: Recent findings, current challenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25(2), 128134. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503669 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326339. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Zentall, S., Lickenbrock, D. M., Ekas, N. V., Oshio, T., & Planalp, E. (2014). Attachment in the making: Mother and father sensitivity and infants’ responses during the still-face paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 125, 6384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Burt, N. M., Edwards, E. S., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of self-regulation: A multidisciplinary review and integrative conceptual framework. Psychological Bulletin, 141(3), 602654. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038662 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Gartstein, M. A., Putnam, S. P., Lance, K. O., Iddins, E., Waits, R., & Lee, L. (2011). Emerging effortful control in toddlerhood: The role of infant orienting/regulation, maternal effortful control, and maternal time spent in caregiving activities. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(1), 189199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.12.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Gartstein, M. A., Putnam, S. P., McKay, T., Iddins, E., Robertson, C., Ramsay, K., & Rittmueller, A. (2009). Maternal and contextual influences and the effect of temperament development during infancy on parenting in toddlerhood. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 103116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooker, R. J., & Buss, K. A. (2010). Dynamic measures of RSA predict distress and regulation in toddlers. Developmental Psychobiology, 52(4), 372382. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20432 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1986). The EAS approach to temperament. In Dunn, J., & Plomin, R. (Ed.), The study of temperament: Changes, continuities and challenges (pp. 6777). L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Uchino, B. N., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Individual differences in the autonomic origins of heart rate reactivity: The psychometrics of respiratory sinus arrhythmia and preejection period. Psychophysiology, 31(4), 412419. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02449.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D. (1997). Cardiac vagal tone indices of temperamental reactivity and behavioral regulation in young children. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 31, 125135. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199709)31: 3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D., Dedmon, S. E., Gill, K. L., Lomax, L. E., & Johnson, L. M. (2002). Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. Infancy, 3(2), 175197. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0302_4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, S. D., & Degnan, K. A. (2006). Temperament in early development. In Ammerman, R. T. (Ed.), Comprehensive handbook of personality and psychopathology. (vol. 3, pp. 6484). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., & Hill, A. (2007). Caregiver influences on emerging emotion regulation. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. (vol. 229248, pp. 229248). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7(3), 350369. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00072 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, A. S., Briggs-Gowan, M., Jones, S. M., & Little, T. D. (2003). The infant-toddler social and emotional assessment (ITSEA): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(5), 495514. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025449031360 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Hall, S. E., Hajal, N. J., Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2017). Emotion dysregulation as a vulnerability to psychopathology. In Hinshaw, S. P., & Beauchaine, T. P. (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (pp. 346386). Wiley.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75(2), 317333. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Teti, L. O. (1994). The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3), 73102. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166139 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2003). Infant and maternal behavior moderate reactivity to novelty to predict anxious behavior at 2.5 years. Development and Psychopathology, 15(02), 297310.Google Scholar
Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2004). Infant and maternal behaviors regulate infant reactivity to novelty at 6 months. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 11231132. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1123 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2006). Infant and maternal behavior moderate reactivity to novelty to predict anxious behavior at 2.5 years. Development and Psychopathology, 18(1), 1734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., & Linehan, M. M. (2009). A biosocial developmental model of borderline personality: Elaborating and extending Linehan’s theory. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 495510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, S. E., Puzia, M. E., & Yaptangco, M. (2015). The ontogeny of chronic distress: Emotion dysregulation across the life span and its implications for psychological and physical health. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 9199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.023 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Vlisides-Henry, R. D., Kaliush, P. R., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2020). Emotion generation, regulation, and dysregulation as multilevel transdiagnostic constructs. In Beauchaine, T. P., & Crowell, S. E. (Ed.), The oxford handbook of emotion dysregulation (pp. 8597). Oxford University Press, https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190689285.001.0001 Google Scholar
Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Development, 77(1), 4458. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00855.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Glynn, L. M., Waffarn, F., & Sandman, C. A. (2011). Prenatal maternal stress programs infant stress regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(2), 119129. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02314.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., Zoller, D., & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers’ emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology, 30(6), 928936. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.928 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Early, D. M., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Cox, M. J., Saluja, G., Pianta, R. C., Bradley, R. H., & Payne, C. C. (2002). Maternal sensitivity and child wariness in the transition to kindergarten. Parenting: Science and Practice, 2(4), 355377. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327922PAR0204_02 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6(1), 495525. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76(5), 10551071. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00897.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enlow, M. B., King, L., Schreier, H. M., Howard, J. M., Rosenfield, D., Ritz, T., & Wright, R. J. (2014). Maternal sensitivity and infant autonomic and endocrine stress responses. Early Human Development, 90(7), 377385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.04.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). The development of self-control of emotion: Intrinsic and extrinsic influences. Motivation and Emotion, 27(1), 726. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023622324898 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaertner, B. M., Spinrad, T. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2008). Focused attention in toddlers: Measurement, stability, and relations to negative emotion and parenting. Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 17(4), 339363, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1002/icd.580 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gartstein, M. A., Bridgett, D. J., Young, B. N., Panksepp, J., & Power, T. (2013). Origins of effortful control: Infant and parent contributions. Infancy, 18(2), 149183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26(1), 6486. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(02)00169-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giusti, L., Provenzi, L., & Montirosso, R. (2018). The face-to-face still-face (FFSF) paradigm in clinical settings: Socio-emotional regulation assessment and parental support with infants with neurodevelopmental disabilities. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 789. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00789 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, P., & Derefinko, K. (2013). Cardiac vagal control and children’s adaptive functioning: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 94(1), 2237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.04.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, P. A., Reavis, R. D., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2007). The role of emotion regulation in children’s early academic success. Journal of School Psychology, 45(1), 319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.09.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H., Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2005). The uniqueness of the child-father attachment relationship: Fathers’ sensitive and challenging play as a pivotal variable in a 16-year longitudinal study. Social Development, 14(3), 361384. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00202 Google Scholar
Guedeney, A., Matthey, S., & Puura, K. (2013). Social withdrawal behavior in infancy: A history of the concept and a review of published studies using the alarm distress baby scale. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34(6), 516531, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1002/imhj.21412 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (2002). Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27(1-2), 199220. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00045-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halberstadt, A. G., Cassidy, J., Stifter, C. A., Parke, R. D., & Fox, N. A. (2001). Self-expressiveness within the family context: Psychometric support for a new measure. Psychological Assessment, 13(4), 325341. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.1.93 Google Scholar
Haley, D. W., & Stansbury, K. (2003). Infant stress and parent responsiveness: Regulation of physiology and behavior during still-face and reunion. Child Development, 74(5), 15341546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemmi, M. H., Wolke, D., & Schneider, S. (2011). Associations between problems with crying, sleeping and/or feeding in infancy and long-term behavioural outcomes in childhood: A meta-analysis. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 96(7), 622629. https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.2010.191312 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, A. L., Degnan, K. A., Calkins, S. D., & Keane, S. P. (2006). Profiles of externalizing behavior problems for boys and girls across preschool: The roles of emotion regulation and inattention. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 913928. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.913 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinnant, J. B., & El-Sheikh, M. (2009). Children’s externalizing and internalizing symptoms over time: The role of individual differences in patterns of RSA responding. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(8), 10491061. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9341-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huffman, L. C., Bryan, Y. E., del Carmen, R., Pedersen, F. A., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Forges, S. W. (1998). Infant temperament and cardiac vagal tone: Assessments at twelve weeks of age. Child Development, 69(3), 624635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, K. (2000). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7(4), 418434. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.7.4.418 Google Scholar
Kiel, E. J., & Kalomiris, A. E. (2015). Current themes in understanding children’s emotion regulation as developing from within the parent-child relationship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 1116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent-child mutuality on self-regulation: A relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Development, 83(4), 12751289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 199, https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.18.2.199 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 343354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.343 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: Establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 627642. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.627 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Development, 80(3), 762775. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, B., Kaliush, P. R., Conradt, E., Terrell, S., Neff, D., Allen, A. K., Smid, M. C., Monk, C., & Crowell, S. E. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of emotion dysregulation: Part I. Psychopathology, self-injury, and parasympathetic responsivity among pregnant women. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000336 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonigan, C. J., Vasey, M. W., Phillips, B. M., & Hazen, R. A. (2004). Temperament, anxiety, and the processing of threat-relevant stimuli. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 820. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangelsdorf, S. C., Shapiro, J. R., & Marzolf, D. (1995). Developmental and temperamental differences in emotion regulation in infancy. Child Development, 66(6), 18171828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, C. A., & Wells, R. S. (2015). Reporting the use of multiple imputation for missing data in higher education research. Research in Higher Education, 56(4), 397409. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11162-014-9344-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinez-Torteya, C., Dayton, C. J., Beeghly, M., Seng, J. S., McGinnis, E., Broderick, A., Rosenblum, K., & Muzik, M. (2014). Maternal parenting predicts infant biobehavioral regulation among women with a history of childhood maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 26(2), 379392. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCoy, D. C., & Raver, C. C. (2011). Caregiver emotional expressiveness, child emotion regulation, and child behavior problems among head start families. Social Development, 20(4), 741761. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00608.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Mennin, D. S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Emotion dysregulation and adolescent psychopathology: A prospective study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(9), 544554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.06.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesman, J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of the still-face paradigm: A review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29(2), 120162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.02.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mittal, V. A., & Wakschlag, L. S. (2017). Research domain criteria (RDoC) grows up: Strengthening neurodevelopmental investigation within the RDoC framework. Journal of Affective Disorders, 216, 3035. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, G. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2004). Infants’ vagal regulation in the still-face paradigm is related to dyadic coordination of mother-infant interaction. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 10681080. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1068 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, G. A., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., Propper, C. B., Calkins, S. D., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Cox, M. J. (2009). Mother-infant vagal regulation in the face-to-face still-face paradigm is moderated by maternal sensitivity. Child Development, 80(1), 209223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1998). Early child care and self-control, compliance, and problem behavior at twenty-four and thirty-six months. Child Development Research, 1145(4), 1145. https://doi.org/10.2307/1132367 Google Scholar
O’Connor, M. J. (2001). Prenatal alcohol exposure and infant negative affect as precursors of depressive features in children. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of The World Association for Infant Mental Health, 22(3), 291299, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1002/imhj.1002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obrist, P. A. (1976). The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction – as it appears today. Psychophysiology, 13(2), 95107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okorn, A., Verhoeven, M., & Van Baar, A. (2022). The importance of mothers’ and fathers’ positive parenting for toddlers’ and preschoolers’ social-emotional adjustment. Parenting, 22(2), 128151. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2021.1908090 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, T. K., Lenartowicz, A., Berkman, E. T., Ji, D., Poldrack, R. A., & Knowlton, B. J. (2016). Putting the brakes on the brakes: Negative emotion disrupts cognitive control network functioning and alters subsequent stopping ability. Experimental Brain Research, 234(11), 31073118. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4709-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peltola, M. J., Forssman, L., Puura, K., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Leppänen, J. M. (2015). Attention to faces expressing negative emotion at 7 months predicts attachment security at 14 months. Child Development, 86(5), 13211332. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12380 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penela, E. C., Henderson, H. A., Hane, A. A., Ghera, M. M., & Fox, N. A. (2012). Maternal caregiving moderates the relation between temperamental fear and social behavior with peers. Infancy, 17(6), 715730. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00114.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, N. B., & Calkins, S. D. (2018). A biopsychosocial perspective on the development of emotion regulation across childhood. In Cole, P. M., & Hollenstein, T. (Ed.), Emotion regulation: A matter of time (pp. 330). Taylor & Francis, https://doi.org/10.4324/9781351001328 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, N. B., Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (2016). Indirect effects of maternal sensitivity on infant emotion regulation behaviors: The role of vagal withdrawal. Infancy, 21(2), 128153. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrenko, A., Kanya, M. J., Rosinski, L., McKay, E. R., & Bridgett, D. J. (2019). Effects of infant negative affect and contextual factors on infant regulatory capacity: The moderating role of infant sex. Infant and Child Development, 28(6), e2157, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1002/icd.2157 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2011). Prenatal programming of postnatal plasticity? Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 2938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W., Davila, M. I., Lewis, G. F., Kolacz, J., Okonmah‐Obazee, S., Hane, A. A., Kwon, K. Y., Ludwig, R. J., Myers, M. M., & Welch, M. G. (2019). Autonomic regulation of preterm infants is enhanced by family nurture intervention. Developmental Psychobiology, 61(6), 942952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Maiti, A. K. (1994). Vagal tone and the physiological regulation of emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 2(3), 167186. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166144 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., Portales, A. L., & Greenspan, S. I. (1996). Infant regulation of the vagal “brake” predicts child behavior problems: A psychobiological model of social behavior. Developmental Psychobiology, 29(8), 697712. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199612)29: 3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W., & Furman, S. A. (2011). The early development of the autonomic nervous system provides a neural platform for social behaviour: A polyvagal perspective. Infant and Child Development, 20(1), 106118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, C. L., Wouden-Miller, M., Silva, S. S., & Porter, A. E. (2003). Marital harmony and conflict: Links to infants’ emotional regulation and cardiac vagal tone. Infancy, 4(2), 297307, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1207/S15327078IN0402_09 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interaction effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437448, http://www.quantpsy.org CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, S. P., Helbig, A. L., Gartstein, M. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Leerkes, E. (2014). Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire-revised. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(4), 445458. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2013.841171 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development of short and very short forms of the children’s behavior questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87(1), 102112. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qu, M., Zhang, Y., Webster, J. G., & Tompkins, W. J. (1986). Motion artifact from spot and band electrodes during impedance cardiography. IEEE Transactions On Biomedical Engineering, BME-33(11), 10291036. https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.1986.325869 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rash, J. A., Thomas, J. C., Campbell, T. S., Letourneau, N., Granger, D. A., & Giesbrecht, G. F. (2016). Developmental origins of infant stress reactivity profiles: A multi-system approach. Developmental Psychobiology, 58(5), 578599. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21403 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, K. B., Schröder, T., & von Scheve, C. (2014). Dissecting the sociality of emotion: A multilevel approach. Emotion Review, 6(2), 124133. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913503383 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roisman, G. I., Newman, D. A., Fraley, R. C., Haltigan, J. D., Groh, A. M., & Haydon, K. C. (2012). Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis-stress: Recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Development and Psychopathology, 24(2), 389402. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000065 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40(1), 2139. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23087906 Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In Lamb, M. E., & Brown, A. L. (Ed.), Advances in developmental psychology (pp. 3786). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (2002). Temperament in children. In Backman, L., & von Hofsten, C. (Ed.), Psychology at the turn of the millennium, volume 2 (1st ed. pp. 3352). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Santucci, A. K., Silk, J. S., Shaw, D. S., Gentzler, A., Fox, N. A., & Kovacs, M. (2008). Vagal tone and temperament as predictors of emotion regulation strategies in young children. Developmental Psychobiology, 50(3), 205216. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20283 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7(2), 147177. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schore, A. N. (2001). The effects of early relational trauma on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of The World Association for Infant Mental Health, 22(1-2), 201269, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1002/1097-0355(200101/04)22: 3.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spangler, G. (1990). Mother, child, and situational correlates of toddlers’ social competence. Infant Behavior and Development, 13(4), 405419. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(90)90013-X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spangler, G., Schieche, M., Ilg, U., Maier, U., & Ackermann, C. (1994). Maternal sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral regulation in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 27(7), 425437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B., Popp, T., Smith, C. L., Kupfer, A., Greving, K., Liew, J., & Hofer, C. (2007). Relations of maternal socialization and toddlers’ effortful control to children’s adjustment and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1170–86, 1170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinrad, T. L., Stifter, C. A., Donelan-McCall, N., & Turner, L. (2004). Mothers’ regulation strategies in response to toddlers’ affect: Links to later emotion self-regulation. Social Development, 13(1), 4055, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00256.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stifter, C., & Dollar, J. (2016). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention (pp. 546607). John Wiley & Sons, Inc, https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy411 Google Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Corey, J. M. (2001). Vagal regulation and observed social behavior in infancy. Social Development, 10(2), 189201, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1111/1467-9507.00158 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2002). The effect of excessive crying on the development of emotion regulation. Infancy, 3(2), 133152. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0302_2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauman, T. J. (2017). Self-regulation and psychopathology: Toward an integrative translational research paradigm. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13(1), 497523. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suurland, J., van der Heijden, K. B., Huijbregts, S. C. J., van Goozen, S. H. M., & Swaab, H. (2018). Infant parasympathetic and sympathetic activity during baseline, stress and recovery: Interactions with prenatal adversity predict physical aggression in toddlerhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(4), 755768. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0337-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, J. C., Letourneau, N., Campbell, T. S., Tomfohr-Madsen, L., & Giesbrecht, G. F. (2017). Developmental origins of infant emotion regulation: Mediation by temperamental negativity and moderation by maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 611628. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000279 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A. (2011a). Emotion and emotion regulation: Two sides of the developing coin. Emotion Review, 3(1), 5361. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910380969 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2014). Socialization of emotion and emotion regulation in the family. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed. pp. 173186). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138(09)62273-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tronick, E. Z., & Cohn, J. F. (1989). Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: Age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Development, 60(1), 85. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131074 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Buuren, S. (2018). Flexible imputation of missing data. CRC press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Kamp, M. R., Klaver, E. C., Thio, B. J., Driessen, J. M., de Jongh, F. H., Tabak, M., & Hermens, H. J. (2020). WEARCON: Wearable home monitoring in children with asthma reveals a strong association with hospital based assessment of asthma control. BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making, 20(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Wagner, N. J., & Waller, R. (2020). Leveraging parasympathetic nervous system activity to study risk for psychopathology: The special case of callous-unemotional traits. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 175185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakschlag, L. S., Tolan, P. H., & Leventhal, B. L. (2010). Ain’t misbehavin’: Towards a developmentally-specified nosology for preschool disruptive behavior. Research Review, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(1), 322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02184.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, H. J., Hawes, T., Campbell, S. M., & Clear, S. J. (2020). Assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation. In The encyclopedia of child and adolescent development (pp. 113). Wiley Online Library, https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119171492.wecad153 Google Scholar
Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1994). Beyond the face: An empirical study of infant affective configurations of facial, vocal, gestural, and regulatory behaviors. Child Development, 65(5), 15031515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1996). Infant affective reactions to the resumption of maternal interaction after the still-face. Child Development, 67(3), 905914, https://doi-org.ezproxy.lib.utah.edu/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01772.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1999). Infant and caregiver engagement phases (ICEP). Harvard Medical School.Google Scholar
Welch, M. G., & Ludwig, R. J. (2017). Calming cycle theory and the co-regulation of oxytocin. Psychodynamic Psychiatry, 45(4), 519540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winsper, C., & Wolke, D. (2014). Infant and toddler crying, sleeping and feeding problems and trajectories of dysregulated behavior across childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(5), 831843. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9813-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Q., & Feng, X. (2020). Infant emotion regulation and cortisol response during the first 2 years of life: Association with maternal parenting profiles. Developmental Psychobiology, 62(8), 10761091. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21965 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Correlations

Figure 1

Table 2. Measures’ descriptive statistics

Figure 2

Figure 1. Baseline RSA and maternal sensitivity interaction plot. Independent variables were centered for graphical representation. Results from Model 1 show significance only for infants who have experienced low maternal sensitivity; ***p < .001 (two-tailed).

Figure 3

Table 3. Regression results

Figure 4

Figure 2. RSA withdrawal and maternal sensitivity interaction plot. Independent variables were centered for graphical representation. RSA withdrawal = still-face task mean minus baseline play mean. Lower scores represent greater withdrawal, or more decrease from baseline to task. Higher scores represent less withdrawal from baseline to task. Because the mean value of RSA change was negative (see Table 3), positive values on the mean-centered x-axis reflect actual RSA withdrawal values that are close to zero (reflecting a lack of change in RSA levels from play to still-face episodes) and not positive RSA changes (which would indicate that RSA levels increased from baseline to still-face). Results from Model 2 show significance only for infants who have experienced low maternal sensitivity; *p < .05, **p < .01 (two-tailed). RSA withdrawal = still-face task mean minus baseline play mean. Lower scores represent greater withdrawal, or more decrease from baseline to task. Higher scores represent less withdrawal or increase from baseline to task.