Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T18:37:50.078Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social anxiety moderates the association between adolescent irritability and bully perpetration

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 March 2024

Michael T. Perino*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Jennifer C. Harper-Lednicky
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Alecia C. Vogel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Chad M. Sylvester
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Deanna M. Barch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA Department of Psychology and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
Joan L. Luby
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
*
Corresponding author: M. T. Perino; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Preliminary work suggests anxiety moderates the relationship between irritability and bullying. As anxiety increases, the link between irritability and perpetration decreases. We hypothesize that any moderation effect of anxiety is driven by social anxiety symptoms. We sought to explicate the moderating effect of anxiety, while clarifying relations to other aggressive behaviors.

Methods:

A sample of adolescents (n = 169, mean = 12.42 years of age) were assessed using clinician rated assessments of anxiety, parent reports of irritability and bullying behaviors (perpetration, generalized aggression, and victimization). Correlations assessed zero-order relations between variables, and regression-based moderation analyses were used to test interactions. Johnson–Neyman methods were used to represent significant interactions.

Results:

Irritability was significantly related to bullying (r = .403, p < .001). Social, but not generalized, anxiety symptoms significantly moderated the effect of irritability on bully perpetration (t(160) = −2.94, b = −.01, p = .0038, ΔR2 = .0229, F(1, 160) = 8.635). As social anxiety symptoms increase, the link between irritability and perpetration decreases.

Conclusions:

Understanding how psychopathology interacts with social behaviors is of great importance. Higher social anxiety is linked to reduced relations between irritability and bullying; however, the link between irritability and other aggression remains positive. Comprehensively assessing how treatment of psychopathology impacts social behaviors may improve future intervention.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Bullying in youth is a major societal and public health problem linked to significant psychological (Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2013), economic (Wolke et al., Reference Wolke, Copeland, Angold and Costello2013), and societal harms (Jantzer et al., Reference Jantzer, Schlander, Haffner, Parzer, Trick, Resch and Kaess2019). Understood as aggressive social behaviors that involve the targeting of peers, often repetitively, to create or maintain an imbalance of power (Volk et al., Reference Volk, Dane and Marini2014), bullying remains common (Hong & Espelage, Reference Hong and Espelage2012) and one of the most damaging youth behaviors (Gladden et al., Reference Gladden, Vivolo-Kantor, Hamburger and Lumpkin2014). Victimization in youth has been shown to lead to commonly occurring psychopathology, such as anxiety (Siegel et al., Reference Siegel, La Greca and Harrison2009), depression (Klomek et al., Reference Klomek, Marrocco, Kleinman, Schonfeld and Gould2008), and suicidality (Takizawa et al., Reference Takizawa, Maughan and Arseneault2014), making the need to understand how to reduce perpetration an important goal. However, how to treat bullying and other destructive peer behaviors in youth, in conjunction with other types of commonly occurring psychopathology, has not been well explicated. Peer relationship behaviors in youth (e.g., bully perpetration, victimization, generalized aggression) have shared relationships to many symptoms of psychopathology (Parker et al., Reference Parker, Rubin, Erath, Wojslawowicz and Buskirk2015; Prinstein & Giletta, Reference Prinstein and Giletta2016); therefore, evaluating how common childhood symptoms interact with aggressive peer behaviors may provide valuable insights in guiding treatment and in designing interventions.

Anxiety is the most common diagnosis in pediatric samples (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005), is marked by deviations in many executive functions (Shanmugan et al., Reference Shanmugan, Wolf, Calkins, Moore, Ruparel, Hopson, Vandekar, Roalf, Elliott, Jackson, Gennatas, Leibenluft, Pine, Shinohara, Hakonarson, Gur, Gur and Satterthwaite2016), and presents symptomatically as excessive worry (Weisberg, Reference Weisberg2009). Irritability, defined by excessive anger and temper outbursts (Leibenluft, Reference Leibenluft2017), is a common transdiagnostic symptom in childhood and adolescence (Copeland et al., Reference Copeland, Brotman and Costello2015). Irritability has been consistently linked with many prevalent childhood emotional problems, including internalizing syndromes, like pediatric anxiety (Stoddard et al., Reference Stoddard, Stringaris, Brotman, Montville, Pine and Leibenluft2014), and externalizing problems, including conduct and oppositional defiant disorder (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Burke, Roberts, Fite, Lochman, Francisco and Reed2017; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Schouboe, Kircanski, Leibenluft, Stringaris and Gotlib2019), though internalizing and externalizing syndromes are often not strongly associated with one another (Juvonen et al., Reference Juvonen, Graham and Schuster2003). While anxiety is often not significantly related to perpetration (Perino et al., Reference Perino, Guassi Moreira and Telzer2019), irritability is often related to perpetration and other aggressive behaviors (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Schouboe, Kircanski, Leibenluft, Stringaris and Gotlib2019). Intriguingly, recent work examining the relationship between anxiety, irritability, and bullying in early adolescence found that anxiety, while not correlated with perpetration, significantly moderated the relationship between adolescent irritability and perpetration. The link between irritability and bully perpetration decreased as anxiety increased (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021), suggesting that decreases in anxiety, without co-occurring reductions in irritability, could theoretically lead to increases in adolescent perpetration behaviors.

Uncovering why anxiety could impact links between irritability and aggressive behaviors could influence treatment decisions for adolescents presenting with certain symptoms. One potential explanation previously put forth is that bully perpetration behaviors are reflective of an antisocial interpersonal strategy (Juvonen & Ho, Reference Juvonen and Ho2008), where youth look for opportunities to gain resources at the expense of other peers (Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003). Perpetration is linked to being viewed as a leader by one’s peers (Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003), increased social status (Hawley et al., Reference Hawley, Little and Card2007; Rose et al., Reference Rose, Swenson and Waller2004), intact socio-emotional intelligence (Garandeau & Lansu, Reference Garandeau and Lansu2019; Kaukiainen et al., Reference Kaukiainen, Björkqvist, Lagerspetz, Österman, Salmivalli, Rothberg and Ahlbom1999), and lower emotional distress (Juvonen et al., Reference Juvonen, Graham and Schuster2003). Developmentally, adolescence is a period of time where youth increase approach-oriented behaviors (McCormick & Telzer, Reference McCormick and Telzer2017) and preferentially respond to rewards (Galván, Reference Galván2013), particularly social ones (Perino et al., Reference Perino, Miernicki and Telzer2016; Somerville et al., Reference Somerville, Jones and Casey2010). If bully perpetration reflects an “approach” oriented behavioral strategy (Kokkinos et al., Reference Kokkinos, Voulgaridou and Markos2016) used to gain certain types of social resources or advantages (Perino et al., Reference Perino, Guassi Moreira and Telzer2019; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003; Volk et al., Reference Volk, Camilleri, Dane and Marini2012), then increasing levels of anxiety, implicated in behavioral inhibition (Vervoort et al., Reference Vervoort, Wolters, Hogendoorn, De Haan, Boer and Prins2010), may reduce perpetration when all other symptoms are held constant. In short, bully perpetration behaviors may be thought of as approach behaviors used to meet social needs (Hawley, Reference Hawley2002, Reference Hawley2003a; Hawley, Reference Hawley2003b; Hawley, Reference Hawley2015); therefore, symptoms which impact approach motivation systems may paradoxically decrease the relationship between co-occurring psychopathology and perpetration.

While the findings described above are suggestive, there are open questions about the relations among anxiety, irritability, and adolescent aggression. Given the emphasis on bullying as a social strategy in the literature, it is possible that any moderating effect of anxiety may be specific to social anxiety symptoms, rather than generalized anxiety (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021). Replicating the original moderation and clarifying whether social anxiety is the specific driver is of great import. Additionally, bully perpetration is just one form of aggressive behaviors seen in peer relationships, so clarifying whether anxiety moderates the relationship between only irritability and bullying, or impacts other aggressive peer behaviors, such as generalized aggression, is needed. Significant factor analytic work (Espelage & Holt, Reference Espelage and Holt2001) has shown that bullying is distinct from other, more generalized forms of aggression, such as fighting and disagreeableness (Espelage et al., Reference Espelage, Low, Rao, Hong and Little2014, Reference Espelage, Van Ryzin and Holt2018; Hawley et al., Reference Hawley, Stump and Ratliff2010). However, the bullying measure (Jarcho et al., Reference Jarcho, Grossman, Guyer, Quarmley, Smith, Fox, Leibenluft, Pine and Nelson2019) previously used included items related to fighting, which may be more related to generalized aggression (Espelage & Holt, Reference Espelage and Holt2001; Hawley et al., Reference Hawley, Stump and Ratliff2010; Juvonen et al., Reference Juvonen, Graham and Schuster2003; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003). Understanding how anxiety impacts associations of irritability with different forms of aggression comprehensively will better elucidate expected outcomes when symptoms change.

In this manuscript, we sought to replicate the finding that anxiety moderates the link between irritability and bully perpetration, while clarifying whether the effect is specific to social anxiety or generalized anxiety. We explored relations between irritability, anxiety symptoms (social, general), and aggressive peer relationship behaviors (bully perpetration, generalized aggression), as well as interaction effects of anxiety on the relationship between irritability and aggression. We hypothesized that anxiety would moderate the relationship between irritability and perpetration, but that the effect was specific to social anxiety, rather than generalized anxiety. Additionally, we attempted to determine whether the moderation effect differed for different types of aggressive childhood behaviors (bully perpetration, generalized aggression). The results of these analyses aim to improve our understanding of how to better intervene, and what to expect when interventions are applied in these commonly co-occurring behavioral problems in youth.

Methods

Participants

The sample used in this study was drawn from an ongoing longitudinal study, the Preschool Depression Study, (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Belden, Pautsch, Si and Spitznagel2009) conducted at Washington University School of Medicine. Preschool-aged participants (between ages 3 and 6 years) were recruited from primary care facilities and preschools/daycares in the surrounding metropolitan area from pamphlets about “assessing emotional development.” Parents who responded to promotional material were screened by trained research assistants via telephone interview, to recruit child participants (i) with internalizing psychopathology (endorsement of ≥ 2 symptoms of depression), as well as participants (ii) without psychopathology and (iii) with externalizing psychopathology (endorsement of ≥ 2 symptoms of externalizing psychopathology [ADHD, ODD, or CD]). Participants were excluded if there was evidence of (i) chronic medical illnesses, (ii) neurological problems, (iii) pervasive developmental disorders, or (iv) language/cognitive delays that would impact the ability to answer questionnaires. (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Heffelfinger, Mrakotsky, Brown, Hessler, Wallis and Spitznagel2003) Consent and assent was collected from all participants who completed assessments and all protocols were approved by the Washington University Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Assessment of clinical symptoms of anxiety

Upon screening and consent, participants and parents were assessed via semi-structured interviews by Master’s level raters for psychopathology using the Kiddie-Schedule of Affective Disorders – Present and Lifetime Version (KSADS-PL) (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Ryan and Rao2000). The KSADS-PL is a semi-structured clinician-rated assessment derived from interviews with both parents and children to assess psychopathology in youth, which has been shown to be reliable and valid for childhood psychiatric diagnoses (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci, Williamson and Ryan1997). Master’s level raters were trained to reliability by an experienced clinician (J.L) and to ensure reliability, all interviews were audiotaped and calibration was provided on 20% of each raters’ cases. (Luby & Belden, Reference Luby and Belden2008) Participants completed a baseline assessment at age 3-6, and were subsequently invited back to continue completing assessments of cognitive and social skills and psychopathology every 1–2 years (Gaffrey et al., Reference Gaffrey, Barch, Singer, Shenoy and Luby2013).

To match the protocol of Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021, we focused our analyses on study wave 12, which focused on early adolescence (mean age 12). Social anxiety and generalized anxiety scores were tabulated by summing symptoms of each from the K-SADS module. The Social anxiety score includes six items (e.g., “is your child shy, fearful in social situations, or uncomfortable with people they don’t know well”) which were marked as either present or not present, while the generalized anxiety score includes nine items (e.g., “does your child worry, have somatic complaints, or have over concern about competence”) which were also marked as present or not present. Positive endorsements were added to create sum scores for social anxiety and generalized anxiety.

Irritability

Consistent with the assessment of anxiety, we utilized measures from study wave 12 to assess for irritability. Specifically, we used an irritability measure previously validated within the sample (Vogel et al., Reference Vogel, Jackson, Barch, Tillman and Luby2019) that took a factor analytic approach using items from the clinician-administered Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA) (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Angold, Small and Copeland1999) (and later the Childhood and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment:CAPA) (Angold et al., Reference Angold, Prendergast, Cox, Harrington, Simonoff and Rutter1995) to differentiate irritability from other forms of emotion dysregulation. Irritability items were from the depression, mania, and conduct modules of the PAPA and CAPA, and included items pertaining to irritability intensity, frequency, spontaneity, irritability concern to caretakers, tearful and crying, angry or resentful intensity and temper tantrum intensity.

Bullying behaviors

Bully role behaviors were assessed using a fourteen item composite measure derived from the Parent Report of the Health and Behavior Questionnaire (Essex et al., Reference Essex, Boyce, Goldstein, Armstrong, Kraemer, Kupfer and Group2002). Specifically, we conceptualized bullying behaviors as done in the Illinois Bully Scale (Espelage & Holt, Reference Espelage and Holt2001) and separately assessed for bully perpetration, generalized aggression, and victimization. The perpetration factor (α = .73) consisted of five items (e.g., taunts and teases peers; is cruel, bullies, is mean to others) and each item was rated on a 0–2 scale; the generalized aggression factor (α = .68) consisted of 5 items (e.g., temper tantrums; kicks, bites, or hits other children, gets in many fights) and each item was rated on a 0–2 scale; and the victimization factor (α = .83) consisted of four items (e.g., is actively picked on; is teased and ridiculed) and each item was rated on a 1–4 scale.

Analytic approach

To characterize the sample, we explored relationships between our demographic, clinical (social anxiety, generalized anxiety, irritability), and bully role (bully perpetration, generalized aggression, victimization) continuous variables. We focused exclusively on the behavioral and clinical assessments at the timepoint that most closely matched the sample from Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021, which was (Timepoint 12 [T12]). A total of 169 participants at T12 were assessed for psychopathology and social behaviors in the current analysis. To reduce biases introduced by including missing data in analyses (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Davis-Kean, Halvorson, King, Logan, Xu, Bainter, Brown, Clay, Cruz, Elsherif, Gerasimova, Joyal-Desmarais, Moreau, Nissen, Schmidt, Uzdavines, Van Dusen and Vasilev2021), we imputed missing data using linear regression (5 iterations) using all the variables included in our analyses (age, sex, race, bullying behaviors, anxiety scores). Descriptive statistics for original and imputed variables are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of demographic, clinical and behavioral variables

First, we ran zero-order correlations between our variables, to assess primary relationships between psychopathology and bully role behaviors. As an additional step, we ran independent t-tests to determine if sex was significantly related to the aforementioned continuous variables. Next, we used regression-based moderation analyses to explore whether anxiety symptoms were significant moderators of the relationship between irritability and bully perpetration and the relationship between irritability and victimization, while controlling for age, sex, and race. We also aimed to expand upon the interactions explored in Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021 by exploring whether there was a differential interaction when looking at the relationship between irritability and generalized aggression. To graphically explore interactions, we ran Johnson–Neyman tests (Johnson & Neyman, Reference Johnson and Neyman1936) to determine data ranges where anxiety significantly moderates the relationships between irritability and aggressive behaviors. Finally, given that bully role behaviors (perpetration, generalized aggression, victimization) are correlated and do not appear in isolation, we ran additional interaction models to control for the shared variance between bullying behaviors and more clearly extrapolate relationship between irritability and specific types of aggressive behaviors. We again ran Johnson–Neyman tests (Johnson & Neyman, Reference Johnson and Neyman1936) to determine data ranges where anxiety significantly moderates the relationships between irritability and aggressive behaviors.

All descriptive statistics, correlations, t-tests, and imputations were run using SPSS (Version 28.0; IBM SPSS, Armonk, NY). All interactions were completed using the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes, Reference Hayes2016). Johnson–Neyman statistics and graphics were run using the Interactions R Toolkit (Long, Reference Long2019).

Results

Relationships between demographic, clinical, and bully role behaviors

Bully role behaviors were significantly correlated to clinical variables, consistent with past research. Perpetration was positively related with irritability (r = .403, p < .001) but negatively correlated with social anxiety (r = −.185, p = .016) and unrelated to generalized anxiety (r = −.021, p = .791). Generalized aggression was positively correlated with irritability (r = .535, p < .001) but not significantly correlated with social (r = .013, p = .869) or generalized (r = .145, p = .059) anxiety. Irritability was significantly correlated with generalized anxiety (r = .366, p < .001) but not social anxiety (r = .046, p = 552, see Table 2 for full results and supplemental Figure 1A–C to see distribution of bully role behaviors). When running independent samples t-tests, generalized aggression (t(167) = 2.223, p = .028) and irritability (t(167) = 2.190, p = .030) were the only variables with significant relations to sex; for both generalized aggression (male mean = 0.23, SD = 0.26; female mean = 0.15, SD = 0.20), and irritability (male mean = 39.88, SD = 8.26, female mean = 37.35, SD = 6.56) males scored higher than females.

Table 2. Correlation matrix for demographic, clinical and behavioral variables

1. Irritability, 2. Number of Social Phobia Symptoms, 3. Number of Generalized Anxiety Symptoms, 4. Bullying Perpetration, 5. Generalized Aggression, 6. Bullying Victimization, 7. Age. *p<.05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Moderation effects of anxiety on bully role behaviors

Our first two regression-based moderation analyses explored whether social anxiety or generalized anxiety significantly moderated the relationship between irritability and bully perpetration. As we hypothesized, there was a significant interaction term between irritability and social anxiety (t(162) = −2.11, b = −.0096, p = .036, ΔR 2 = .0198, F(1, 162) = 4.465), such that as social anxiety increased, the association between irritability and bully perpetration decreased. Using the Johnson–Neyman method, we observed that there was a significant positive association between irritability and bully perpetration at low levels of social anxiety, but no association between irritability and bully perpetration at higher levels of social anxiety. Specifically, irritability was positively, significantly associated with levels of perpetration when there were less than 0.92 social anxiety symptoms (CI [.000, .0157], p = .050, see Fig. 1).

Figure 1. Social anxiety moderates the relationship between irritability and bully perpetration. The Johnson–Neyman technique was employed to explore the moderating effect of social anxiety on the relationships between irritability and bully perpetration, while controlling for demographic variables (age, sex, race). We observed that when social anxiety symptom endorsement was low (below 0.92 symptoms), the association between irritability and perpetration was significant and positive. As social anxiety increased, we saw a corresponding reduction to nonsignificance in the association between irritability and perpetration.

When we ran our regression-based moderation analysis using generalized anxiety symptoms instead of social anxiety symptoms, we observed that there was no significant interaction term (t(162) = .22, b = .0006, p = .822, ΔR 2 = .0006, F(1, 162) = 0.051). Additional moderation analyses exploring whether anxiety moderated the relationship between irritability and victimization were nonsignificant for both social anxiety (t(162) = .049, b = .0005, p = .961, ΔR 2 = 0, F(1, 162) = 0.0024) and generalized anxiety (t(162) = .581, b = .0035, p = .561, ΔR 2 = 0018, F(1, 162) = 0.338).

Next, we explored whether generalized aggression was similar to bully perpetration in that it was moderated by anxiety. However, we found that neither social anxiety (t(162) = .27, b = .0010, p = .789, ΔR 2 = .0003, F(1, 162) = 0.072) nor generalized anxiety (t(162) = .28, b = .0006, p = .777, ΔR 2 = .0003, F(1, 162) = 0.080) significantly moderated the relationship between irritability and generalized aggression.

Given that only social anxiety, and not generalized anxiety, significantly moderated the relationship between irritability and bully behaviors, we focused solely on social anxiety in our follow-up set of moderation analyses. When examining the link between irritability and bully perpetration, while additionally controlling for generalized aggression and victimization, the moderation effect of social anxiety was still significant (t(160) = −2.94, b = −.01, p = .0038, ΔR 2 = .0229, F(1, 160) = 8.635); specifically, irritability was now significantly negatively associated with perpetration when there was endorsement of more than 1.12 social anxiety symptoms (see Fig. 2a). Social anxiety still did not significantly moderate the link between irritability and generalized aggression (t(160) = 1.85, b = .0056, p = .065, ΔR 2 = .009, F(1, 160) = 3.429) when additionally controlling for bully perpetration and victimization. For illustrative purposes, we provide the Johnson–Neyman plot showing that the link between irritability and generalized aggression is significantly positive at all levels of social anxiety (see Fig. 2b).

Figure 2. Moderation effects of social anxiety between irritability and forms of aggressive behaviors. The Johnson–Neyman technique was employed to explore the moderating effect of social anxiety on the relationships between ( a ) irritability and bully perpetration, and ( b ) irritability and generalized aggression, while controlling for other bully role behaviors and demographic variables. For bully perpetration, we saw a nonsignificant negative effect when social anxiety endorsement was low, and a significant, negative effect when social anxiety endorsement was greater than or equal to 1.12 symptoms (CI [−.08, .0]). Increasing social anxiety corresponded with a negative relationship between irritability and bully perpetration. For generalized aggression, there was no statistical significance transition point, as the relationship between irritability and generalized aggression was the same regardless of the level of social anxiety.

Discussion

The current study found that as social anxiety symptoms increased, the link between irritability and bully perpetration became more negative. Without accounting for other bully role behaviors, we observed that as adolescents’ endorsement of social anxiety symptoms increased, the link between irritability and bully perpetration decreased and became nonsignificant. When accounting for other bully role behaviors (generalized aggression, victimization), we observed that as there was greater endorsement of social anxiety symptoms, the association between irritability and bully perpetration actually became significantly negative. The moderating of anxiety on the link between irritability and bully perpetration was seen with social anxiety – but not generalized anxiety – and these effects on the relationship between irritability to bully role behaviors was specific to bully perpetration, and not generalized aggression nor victimization. Bully perpetration was negatively related to social anxiety but positively related to irritability. Irritability was positively related to both generalized aggression and victimization; however, neither generalized aggression nor victimization was related to anxiety nor did anxiety moderate the link between irritability and these behaviors. Our results suggest that symptoms of psychopathology have complex associations with bully role behaviors, and that changes in one domain of psychopathology could impact the manifestation of a broad set of social behaviors.

We replicated some, but not all of the findings previously reported by Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021). We also found a significant relationship between irritability and victimization and found that anxiety did not moderate the link between irritability and victimization. We did not find that generalized anxiety significantly moderated the relationship between irritability and perpetration (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021); however, we did find that social anxiety symptoms moderated this relationship. Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Gardner, Clarkson, Eaton, Wiggins, Leibenluft and Jarcho2021) did not dissociate whether particular types of anxiety were driving moderation effects, so it is unclear if social anxiety was also responsible for the moderation they observed. The observation that social anxiety significantly negatively moderates the link between irritability, a transdiagnostic symptom (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Dougherty, Kessel, Silver and Carlson2021), and perpetration to the point where perpetration becomes negatively associated with irritability is quite interesting. It suggests that even though irritability predicts perpetration, the presence of social anxiety may blunt this expression.

Such a finding is consistent with the hypothesis that bully perpetration reflects a social strategy, and that symptoms which reduce social approach behaviors (e.g., social anxiety) may reduce expected links between psychopathology (such as irritability) and perpetration (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Connor and Scott2018). Bully perpetration has been shown to confer certain types of social advantages, such as increased numbers of romantic partners (Provenzano et al., Reference Provenzano, Dane, Farrell, Marini and Volk2018; Volk et al., Reference Volk, Dane, Marini and Vaillancourt2015), winning competitive endeavors (Dane et al., Reference Dane, Lapierre, Andrews and Volk2022), gaining social status (Spadafora et al., Reference Spadafora, Al-Jbouri, McDowell, Andrews and Volk2022), and deterring rivals (Cairns et al., Reference Cairns, Cairns, Neckerman, Gest and Gariepy1988). While irritability is related to aggression writ large (Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Schouboe, Kircanski, Leibenluft, Stringaris and Gotlib2019), bullying is unique in that it is targeted, goal-directed and inherently about social position (Volk et al., Reference Volk, Dane and Marini2014). We posit that social anxiety acts as an inhibitory force, and when irritability is kept constant, increasing social anxiety will lead to reduced perpetration. On the other hand, other aggressive behaviors likely stem from frustration-intolerance or impulse control difficulties and not social goals (Little et al., Reference Little, Henrich, Jones and Hawley2003). As expected, social anxiety did not moderate the association between irritability and other aggressive behaviors, which were positively associated with irritability at all levels of social anxiety. Therefore, if irritability is held constant, increasing social anxiety may blunt the link between irritability and perpetration but remain positively associated with generalized aggression.

Further explicating how changes in symptom levels may impact the ecology of peer-networks broadly, and bully perpetration specifically, may be an important factor to consider when evaluating treatment efficacy (Gaffney et al., Reference Gaffney, Ttofi and Farrington2021). Addressing psychopathology in individuals may lead to positive, measurable effects for some behaviors, while counter-intuitively creating negative effects in others, suggesting a need for researchers to comprehensively assess psychopathology and social behaviors rather than focus on individual syndromes or behavioral phenotypes. While treating social anxiety in adolescents will have myriad positive outcomes (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Masia-Warner and Klein2004), it is imperative to also address underlying irritability, lest improvements in one domain (anxiety) potentially lead to decrements in another (less prosocial behaviors). Additionally, given the link of irritability to other adverse social behaviors (e.g., victimization), understanding whether interventions have impacts across a wide variety of domains is imperative. Furthermore, bully perpetration behaviors are heterogenous (Farrell et al., Reference Farrell, Della Cioppa, Volk and Book2014) and evolving (Waasdorp et al., Reference Waasdorp, Pas, Zablotsky and Bradshaw2017) so further research is necessary to determine whether the moderating effect of social anxiety on linkages between irritability and bully perpetration are universal or differentially impact specific bullying behaviors. For example, bullying that doesn't require an audience (with a romantic partner) or cyberbullying, which may provide anonymity, may be less impacted by social anxiety compared to relational perpetration. The answers to these questions may help determine how to best target individual-level interventions based on behavioral phenotype.

This study needs to be considered in light of its limitations. The recruitment and assessment protocols used in this longitudinal study (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Belden, Pautsch, Si and Spitznagel2009) resulted in their being ample participants with symptoms of psychopathology. However, our measures of anxiety (clinician-rated symptoms) (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Ryan and Rao2000) and bully role behaviors (composite measure) (Essex et al., Reference Essex, Boyce, Goldstein, Armstrong, Kraemer, Kupfer and Group2002) would have benefitted by utilizing alternative information sources, such as peer reports and self-reports (Makol et al., Reference Makol, Youngstrom, Racz, Qasmieh, Glenn and De Los Reyes2020). It is currently unclear if the relations reported here would equally apply to all forms of bullying. For example, cyberbullying, which often requires less direct contact and may provide perpetrators with anonymity and physical distance from victims may demonstrate weaker links with social anxiety. This hypothesis was not testable in our dataset, but is worthy of further inquiry. Additionally, given the relatively small distribution of scores in our measures of anxiety, using measures with greater distribution across trait levels may help improve statistical assessments. To increase the precision of our moderation analyses examining the links between irritability and bully perpetration, we controlled for demographic variables and co-occuring bully role behaviors. We observed that social anxiety interaction effect accounted for 2% of the model variance, which would be considered a small effect. While our own findings partially replicate prior work, the need to replicate these effects with higher powered samples is paramount. While this work adds vital information by examining generalized aggression in addition to perpetration, it is important for future work to comprehensively assess how associations with psychopathology relate to other types of bully role behaviors, such as prosocial behavior (i.e., bystander intervention) (Jenkins et al., Reference Jenkins, Snyder Kaminski and Miller2021). We hypothesize that increasing social anxiety may moderate the relationship between irritability and other approach-oriented behaviors, be they antisocial or prosocial, and suggest this as a needed line of inquiry.

This study demonstrates that social anxiety significantly impacts the relationship between irritability and bully perpetration. Bully perpetration is a persistent (Espelage & Swearer, Reference Espelage and Swearer2003) and damaging (Brimblecombe et al., Reference Brimblecombe, Evans-Lacko, Knapp, King, Takizawa, Maughan and Arseneault2018) problem, and current interventions have positive, albeit small, effects (Fraguas et al., Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Ayora, Durán-Cutilla, Abregú-Crespo, Ezquiaga-Bravo, Martín-Babarro and Arango2021). Treating common symptoms can impact the relationship between perpetration and other related symptoms, which may ultimately provide informative insight into how symptom change can have cascading effects on reducing various forms of antisocial behavior.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424000439.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the children and caregivers recruited for the Preschool Depression Study for their time and commitment.

Funding statement

All phases of this study were supported by a National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant, R01 MH064769-06A1. Dr Perino’s work was supported by NIH grant: K99HD105002/R00HD105002 (PI: Perino). Dr Vogel was supported by NIH grant: T32 MH100019 (PI’s: Barch and Luby).

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Michael T. Perino and Jennifer C. Harper-Lednicky are co-first authors.

References

Angold, A., Prendergast, M., Cox, A., Harrington, R., Simonoff, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). The child and adolescent psychiatric assessment (CAPA). Psychological Medicine, 25(4), 739753. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329170003498X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brimblecombe, N., Evans-Lacko, S., Knapp, M., King, D., Takizawa, R., Maughan, B., & Arseneault, L. (2018). Long term economic impact associated with childhood bullying victimisation. Social Science & Medicine, 208, 134141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.05.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., Neckerman, H. J., Gest, S. D., & Gariepy, J.-L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive behavior: Peer support or peer rejection? Developmental Psychology, 24(6), 815823. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.6.815 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, H.-W. B., Gardner, E. S., Clarkson, T., Eaton, N. R., Wiggins, J. L., Leibenluft, E., & Jarcho, J. M. (2021). Bullying perpetration and victimization in youth: Associations with irritability and anxiety. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 53(5), 18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-021-01192-9 Google ScholarPubMed
Copeland, W. E., Brotman, M. A., & Costello, E. J. (2015). Normative irritability in youth: Developmental findings from the great smoky mountains study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(8), 635642. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.05.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dane, A. V., Lapierre, K. R., Andrews, N. C., & Volk, A. A. (2022). Evolutionarily relevant aggressive functions: Differentiating competitive, impression management, sadistic and reactive motives. Aggressive Behavior, 48(3), 331340. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.22020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, H. L., Angold, A., Small, B., & Copeland, W. (1999). The preschool age psychiatric assessment. The Oxford Handbook of Infant, Toddler, and Preschool Mental Health Assessment.Google Scholar
Espelage, D. L., & Holt, M. K. (2001). Bullying and victimization during early adolescence: Peer influences and psychosocial correlates. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 2(2-3), 123142. https://doi.org/10.1300/J135v02n02_08 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espelage, D. L., Low, S., Rao, M. A., Hong, J. S., & Little, T. D. (2014). Family violence, bullying, fighting, and substance use among adolescents: A longitudinal mediational model. Journal of Research On Adolescence, 24(2), 337349. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12060 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espelage, D. L., & Swearer, S. M. (2003). Research on school bullying and victimization: What have we learned and where do we go from here? School Psychology Review, 32(3), 365383. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2003.12086206 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espelage, D. L., Van Ryzin, M. J., & Holt, M. K. (2018). Trajectories of bully perpetration across early adolescence: Static risk factors, dynamic covariates, and longitudinal outcomes. Psychology of Violence, 8(2), 141150. https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000095 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Goldstein, L. H., Armstrong, J. M., Kraemer, H. C., Kupfer, D. J., & Group, M. A. B. W. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social, and academic difficulties in middle childhood. II: Developing the MacArthur health and behavior questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(5), 588603. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200205000-00017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, S. C., Burke, J. D., Roberts, M. C., Fite, P. J., Lochman, J. E., Francisco, R., & Reed, G. M. (2017). Irritability in child and adolescent psychopathology: An integrative review for ICD-11. Clinical Psychology Review, 53, 2945. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.01.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrell, A. H., Della Cioppa, V., Volk, A. A., & Book, A. S. (2014). Predicting bullying heterogeneity with the HEXACO model of personality. International Journal of Advances in Psychology, 3(2), 3039. https://doi.org/10.14355/IJAP.2014.0302.02 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, P. H., Masia-Warner, C., & Klein, R. G. (2004). Skills for social and academic success: A school-based intervention for social anxiety disorder in adolescents. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 7(4), 241249. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-004-6088-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraguas, D., Díaz-Caneja, C. M., Ayora, M., Durán-Cutilla, M., Abregú-Crespo, R., Ezquiaga-Bravo, I., Martín-Babarro, J., & Arango, C. (2021). Assessment of school anti-bullying interventions: A meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(1), 4455. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.3541 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaffney, H., Ttofi, M. M., & Farrington, D. P. (2021). What works in anti-bullying programs? Analysis of effective intervention components. Journal of School Psychology, 85, 3756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2020.12.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaffrey, M. S., Barch, D. M., Singer, J., Shenoy, R., & Luby, J. L. (2013). Disrupted amygdala reactivity in depressed 4-to 6-year-old children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(7), 737746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galván, A. (2013). Neural systems underlying reward and approach behaviors in childhood and adolescence. The Neurobiology of Childhood, 16, 167188. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2013_240 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garandeau, C. F., & Lansu, T. A. (2019). Why does decreased likeability not deter adolescent bullying perpetrators? Aggressive Behavior, 45(3), 348359. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21824 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gladden, R. M., Vivolo-Kantor, A. M., Hamburger, M. E., & Lumpkin, C. D. (2014). Bullying surveillance among youths: Uniform definitions for public health and recommended data elements, version 1.0. Atlanta, GA; National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and U.S. Department of Education; 2014. https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/pdf/bullying-definitions-final-a.pdf.Google Scholar
Hawley, P. H. (2002). Social dominance and prosocial and coercive strategies of resource control in preschoolers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(2), 167176. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250042000726 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawley, P. H. (2003a). Prosocial and coercive configurations of resource control in early adolescence: A case for the well-adapted Machiavellian. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, (1982-), 279309. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2003.0013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawley, P. H. (2003b). Strategies of control, aggression, and morality in preschoolers: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(3), 213235. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00073-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawley, P. H. (2015). Social dominance in childhood and its evolutionary underpinnings: Why it matters and what we can do. Pediatrics, 135(Supplement 2), S31S8. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-3549D CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawley, P. H., Little, T. D., & Card, N. A. (2007). The allure of a mean friend: Relationship quality and processes of aggressive adolescents with prosocial skills. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(2), 170180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025407074630 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawley, P. H., Stump, K. N., & Ratliff, J. (2010). Sidestepping the jingle fallacy: Bullying, aggression, and the importance of knowing the diff erence. In Bullying in north American schools (pp. 121136). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203842898 Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2016). The PROCESS macro for SPSS and SAS.Google Scholar
Hong, J. S., & Espelage, D. L. (2012). A review of research on bullying and peer victimization in school: An ecological system analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17(4), 311322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2012.03.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, K. L., Schouboe, S. N., Kircanski, K., Leibenluft, E., Stringaris, A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Irritability, externalizing, and internalizing psychopathology in adolescence: Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations and moderation by sex. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48(5), 781789. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2018.1460847 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jantzer, V., Schlander, M., Haffner, J., Parzer, P., Trick, S., Resch, F., & Kaess, M. (2019). The cost incurred by victims of bullying from a societal perspective: Estimates based on a German online survey of adolescents. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(4), 585594. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1224-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarcho, J. M., Grossman, H. Y., Guyer, A. E., Quarmley, M., Smith, A. R., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., Pine, D. S., & Nelson, E. E. (2019). Connecting childhood wariness to adolescent social anxiety through the brain and peer experiences. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(7), 11531164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00543-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, L. N., Snyder Kaminski, S., & Miller, M. (2021). Bystander intervention in bullying: Differences across latent profiles. International Journal of Bullying Prevention, 3(2), 130137. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42380-020-00067-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, P. O., & Neyman, J. (1936). Tests of certain linear hypotheses and their application to some educational problems. Statistical Research Memoirs.Google Scholar
Juvonen, J., Graham, S., & Schuster, M. A. (2003). Bullying among young adolescents: The strong, the weak, and the troubled. Pediatrics, 112(6), 12311237. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.112.6.1231 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juvonen, J., & Ho, A. Y. (2008). Social motives underlying antisocial behavior across middle school grades. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37(6), 747756. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9272-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D. A., Ryan, N. D., & Rao, U. (2000). K-SADS-PL. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(10), 1208. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200010000-00002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., Williamson, D., & Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaukiainen, A., Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., Österman, K., Salmivalli, C., Rothberg, S., & Ahlbom, A. (1999). The relationships between social intelligence, empathy, and three types of aggression. Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research On Aggression, 25(2), 8189. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1999)25: 3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D. N., Dougherty, L. R., Kessel, E. M., Silver, J., & Carlson, G. A. (2021). A transdiagnostic perspective on youth irritability. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(5), 437443. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211035101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klomek, A. B., Marrocco, F., Kleinman, M., Schonfeld, I. S., & Gould, M. S. (2008). Peer victimization, depression, and suicidiality in adolescents. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 38(2), 166180. https://doi.org/10.1521/suli.2008.38.2.166 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kokkinos, C. M., Voulgaridou, I., & Markos, A. (2016). Personality and relational aggression: Moral disengagement and friendship quality as mediators. Personality and Individual Differences, 95, 7479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.028 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leibenluft, E. (2017). Irritability in children: What we know and what we need to learn. World Psychiatry, 16(1), 100101. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20397 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. D., Henrich, C. C., Jones, S. M., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Disentangling the “whys” from the “whats” of aggressive behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(2), 122133. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250244000128 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, J. A. (2019). Interactions: Comprehensive, user-friendly toolkit for probing interactions. R Package Version, 1(0), 33.Google Scholar
Luby, J. L., & Belden, A. C. (2008). Clinical characteristics of bipolar vs. unipolar depression in preschool children: An empirical investigation. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(12), 19601969. https://doi.org/10.4088/jcp.v69n1216 1960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Belden, A. C., Pautsch, J., Si, X., & Spitznagel, E. (2009). The clinical significance of preschool depression: Impairment in functioning and clinical markers of the disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 112(1-3), 111119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.03.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Heffelfinger, A. K., Mrakotsky, C., Brown, K. M., Hessler, M. J., Wallis, J. M., & Spitznagel, E. L. (2003). The clinical picture of depression in preschool children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(3), 340348. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200303000-00015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Makol, B. A., Youngstrom, E. A., Racz, S. J., Qasmieh, N., Glenn, L. E., & De Los Reyes, A. (2020). Integrating multiple informants’ reports: How conceptual and measurement models may address long-standing problems in clinical decision-making. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(6), 953970. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702620924439 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCormick, E. M., & Telzer, E. H. (2017). Adaptive adolescent flexibility: Neurodevelopment of decision-making and learning in a risky context. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(3), 413423. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01061 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, J. G., Rubin, K. H., Erath, S. A., Wojslawowicz, J. C., & Buskirk, A. A. (2015). Developmental Psychopathology. One: Theory and Method, Developmental Psychopathology, 1, 419493. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939383.ch12 Google Scholar
Perino, M. T., Guassi Moreira, J. F., & Telzer, E. H. (2019). Links between adolescent bullying and neural activation to viewing social exclusion. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(6), 14671478. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00739-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perino, M. T., Miernicki, M. E., & Telzer, E. H. (2016). Letting the good times roll: Adolescence as a period of reduced inhibition to appetitive social cues. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(11), 17621771. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw096 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinstein, M. J., & Giletta, M. (2016). 12 peer relations and developmental psychopathology. Wiley.Google Scholar
Provenzano, D. A., Dane, A. V., Farrell, A. H., Marini, Z. A., & Volk, A. A. (2018). Do bullies have more sex? The role of personality. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 4(3), 221232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-017-0126-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, A. J., Swenson, L. P., & Waller, E. M. (2004). Overt and relational aggression and perceived popularity: Developmental differences in concurrent and prospective relations. Developmental Psychology, 40(3), 378387. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.3.378 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shanmugan, S., Wolf, D. H., Calkins, M. E., Moore, T. M., Ruparel, K., Hopson, R. D., Vandekar, S. N., Roalf, D. R., Elliott, M. A., Jackson, C., Gennatas, E. D., Leibenluft, E., Pine, D. S., Shinohara, R. T., Hakonarson, H., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2016). Common and dissociable mechanisms of executive system dysfunction across psychiatric disorders in youth. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(5), 517526. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15060725 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, R. S., La Greca, A. M., & Harrison, H. M. (2009). Peer victimization and social anxiety in adolescents: Prospective and reciprocal relationships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38(8), 10961109. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9392-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M., & Casey, B. (2010). A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 124133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spadafora, N., Al-Jbouri, E., McDowell, H., Andrews, N. C., & Volk, A. A. (2022). Be a little rude, but not too much: Exploring classroom incivility and social network position in adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 42(4), 565585. https://doi.org/10.1177/02724316211058074 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoddard, J., Stringaris, A., Brotman, M. A., Montville, D., Pine, D. S., & Leibenluft, E. (2014). Irritability in child and adolescent anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 31(7), 566573. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22151 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takizawa, R., Maughan, B., & Arseneault, L. (2014). Adult health outcomes of childhood bullying victimization: Evidence from a five-decade longitudinal British birth cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(7), 777784. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101401 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, H. J., Connor, J. P., & Scott, J. G. (2018). Why do children and adolescents bully their peers? A critical review of key theoretical frameworks. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 53(5), 437451. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-017-1462-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., & McDougall, P. (2003). Bullying is power: Implications for school-based intervention strategies. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 19(2), 157176. https://doi.org/10.1300/J008v19n02_10 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., & McDougall, P. (2013). The biological underpinnings of peer victimization: Understanding why and how the effects of bullying can last a lifetime. Theory into Practice, 52(4), 241248. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2013.829726 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vervoort, L., Wolters, L. H., Hogendoorn, S. M., De Haan, E., Boer, F., & Prins, P. J. (2010). Sensitivity of Gray’s behavioral inhibition system in clinically anxious and non-anxious children and adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(5), 629633. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.12.021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogel, A. C., Jackson, J. J., Barch, D. M., Tillman, R., & Luby, J. L. (2019). Excitability and irritability in preschoolers predicts later psychopathology: The importance of positive and negative emotion dysregulation. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 10671083. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000609 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volk, A. A., Camilleri, J. A., Dane, A. V., & Marini, Z. A. (2012). Is adolescent bullying an evolutionary adaptation? Aggressive Behavior, 38(3), 222238. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21418 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volk, A. A., Dane, A. V., & Marini, Z. A. (2014). What is bullying? A theoretical redefinition. Developmental Review, 34(4), 327343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.09.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volk, A. A., Dane, A. V., Marini, Z. A., & Vaillancourt, T. (2015). Adolescent bullying, dating, and mating: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Evolutionary Psychology, 13(4), 111. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704915613909 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waasdorp, T. E., Pas, E. T., Zablotsky, B., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2017). Ten-year trends in bullying and related attitudes among 4th-to 12th-graders. Pediatrics, 139(6), e20162615. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2615 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, R. B. (2009). Overview of generalized anxiety disorder: Epidemiology, presentation, and course. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70(Suppl 2), 49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolke, D., Copeland, W. E., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2013). Impact of bullying in childhood on adult health, wealth, crime, and social outcomes. Psychological Science, 24(10), 19581970. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613481608 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, A. D., Davis-Kean, P., Halvorson, M., King, K. M., Logan, J. R., Xu, M., Bainter, S., Brown, D., Clay, J. M., Cruz, R. A., Elsherif, M. M., Gerasimova, D., Joyal-Desmarais, K., Moreau, D., Nissen, J., Schmidt, K., Uzdavines, A., Van Dusen, B., & Vasilev, M. (2021). Missing data and multiple imputation decision tree. PsyArXiv.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of demographic, clinical and behavioral variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Correlation matrix for demographic, clinical and behavioral variables

Figure 2

Figure 1. Social anxiety moderates the relationship between irritability and bully perpetration. The Johnson–Neyman technique was employed to explore the moderating effect of social anxiety on the relationships between irritability and bully perpetration, while controlling for demographic variables (age, sex, race). We observed that when social anxiety symptom endorsement was low (below 0.92 symptoms), the association between irritability and perpetration was significant and positive. As social anxiety increased, we saw a corresponding reduction to nonsignificance in the association between irritability and perpetration.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Moderation effects of social anxiety between irritability and forms of aggressive behaviors. The Johnson–Neyman technique was employed to explore the moderating effect of social anxiety on the relationships between (a) irritability and bully perpetration, and (b) irritability and generalized aggression, while controlling for other bully role behaviors and demographic variables. For bully perpetration, we saw a nonsignificant negative effect when social anxiety endorsement was low, and a significant, negative effect when social anxiety endorsement was greater than or equal to 1.12 symptoms (CI [−.08, .0]). Increasing social anxiety corresponded with a negative relationship between irritability and bully perpetration. For generalized aggression, there was no statistical significance transition point, as the relationship between irritability and generalized aggression was the same regardless of the level of social anxiety.

Supplementary material: File

Perino et al. supplementary material

Perino et al. supplementary material
Download Perino et al. supplementary material(File)
File 37.5 KB