Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T22:06:08.316Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Risk and resilience in preterm children at age 6

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 September 2014

Julie Poehlmann-Tynan*
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin–Madison
Emily D. Gerstein
Affiliation:
University of Missouri–St. Louis
Cynthia Burnson
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin–Madison
Lindsay Weymouth
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin–Madison
Daniel M. Bolt
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin–Madison
Sarah Maleck
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin–Madison
A. J. Schwichtenberg
Affiliation:
Purdue University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Julie Poehlmann-Tynan, Department of Human Development & Family Studies, University of Wisconsin, 1300 Linden Drive, Madison, WI 53706; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Children born preterm are at risk for experiencing significant deleterious developmental outcomes throughout their childhood and adolescence. However, individual variation and resilience are hallmarks of the preterm population. The present study examined pathways to resilience across multiple domains (e.g., social activities, peer relations, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptomology, externalizing and internalizing behavior, and sleep quality) as children born preterm reached school age. The study also examined early child and family predictors of resilience. Using a prospective longitudinal design, 173 infants born preterm and without significant neurological complications were assessed at five time points: neonatal intensive care unit discharge, 9 months, 16 months, 24 months, and 6 years. Three pathways of adaptation emerged at 6 years: children who were resilient, those who remained at-risk, and children who exhibited significant difficulties. Resilient children were less likely to have experienced negative parenting at 9 and 16 months, more likely to delay gratification at 24 months, and more likely to experience neonatal health complications than nonresilient children.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. DSM-IV-TR (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Anders, T. F., & Keener, M. A. (1985). Developmental course of nighttime sleep-wake patterns in full-term and premature infants during the first year of life: I. Sleep, 8, 173192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asaka, Y., & Takada, S. (2010). Activity-based assessment of the sleep behaviors of VLBW preterm infants and full-term infants at around 12 months of age. Brain and Development, 32, 150155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Mischel, W., Downey, G., Peake, P. K., & Rodriguez, M. (2000). Regulating the interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 776792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aylward, G. P. (2005). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of infants born prematurely. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 427440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 6594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bhutta, A. T., Cleaves, M. A., Casey, P. H., Cradock, M. M., & Anand, K. J. S. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288, 728737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N., Powls, A., Cooke, R., & Marlow, N. (1997). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and other psychiatric outcomes in very low birthweight children at 12 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 931941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, B., Whiteside, L., Mundfrom, D. J., Casey, P. H., Kelleher, K. J., & Pope, S. K. (1994). Early indicators of resilience an their relation to experiences in the home environments of low birthweight, premature children living in poverty. Child Development, 65, 346360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, L. (2007). Heart rate variability in premature infants during feeding. Biological Research for Nursing, 8, 283293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buka, S. L., Lipsitt, L. P., & Tsuang, M. T. (1992). Emotional and behavioral development of low-birth weight infants. In Friedman, S. L. & Sigman, M. D. (Eds.), Annual advances in applied developmental psychology: Vol. 6. The psychological development of low-birthweight children (pp. 187214). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Bul, K. C., & van Baar, A. L. (2012). Behavior problems in relation to sustained selective attention skills of moderately preterm children. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 24, 111123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caravale, B., Tozzi, C., Albino, G., & Vicari, S. (2005). Cognitive development in low risk preterm infants at 3–4 years of life. Archives of Disease in Childhood: Fetal and Neonatal, 90, 474479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, C. A. C., Woodward, L. J., Horwood, L. J., & Moor, S. (2008). Development of emotional and behavioral regulation in children born extremely preterm and very preterm: Biological and social influences. Child Development, 79, 14441462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, R. (1985). The Parent–Child Early Relational Assessment. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Medical School, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Clark, R. (1999). The Parent–Child Early Relational Assessment: A factorial validity study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 59, 821846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R., Paulson, A., & Conlin, S. (1993). Assessment of developmental status and parent–infant relationships: The therapeutic process of evaluation. In Zeanah, C. (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (pp. 191209). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners third edition manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Crnic, K. A., Ragozin, A. S., Greenberg, M. T., Robinson, N. M., & Basham, R. B. (1983). Social interaction and developmental competence of preterm and full-term infants during the first year of life. Child Development, 54, 11991210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidoff, J. M., Dias, T., Damus, K., Russell, R., Bettegowda, V. R., Dolan, S., et al. (2006). Changes in the gestational age distribution among U.S. singleton births: Impact on rates of late preterm birth, 1992 to 2002. Seminars in Perinatology, 30, 815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, D. W., & Burns, B. (2001). Problems of self-regulation: A new way to view deficits in children born prematurely. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 22, 305323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, L., Edwards, H., Mohay, H., & Wollin, J. (2003). The impact of very premature birth on the psychological health of mothers. Early Human Development, 73, 6170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiPietro, J. A., & Allen, M. C. (1991). Estimation of gestational age: Implications for developmental research. Child Development, 62, 11841199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 3372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, R. (2006). From biological rhythms to social rhythms: Physiological precursors of mother–infant synchrony. Developmental Psychology, 42, 175188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R. (2009). The development of regulatory functions from birth to 5 years: Insights from premature infantas. Child Development, 80, 544561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, R. F., Indurkhya, A., & McCormick, M. C. (2004). Prevalence, stability, and predictors of clinically significant behavior problems in low birth weight children at 3, 5, and 8 years of age. Pediatrics, 114, 736743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. G., Fox, N. A., & Lewis, M. (1983). The relationship between neonatal characteristics and three-month mother–infant interaction in high-risk infants. Child Development, 54, 12861296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halperin, J. M., Trampush, J. W., Miller, C. J., Marks, D. J., & Newcorn, J. H. (2008). Neuropsychological outcome in adolescents/young adults with childhood ADHD: Profiles of persisters, remitters and controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 958966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, B. E., Martin, J. A., & Ventura, S. J. (2012). Births: Preliminary data for 2011. National Vital Statistics Reports, 61, 120.Google ScholarPubMed
Harris, M. N., Voigt, R. G., Barbaresi, W. J., Voge, G. A., Killian, J. M., Weaver, A. L., et al. (2013). ADHD and learning disabilities in former late preterm infants: A population-based birth cohort. Pediatrics, 132, e630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, J. L., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Waldfogel, J. (2003). Sustained effects of high participation in an early intervention for low-birth-weight premature infants. Developmental Psychology, 39, 730744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., Fan, C., Jassy, J. S., & Owens, E. B. (2007). Neuropsychological functioning of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder followed prospectively into adolescence: Evidence for continuing deficits?. Neuropsychology, 21, 263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingersoll, E. W., & Thoman, E. B. (1999). Sleep/wake states of preterm infants: Stability, developmental change, diurnal variation, and relation with caregiving activity. Child Development, 70, 110.Google ScholarPubMed
Institute of Medicine of the Academies, Assuring Healthy Outcomes. (2006). Neurodevelopmental, health, and family outcomes for infants born preterm. In Behrman, R. E. & Butler, A. S. (Eds.), Preterm birth: Causes, consequences, and prevention (pp. 346397). Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Jaekel, J., Wolke, D., & Chernova, J. (2012). Mother and child behaviour in very preterm and term dyads at 6 and 8 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 54, 716723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopp, C. B. (1990). Risk in infancy: Appraising the research. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 36, 117140.Google Scholar
Landry, S. H., Chapieski, M. L., Richardson, M. A., Palmer, J., & Hall, S. (1990). The social competence of children born prematurely: Effects of medical complications and parent behaviors. Child Development, 61, 16051616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C., & Swank, P. (1997). Predicting cognitive language and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varying degrees of biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 6, 10401053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindström, K., Lindblad, F., & Hjern, A. (2011). Preterm birth and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in schoolchildren. Pediatrics, 127, 858865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Littman, B., & Parmelee, A. (1978). Medical correlates of infant development. Pediatrics, 61, 470474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loe, I. M., Lee, E. S., Luna, B., & Feldman, H. M. (2011). Behavior problems of 9- to 16-year-old preterm children: Biological, sociodemographic, and intellectual contributions. Early Human Development, 87, 247252. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.01.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundqvist-Persson, C., Lau, G., Nordin, P., Bona, E., & Sabel, K. (2012). Preterm infants' early developmental status is associated with later developmental outcome. Acta Paediatrica, 101, 172178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luthar, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: Implications for interventions and social policies. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 857885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
March of Dimes. (2007). Peristats: Preterm birth rates in the United States, 1997–2007. Retrieved from http://www.marchofdimes.com/peristatsGoogle Scholar
Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Sutton, P. D., Ventura, S. J., Mathews, T. J., Kirmeyer, S., et al. (2010). Births: Final data for 2007. National Vital Statistics Reports, 58(24), 1125.Google ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (1999). Resilience comes of age: Reflections on the past and outlook for the next generation of research. In Glantz, M. D. & Johnson, J. L. (Eds.), Resilience and development: Positive life adaptations (pp. 281296). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on successful children. American Psychologist, 53, 205220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Obradović, J. (2006). Competence and resilience in development. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 1327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Reed, M. J. (2002). Resilience in development. In Snyder, C. R. & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 7488). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, M., Ho, J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2012). Executive functions in girls with ADHD followed prospectively into young adulthood. Neuropsychology, 26, 278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratificaiton in children. Science, 244, 933938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morse, S. B., Zheng, H., Tang, Y., & Roth, J. (2009). Early school-age outcomes of late preterm infants. Pediatrics, 123, e622e629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus user's guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles: Author.Google Scholar
Nadeau, L., Tessier, R., Lefebvre, F., & Robaey, P. (2004). Victimization: A newly recognized outcome of prematurity. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46, 508513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obradović, J. (2010). Effortful control and adaptive functioning of homeless children: Variable-focused and person-focused analyses. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 109117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, L. M., Holbrook, C. R., Mervis, C. B., Klaus, C. J., Bruner, J. L., Raffield, T. J., et al. (2003). Sleep and neurobehavioral characteristics of 5- to 7-year-old children with parentally reported symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 111, 554563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, M., Asay, J. H., & McCluskey-Fawcett, K. (1999). Family functioning and maternal depression following premature birth. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 17, 175188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owens, J. A., Spirito, A., & McGuinn, M. (2000). The children's sleep habits questionnaire (CSHQ): Psychometric properties of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep, 23, 10431051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pastor, D. A., Barron, K. E., Miller, B. J., & Davis, S. L. (2007). A latent profile analysis of college students' achievement goal orientation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poehlmann, J., & Eddy, J. M. (2010). Children of incarcerated parents: An agenda for developmental and intervention research. In Eddy, J. M. & Poehlmann, J. (Eds.), Children of incarcerated parents: A handbook for researchers and practitioners. Washington, DC: Urban Institute Press.Google Scholar
Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. J. M., Bolt, D., & Dilworth-Bart, J. (2009). Predictors of depressive symptom trajectories in mothers of preterm or low birth weight infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 690704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. J. M., Bolt, D., Hane, A., Burnson, C., & Winters, J. (2011). Infant physiological regulation and maternal risks as predictors of dyadic interaction trajectories in families with a preterm infant. Developmental Psychology, 47, 91105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. J. M., Shah, P., Shlafer, R., Hahn, E., & Maleck, S. J. (2010). The development of effortful control in children born preterm. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 522536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. J. M., Shlafer, R. J., Hahn, E., Bianchi, J.-P., & Warner, R. L. (2011). Emerging self-regulation in toddlers born preterm or low birthweight: Differential susceptibility to early parenting? Development and Psychopathology, 23, 177193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pridham, K., Lin, C., & Brown, R. (2001). Mothers' evaluation of their caregiving for premature and full-term infants through the first year: Contributing factors. Research in Nursing and Health, 24, 157169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quigley, M. A., Poulsen, G., Boyle, E., Wolke, D., Field, D., Alfirevic, Z., et al. (2012). Early term and late preterm birth are associated with poorer school performance at age 5 years: A cohort study. Archives of Disease in Childhood–Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 97, F167F173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 13941408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saigal, S., & Doyle, L. W. (2008). An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet, 371, 261269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, D. T., Bauer, C. R., Kraemer, H. C., & Tyson, J. (1997). The neonatal health index. In Gross, R. T., Spiker, D., & Haynes, C. W. (Eds.), Helping low birthweight premature babies (pp. 341357). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Shah, P. E., Robbins, N. C., Coelho, R. B., & Poehlmann, J. (2013). The paradox of prematurity: The behavioral vulnerability of late preterm infants and the cognitive susceptibility of very preterm infants at 36 months post-term. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 5062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K., Landry, S. H., & Swank, P. R. (2006). The role of early maternal responsiveness in supporting school aged cognitive development for children who vary in birth status. Pediatrics, 117, 16081617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommerfelt, K., Ellertsen, B., & Markestad, T. (1993). Personality and behaviour in eight-year-old, non-handicapped children with birth weight under 1500 g. Acta Paediatrica, 82, 723728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, J., Mohay, H., & O'Callaghan, M. (2008). A comparison of executive function in very preterm and term infants at 8 months corrected age. Early Human Development, 85, 225230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H. G., Klein, N., & Hack, M. (2000). School age consequences of birthweight less than 750 g: A review and update. Developmental Psychology, 17, 289321.Google Scholar
Treyvaud, K., Indur, T. E., Lee, K. J., Northam, E. A., Doyle, L. W., & Anderson, P. J. (2012). Can the home environment promote resilience for children born very preterm in the context of social and medical risk? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 326337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Baar, A. L., Vermaas, J., Knots, E., de Kleine, M. K., & Soons, P. (2009). Functioning at school age of moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 weeks' gestational age. Pediatrics, 124, 251257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vergara, E. R., & Bigsby, R. (2004). Developmental and therapeutic interventions in the NICU. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Voigt, B., Pietz, J., Pauen, S., Kliegel, M., & Reuner, G. (2012). Cognitive development in very vs. moderately to late preterm and full-term children: Can effortful control account for group differences in toddlerhood? Early Human Development, 88, 307313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 13361346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, J. W., Buka, S. L., McCormick, M. C., Fitzmaurice, G. M., & Indurkhya, A. (2006). Behavioral problems and the effects of early intervention on eight-year-old children with learning disabilities. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 10, 329338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, Q., Chen, S. H., & Main, A. (2012). Commonalities and differences in the research on children's effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self-regulation. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 112121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar