Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T19:23:59.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pubertal development mediates the association between family environment and brain structure and function in childhood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2019

Sandra Thijssen*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Netherlands Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Paul F. Collins
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Monica Luciana
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Sandra Thijssen, Erasmus University of Rotterdam, Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, P.O. Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, the Netherlands; E-mail: [email protected]; Permanent E-mail: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Psychosocial acceleration theory suggests that pubertal maturation is accelerated in response to adversity. In addition, suboptimal caregiving accelerates development of the amygdala–medial prefrontal cortex circuit. These findings may be related. Here, we assess whether associations between family environment and measures of the amygdala–medial prefrontal cortex circuit are mediated by pubertal development in more than 2000 9- and 10-year-old children from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study (http://dx.doi.org/10.15154/1412097). Using structural equation modeling, demographic, child-reported, and parent-reported data on family dynamics were compiled into a higher level family environment latent variable. Magnetic resonance imaging preprocessing and compilations were performed by the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study's data analysis core. Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) thickness, area, white matter fractional anisotropy, amygdala volume, and cingulo-opercular network–amygdala resting-state functional connectivity were assessed. For ACC cortical thickness and ACC fractional anisotropy, significant indirect effects indicated that a stressful family environment relates to more advanced pubertal stage and more mature brain structure. For cingulo-opercular network–amygdala functional connectivity, results indicated a trend in the expected direction. For ACC area, evidence for quadratic mediation by pubertal stage was found. Sex-stratified analyses suggest stronger results for girls. Despite small effect sizes, structural measures of circuits important for emotional behavior are associated with family environment and show initial evidence of accelerated pubertal development.

Type
Regular Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

During childhood and to a lesser extent during adolescence, the quality of parenting and the larger family environment exert a considerable effect on several domains of child development. It is not surprising then that suboptimal environments are associated with poor outcomes. For example, parental separation and parental or parent–child conflict have been associated with child behavior problems and poor school performance (e.g., Erman & Härkönen, Reference Erman and Härkönen2017; Harold, Aitken, & Shelton, Reference Harold, Aitken and Shelton2007; Kreidl, Štípková, & Hubatková, Reference Kreidl, Štípková and Hubatková2017; Moed et al., Reference Moed, Gershoff, Eisenberg, Hofer, Losoya, Spinrad and Liew2017). Associations with physical development have also been reported, suggesting that children experiencing family adversity may have an earlier onset of puberty (e.g., Ellis & Garber, Reference Ellis and Garber2000; Jorm, Christensen, Rodgers, Jacomb, & Easteal, Reference Jorm, Christensen, Rodgers, Jacomb and Easteal2004; Moffitt, Caspi, Belsky, & Silva, Reference Moffitt, Caspi, Belsky and Silva1992). Caregiving also affects brain development in ways that are only beginning to be understood. In a typically developing, nonclinical sample, lower levels of early parental sensitivity were associated with smaller total brain and gray matter volume (controlled for infant head size) later in childhood (Kok et al., Reference Kok, Thijssen, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Verhulst, White and Tiemeier2015). In a different study, maternal sensitivity toward the child at age 12 predicted reduced growth in the amygdala and greater thinning of the orbitofrontal cortex 4 years later (Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Simmons, Dennison, Vijayakumar, Schwartz, Yap and Allen2014). Similarly to pubertal development, there is some evidence that brain development, specifically the functional development of the amygdala–medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) circuit, may be accelerated in the context of lower levels of supportive parental care (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013; Thijssen et al., Reference Thijssen, Muetzel, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Tiemeier, Verhulst and van IJzendoorn2017). It has been hypothesized that this acceleration may, in turn, impact the child's emotional functioning. However, it is unclear how these findings relate to the reports of accelerated pubertal development. Here we aim to examine whether associations between a child's family environment and measures of the amygdala–mPFC circuit are mediated by child pubertal stage in a large sample of 9- to 10-year-old children characterized by a broad range of pubertal development.

Accelerated brain development in response to adversity has been reported for the amygdala–mPFC circuit. The first of this circuit's subparts, the amygdala, is suggested to play a role in emotional learning and may facilitate attention to salient cues (Phelps & LeDoux, Reference Phelps and LeDoux2005). The mPFC, in contrast, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), has been implicated in higher order emotional and cognitive functioning (Forbes & Grafman, Reference Forbes and Grafman2010; Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis, Reference Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone and Nieuwenhuis2004), and may provide top-down regulation of amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli. Functional coupling of the amygdala and mPFC may thus be involved in emotion regulation (Hariri, Mattay, Tessitore, Fera, & Weinberger, Reference Hariri, Mattay, Tessitore, Fera and Weinberger2003; Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, Reference Ochsner, Bunge, Gross and Gabrieli2002; Phan et al., Reference Phan, Fitzgerald, Nathan, Moore, Uhde and Tancer2005). In children, responses of the amygdala and mPFC to emotional stimuli (i.e., fearful faces) are positively correlated (Gee, Humphreys, et al., Reference Gee, Humphreys, Flannery, Goff, Telzer, Shapiro and Tottenham2013). In contrast, adolescents and adults show a negative pattern of amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity in response to the same stimuli, which is interpreted as effective top-down control of the amygdala by the mPFC (however, as findings are correlational, the opposite pattern of the amygdala decreasing mPFC activity could also be true). In childhood, emotion regulation skills are still developing, and parents play an important role in helping their children regulate their emotions (Tottenham, Reference Tottenham2015). When a photo of the mother (vs. a stranger) is presented during a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task, children show evidence of maternal buffering and, similar to adolescents and adults, display negative connectivity between the amygdala and the mPFC when processing fearful faces (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Telzer, Humphreys, Goff, Shapiro and Tottenham2014). Of importance, previously institutionalized youth have been shown to demonstrate negative connectivity earlier in development and without presentation of parental stimuli, suggesting accelerated development in response to extreme early life adversity (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013). Following up on this finding, Thijssen et al. (Reference Thijssen, Muetzel, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Tiemeier, Verhulst and van IJzendoorn2017) showed that, in typically developing 6- to 10-year-old children, amygdala–mPFC connectivity at rest increased with age in children with less sensitive parents, but it did not in children with more sensitive parents. As amygdala–mPFC resting-state functional connectivity has been found to increase from age 10.5 onward (Gabard-Durnam et al., Reference Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Gee, Humphreys, Telzer and Tottenham2014), these results provide initial evidence for accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit in response to normative, yet less than optimal, caregiving.

These findings of accelerated maturation in response to caregiving adversity are in line with psychosocial acceleration theory (Belsky, Steinberg, & Draper, Reference Belsky, Steinberg and Draper1991). This theory poses that children adaptively adjust their development to match local conditions. Parental care and investment provide children with information about availability and predictability of resources and relationships, with less than optimal care suggesting scarcity of resources and low quality of interpersonal relationships. According to this theory, children may respond to adversity with a speeding up of development, as this may ultimately increase their reproductive opportunities and success. Several studies have shown that parental warmth is associated with delayed puberty in girls (defined as later menarche, earlier Tanner stage, or earlier pubertal stage according to the Pubertal Development Scale; Ellis, McFadyen-Ketchum, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, Reference Ellis, McFadyen-Ketchum, Dodge, Pettit and Bates1999; Graberc, Brooks-Gunn, & Warren, Reference Graberc, Brooks-Gunn and Warren1995; Romans, Martin, Gendall, & Herbison, Reference Romans, Martin, Gendall and Herbison2003), and that family conflict or parental psychopathology is associated with the earlier initiation of puberty (e.g., Ellis & Garber, Reference Ellis and Garber2000; Jorm et al., Reference Jorm, Christensen, Rodgers, Jacomb and Easteal2004; Moffitt et al., Reference Moffitt, Caspi, Belsky and Silva1992). Similarly, accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit may have implications for child fitness and survival. Although a long childhood is critical for the development of highly competent behavior and long-term well-being, in the short run, it may be adaptive to be capable of self-regulation instead of relying on a parent for emotion regulation when quality of parental care is low, even at the cost of long-term health or well-being (Belsky, Ruttle, Boyce, Armstrong, & Essex, Reference Belsky, Ruttle, Boyce, Armstrong and Essex2015; Hochberg & Belsky, Reference Hochberg and Belsky2013).

It is currently unclear what mechanisms may explain the accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit. It may be that a suboptimal family environment constitutes a source of chronic stress. This stress response may directly affect amygdala–mPFC circuit development (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013). Alternatively, given that an adverse family background has also been associated with precocious pubertal development, it is possible that the precocious emergence of the adolescent/adult pattern of amygdala–mPFC connectivity is a consequence of an early onset of puberty. Several studies have shown correlations between pubertal hormones and brain development and thus provide some evidence of organizing effects of adrenal and gonadal hormones. While sex-dimorphic effects are prominent (Bramen et al., Reference Bramen, Hranilovich, Dahl, Forbes, Chen, Toga and Sowell2011), studies have also reported strong similarities between associations of estradiol and testosterone and adolescent brain development (Herting, Gautam, Spielberg, & Dahl, Reference Herting, Gautam, Spielberg and Dahl2015). Similarly, pubertal developmental stage has been related to brain development in both sexes (Goddings et al., Reference Goddings, Mills, Clasen, Giedd, Viner and Blakemore2014; Herting et al., Reference Herting, Gautam, Spielberg and Dahl2015). In some studies, previously institutionalized children have been reported to experience an earlier onset of puberty (Adolfsson & Westphal, Reference Adolfsson and Westphal1981; Proos, Reference Proos2009; Teilmann, Pedersen, Skakkebaek, & Jensen, Reference Teilmann, Pedersen, Skakkebaek and Jensen2006), which may relate to the observation of accelerated amygdala–mPFC development in this group. However, several more recent studies reported nonsignificant associations between previous institutionalization and earlier age of menarche (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Tang, Almas, Degnan, McLaughlin, Nelson and Drury2018; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Miller, Dorn, Desjardins, Donzella and Gunnar2017). Thus, the literature is inconsistent, which could be due to methodological distinctions across studies.

Although evidence has been found for accelerated functional development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013; Thijssen et al., Reference Thijssen, Muetzel, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Tiemeier, Verhulst and van IJzendoorn2017), it is also possible that the development of amygdala and mPFC structure is accelerated in response to family adversity or lower quality care. It has been suggested that developmental changes in connectivity may be the consequence of changes in structural connections between two brain regions (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013; Wendelken et al., Reference Wendelken, Ferrer, Ghetti, Bailey, Cutting and Bunge2017). Similarly, although not perfectly, brain function and gray matter structure have been found to correlate (Lu et al., Reference Lu, Dapretto, O'Hare, Kan, McCourt, Thompson and Sowell2009), and accelerated development of one modality may suggest acceleration in others. In line with this hypothesis, Tyborowska et al. (Reference Tyborowska, Volman, Niermann, Pouwels, Cillessen, Toni and Roelofs2018) report increased developmental gray matter reductions from age 14 to 17 in several brain regions such as the amygdala, insula, and prefrontal cortex in relation to early life stress.

The present study used data from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, an epidemiologically informed sample of 9- to 10-year-old children, to assess the association between family environment and child structural (T1 and diffusion tensor imaging [DTI] data) and functional (resting-state fMRI) brain measures related to the amygdala–mPFC circuit made available by ABCD in the context of the project's first public data release (see Methods). We further examined whether such associations are mediated by pubertal development. We hypothesize that associations between more stressful family environments and child brain structure and function are mediated by accelerated pubertal development. As a precocious onset of puberty in response to a suboptimal family environment has been reported mostly for girls (Ellis, Reference Ellis2004), and as exploratory analyses by Thijssen et al. (Reference Thijssen, Muetzel, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Tiemeier, Verhulst and van IJzendoorn2017) suggest that accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit may be more prominent in girls than in boys, we also performed exploratory analyses stratified by sex.

Method

Participants

The present study used data collected for the ABCD Study (first public data release: http://dx.doi.org/10.15154/1412097, downloaded on February 15, 2017; for resting-state fMRI data, data from release 1.1 was used: http://dx.doi.org/10.15154/1412097). The ABCD Study aims to follow a population-based, prospective cohort from ages 9 to 10 years and onward. Data are collected across 21 sites in the United States. Up until the first data release, 4,524 participants were recruited and tested. For information on ABCD recruitment, please see Garavan et al. (Reference Garavan, Bartsch, Conway, Decastro, Goldstein, Heeringa and Zahs2018). The ABCD Study includes a representative sample of both twin and nontwin participants as well as siblings within families. For the present study, 430 twin pairs were excluded, and from each of 83 sibling pairs, 1 child was randomly excluded from the analysis. From an additional 28 sibling pairs, 1 sibling had unusable resting-state fMRI data, and this individual was excluded (see Quality Assurance). We further excluded 194 participants who attended the research center supervised by someone other than their biological parent, in order to increase the validity of the parent reported measures. Finally, 176 children were excluded due to MRI incidental findings. These exclusions resulted in a final sample of 3,183 children. For these children, parents provided informed consent, and children provided assent to participate. Data collection for the ABCD Study was approved by a centralized internal review board of the University of San Diego, as well as by the review boards of all research sites.

Measures

Family environment

For a detailed description and rationale for the measures collected in the ABCD Study that may be relevant for the family environment construct, please see Barch et al. (Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz and Sher2017) for demographic, physical and mental health assessments, and Zucker et al. (Reference Zucker, Gonzalez, Feldstein Ewing, Paulus, Arroyo, Fuligni and Wills2018) for the assessment of culture and environment.

In order to create a latent measure reflecting the quality of family environment, three types of information were used: child-reported information about family dynamics and relationships, parent-reported information about family dynamics and relationships, and demographic and parent information. Child-reported questionnaires included an abbreviated version of the maternal acceptance scale of the Child Report of Parent Behavior Inventory (Schaefer, Reference Schaefer1965), the conflict scale from the Family Environment Scale (FES; Moos & Moos, Reference Moos and Moos1976), and the Parental Monitoring Survey (Chilcoat & Anthony, Reference Chilcoat and Anthony1996). The abbreviated version of the maternal acceptance scale of the Child Report of Parent Behavior Inventory assesses maternal acceptance versus rejection and consists of 5 items (e.g., “smiles at me very often”) measured on a 3-point Likert scale ranging from not like her to a lot like her. The Parental Monitoring Survey consists of 5 items answered on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from never to always or almost always (e.g., “how often do your parents know where you are?”), and assesses whether the child believes that his/her parent knows of his/her whereabouts and activities. The conflict scale of the FES consists of 9 true or false items that aim to measure conflict within the family (e.g., “we fight a lot in our family”). The parent-reported information also included the conflict scale from the FES, and was complimented by one background item from the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci and Ryan1997) measuring the relationship between the parent and the child (“in general, how do you and your child get along?” using a 3-point Likert scale ranging from very well to a lot of conflict). Demographic and parent information included family yearly income (rated on a 10-point scale ranging from less than $5,000 to $200,000 or more), parental education (6-point scale ranging from finished high school or less to professional school or doctoral degree; PhenX; Stover, Harlan, Hammond, Hendershot, & Hamilton, Reference Stover, Harlan, Hammond, Hendershot and Hamilton2010), parental psychopathology (total problem score of the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment Adult Self-Report; Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003), parental relationship status (i.e., are biological parents still together?), and the planned nature of the pregnancy (i.e., did parents plan this pregnancy; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Avenevoli, Costello, Green, Gruber, Heeringa and Zaslavsky2009).

Using Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2007), all questionnaire items were submitted to structural equation modeling (except for the income and parental psychopathology variables, all variables were classified as categorical). We excluded variables with factor loadings <0.2. For the child-reported items, items were first loaded on a latent variable referring to the questionnaire scale. These questionnaire scale latent variables were then combined in a child-reported latent variable. All parent-reported items (9 FES and 1 Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia items) were combined in a parent-reported latent variable. However, 2 items of the FES showed a low factor loading (<0.2) on the parent variable and were removed from the model. The variables referring to demographical information were combined into a demographics latent variable. The child-reported (STD standardized loading = 0.53), parent-reported (STD standardized loading = 0.55), and demographical latent variables (STD standardized loading = 0.44) were then combined to yield an overall family environment latent variable (see Supplemental Figure S.1 for all factor loadings). The model had reasonable fit, root mean square error of approximation = .040, 95% confidence interval (CI) [.038, .041], comparative fit index = .87, and Tucker–Lewis index = .86. Low scores on the family environment variable indicate a more stressful/less supportive family environment (i.e., increased family conflict, lower parental acceptance and monitoring, lower socioeconomic status, and/or higher parental psychopathology).

Although the child- and parent-reported measures of family dynamics reflect concurrent and not early life relationships, parent–child relationship patterns are relatively stable over time (Loeber et al., Reference Loeber, Drinkwater, Yin, Anderson, Schmidt and Crawford2000), as is socioeconomic status. The demographic latent variable includes a broader time frame of events (planned nature of pregnancy and parental separation during child's life).

Child behavior

In order to assess the predictive validity of the family environment variable, family environment was associated with measures of child behavior. From the multitude of child behavior measures available (please see Barch et al., Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz and Sher2017; Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Gonzalez, Feldstein Ewing, Paulus, Arroyo, Fuligni and Wills2018, for the conclusive list), we included measures of both positive behavior and behavioral problems, reported by both the parent and the child. As such, we included the broadbent internalizing and externalizing scales of the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edelbrook, Reference Achenbach and Edelbrook1983), and the parent- and child-reported prosocial scale of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, Meltzer, & Bailey, Reference Goodman, Meltzer and Bailey1998). The items comprising the internalizing (M = 5.22, SD = 5.44) and externalizing (M = 4.25, SD = 5.45) scales can be answered on a 3-point Likert scale. The prosocial scale consists of five items that are answered on a 3-point Likert scale (1 = not true, 2 = somewhat true, 3 = certainly true). Three of the five items were retained in the ABCD Study (M = 1.75, SD = 0.40; M = 1.69, SD = 0.36, for parent and child report, respectively).

The child behavior measures were nonnormally distributed, with many parents reporting few behavioral problems and many parents and children reporting high prosocial behavior. Thus, the Child Behavior Checklist scales were log transformed, and prosocial behavior scales were inverse transformed.

Pubertal stage

Both the child and the parent reported on the child's pubertal stage using the Pubertal Development Scale (Petersen, Crockett, Richards, & Boxer, Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988). Correlations between the parent and child measures were r = .562, p < .001 for girls and r = .197, p < .001 for boys, respectively. More parent-reported compared to child-reported pubertal stage scores were available for the sample (3,107 vs. 2,918). Moreover, whereas age correlated significantly with the parent-reported score for both boys and girls (r = .096 p < .001, r = .265 p < .001, respectively), as well as with the self-reported score for girls (r = .239, p < .001), the correlation between age and self-reported pubertal stage for boys was not significant (r = –.030, p = .218). We, therefore, decided to use the parent-provided data as our primary measure of pubertal stage and supplemented missing scores by child-reported information when available. Although the sample covered the full range of pubertal development, few children were reported to be in Stage 4 (girls: n = 34 and boys: n = 13) of pubertal development. No parents reported their child to be in pubertal Stage 5, but 4 girls and 4 boys self-reported Stage 5 of pubertal development. Because of these low numbers we combined Stage 3, 4, and 5 as Stage 3+. This recoded variable was used for all analyses, except for the quadratic mediation models. As quadratic mediation models cannot handle categorical mediators, for the quadratic mediation analysis of brain structure, the originally coded variable (pubertal stage) was used and treated continuously. Forty-one girls reported or were reported to have experienced menarche.

Magnetic resonance imaging

Measures of the amygdala–mPFC circuit

ABCD provides tabulated summary statistics of MRI data based on processing algorithms implemented by its data analytic core (Hagler et al., Reference Hagler, Hatton, Makowski, Cornejo, Fair, Dick and Dale2018). Given that both Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al. (Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013) and Thijssen et al. (Reference Thijssen, Muetzel, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Tiemeier, Verhulst and van IJzendoorn2017) report correlations between familial environment and amygdala–ACC (which is part of the mPFC) connectivity, we focused on indices of amygdala and ACC gray and white matter structure. As such, we examined amygdala volumes, ACC cortical thickness and area, and ACC white matter fractional anisotropy. Unfortunately, no direct measure of amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity has been released by ABCD to date, but amygdala functional connectivity with several well-characterized resting-state networks (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Laumann, Adeyemo, Huckins, Kelley and Petersen2016) was provided in the first data release. Here, we assessed associations between family environment and connectivity of the amygdala and the cingulo-opercular network, which includes the caudal ACC and insula and has been implicated in cognitive control (Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Fair, Miezin, Cohen, Wenger, Dosenbach and Petersen2007). This network, however, does not encompass the entire mPFC and includes regions outside of the mPFC. This measure can therefore merely be seen as a proxy of the network of direct interest. Among the available resting-state functional networks that have been analyzed for the ABCD cohort, another network that encompasses a large part of the mPFC is the default mode network. However, as this network also includes regions such as the posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus, and temporoparietal junction, and as its functionality (task negative network involved in self-referencing, theory of mind, and episodic memory; Whitfield-Gabrieli & Ford, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford2012) less well matches the function of amygdala–mPFC circuit (emotion regulation), it was not included as an outcome of interest. Comparable to the cingulo-opercular network, the salience network includes the insula and ACC. However, as the salience network only includes a small fraction of the ACC, this network was not selected.

To assess the discriminant validity of our results, we also analyzed measures of motor processing, specifically (mean left and right) precentral cortical thickness and cortical area, white matter fractional anisotropy, as well as somatomotor-mouth–amygdala connectivity. As the motor cortex is one of the first brain regions to reach peak cortical thickness/area (Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Blumenthal, Jeffries, Castellanos, Liu, Zijdenbos and Rapoport1999), we did not expect that family environment would relate to precentral gray matter via pubertal stage.

MRI acquisition

For details on MRI acquisition in the ABCD Study, please see Casey et al. (Reference Casey, Cannonier, Conley, Cohen, Barch, Heitzeg and Dale2018). Across all sites, participants were familiarized to the MRI environment using a mock scanner. A 2-hr MRI scanning session was performed on different 3T scanners from Siemens (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany), Philips (Philips Medical Systems, Best, the Netherlands), as well as GE (General Electric, Milwaukee, MI, USA). A T1-weighted anatomical scan was acquired as follows: Siemens scanners: resonance time (TR) = 2500 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.88 ms, flip angle = 8 degrees, 176 transverse slices, voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm; Philips scanners: TR = 6.31 ms, TE = 2.9 ms, flip angle = 8 degrees, 255 transverse slices, voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm; GE scanners: TR = 2500 ms, TE = 2.0 ms, flip angle = 8 degrees, 208 transverse slices, voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm.

Diffusion tensor imaging data were acquired using a multiband, echo-planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: TR = 4100 ms Siemens and GE/5300 ms Philips, TE = 88 ms Siemens/81 ms Philips/89 ms GE, flip angle = 90 degrees Siemens/78 degrees Philips/ 77 degrees GE, matrix = 140 × 140, field of view = 240 mm × 240 mm, slice thickness = 1.7 mm, number of slices = 81, 96 diffusion directions, acquisition time = 7 min 31 s Siemens/9 min 14 s Philips/7 min 30 s GE.

The resting-state fMRI sequence utilized the same gradient-echo blood oxygen level dependent echo-planar imaging sequence for each scanner, with TR = 800 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 52 degrees, 60 transverse slices, multiband acceleration = 6, and voxel resolution of 2.4 × 2.4 × 2.4 mm. Three to four runs of 5 min were acquired. The first two runs as well as Run 3 and 4 were separated by a 10-s film clip. The second and the third run were separated by a diffusion scan.

Preprocessing and MRI data analysis

MRI preprocessing and analyses were performed by the ABCD consortium's data analytic core. Please see Hagler et al. (Reference Hagler, Hatton, Makowski, Cornejo, Fair, Dick and Dale2018) for detailed information.

T1-weighted images were corrected for gradient nonlinearity distortions (Jovicich et al., Reference Jovicich, Czanner, Greve, Haley, Van Der Kouwe, Gollub and Dale2006). T2-weighted images were registered to T1-weighted images using mutual information (Wells, Viola, Atsumi, Nakajima, & Kikinis, Reference Wells, Viola, Atsumi, Nakajima and Kikinis1996). Intensity nonuniformity correction was performed based on tissue segmentation and sparse spatial smoothing. Data were resampled with 1 mm isotropic voxels into rigid alignment with an atlas brain. Cortical surface reconstruction was performed using FreeSurfer v5.3.0 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). Briefly, FreeSurfer was used for skull stripping (Ségonne et al., Reference Ségonne, Dale, Busa, Glessner, Salat, Hahn and Fischl2004), white matter segmentation, initial mesh creation (Dale, Fischl, & Sereno, Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999), correction of topological defects (Fischl, Liu, & Dale, Reference Fischl, Liu and Dale2001; Ségonne, Pacheco, & Fischl, Reference Ségonne, Pacheco and Fischl2007), surface optimization (Dale et al., Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999; Dale & Sereno, Reference Dale and Sereno1993; Fischl & Dale, Reference Fischl and Dale2000), and nonlinear registration to a spherical surface-based atlas (Fischl, Sereno, Tootell, & Dale, Reference Fischl, Sereno, Tootell and Dale1999). Subcortical structures were labeled with atlas-based segmentation (Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Salat, Busa, Albert, Dieterich, Haselgrove and Dale2002).

As with the T1-weighted images, diffusion images were corrected for gradient nonlinearity distortions (Jovicich et al., Reference Jovicich, Czanner, Greve, Haley, Van Der Kouwe, Gollub and Dale2006). Diffusion data were corrected for eddy current distortion through an iterative model-based approach that used diffusion gradient orientations and amplitudes to predict the pattern of distortions across the entire set of diffusion-weighted volumes in terms of translation, scaling, and shear along the phase-encode direction (Zhuang et al., Reference Zhuang, Hrabe, Kangarlu, Xu, Bansal, Branch and Peterson2006). During each iteration, a robust tensor fit was calculated in which data frames with high residual error were excluded from the linear estimation of tensor model parameters. Outlier data frames (e.g., slices showing signal dropout due to sudden head movements) were replaced with values estimated from the tensor fit based on the censored data. More subtle forms of head motion were corrected using rigid body registration of each data frame with the corresponding eddy current-corrected volume (Hagler et al., Reference Hagler, Ahmadi, Kuperman, Holland, McDonald, Halgren and Dale2009). The diffusion gradient matrix was adjusted for head rotation (Hagler et al., Reference Hagler, Ahmadi, Kuperman, Holland, McDonald, Halgren and Dale2009; Leemans & Jones, Reference Leemans and Jones2009), and mean head motion values were calculated to correct for residual motion effects in group statistical analyses.

Spatial and intensity distortions caused by B0 field inhomogeneity were reduced using the reversing gradient method (Holland, Kuoperman, & Dale, Reference Holland, Kuperman and Dale2010). Pairs of b = 0 (non-diffusion weighted) images with opposite phase encoding polarities were aligned using a nonlinear registration procedure, and the estimated displacement field volume was used to correct distortions in each successive diffusion-weighted volume. To use anatomical regions of interest (ROIs) from FreeSurfer's automated subcortical segmentation and cortical parcellation, b = 0 images were registered to T1-weighted structural images using mutual information (Wells et al., Reference Wells, Viola, Atsumi, Nakajima and Kikinis1996) after coarse prealignment via within-modality registration to atlas brains. Diffusion images were resampled into a standard orientation with 1.7-mm isotropic voxel resolution. Standard-space registration was combined with the motion-correction registration, so that a single resampling step could be performed using cubic interpolation.

Several standard measures related to microstructural tissue properties were calculated after fitting the diffusion tensor, including fractional anisotropy, and mean, longitudinal (or axial), and transverse diffusivity. B values greater than 1,000 were excluded from tensor fitting to avoid need for nonlinear estimation, and diffusion tensor parameters were calculated using a linear estimation approach with log-transformed diffusion-weighted signals (Basser, Mattiello, & LeBihan, Reference Basser, Mattiello and LeBihan1994; Le Bihan et al., Reference Le Bihan, Mangin, Poupon, Clark, Pappata, Molko and Chabriat2001; Pierpaoli, Jezzard, Basser, Barnett, & Di Chiro, Reference Pierpaoli, Jezzard, Basser, Barnett and Di Chiro1996).

Mean DTI measures were calculated for ROIs derived from FreeSurfer's automated segmentation and parcelation. To minimize partial volume effects, mean DTI measures for cortical ROIs were weighted based on the proportion of white versus gray matter within each voxel in an ROI, using information from the cortical surfaces generated by FreeSurfer during processing of T1-weighted images. A similar method was used to calculate weighted mean diffusivity values for subcortical ROIs, to minimize signal contamination due to cerebrospinal fluid partial voluming within voxels (Elman et al., Reference Elman, Panizzon, Hagler, Fennema-Notestine, Eyler and Kremen2017).

Resting-state fMRI data were head motion corrected by registering each frame to the first using AFNI's 3dvolreg (Cox, Reference Cox1996). B0 distortions were corrected using the reversing gradient method (Holland et al., Reference Holland, Kuperman and Dale2010). Displacement field was estimated from spin-echo field map scans, then adjusted for estimated between-scan head motion, and applied to gradient-echo images. Data were corrected for gradient nonlinearity distortions (Jovicich et al., Reference Jovicich, Czanner, Greve, Haley, Van Der Kouwe, Gollub and Dale2006). Finally, between-scan motion correction was performed across all fMRI scans in imaging sessions. Registration was performed between T2-weighted, spin-echo B0 calibration scans and T1-weighted structural images using mutual information (Wells et al., Reference Wells, Viola, Atsumi, Nakajima and Kikinis1996).

Following initial preprocessing, initial volumes were removed from the scan (Siemens/Philips: 8 TRs, GE DV25: 5 TRs, GE DV26: 16 TRs). Data were normalized and demeaned. Then, linear regression was performed to remove quadratic trends and signals correlated with motion and mean time courses of cerebral white matter, ventricles, and whole brain, plus first derivatives (Power et al., Reference Power, Mitra, Laumann, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2014; Satterthwaite et al., Reference Satterthwaite, Wolf, Loughead, Ruparel, Elliott, Hakonarson and Gur2012). Motion regression included six parameters plus their derivatives and squares. Frames with displacement >0.3 mm were excluded from the regression (Power et al., Reference Power, Mitra, Laumann, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2014). Data were band-pass filtered between 0.009 and 0.08 Hz (Hallquist, Hwang, & Luna, Reference Hallquist, Hwang and Luna2013).

Preprocessed time courses were sampled onto the cortical surface projecting 1 mm into cortical gray matter along surface normal vector. Motion censoring was performed to reduce residual effects of head motion (Power et al., Reference Power, Mitra, Laumann, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2014; Power, Barnes, Snyder, Schlaggar, & Petersen, Reference Power, Barnes, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2012). Motion estimates were filtered to attenuate signals (0.31–0.43 Hz) associated with respiration (18.6–25.7 respirations / minute). Time points with framewise displacement (FD) > 0.2 mm were excluded from variance and correlation calculations as well as time periods with <5 contiguous, subthreshold time points and time points that were outliers in SD across ROIs. Subcortical structures were labeled with atlas-based FreeSurfer segmentation (Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Salat, Busa, Albert, Dieterich, Haselgrove and Dale2002). Networks were defined as predefined groups of parcels stemming from functionally defined parcelation based on resting-state functional connectivity patterns (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Laumann, Adeyemo, Huckins, Kelley and Petersen2016). A seed-based, correlational approach (Van Dijk et al., Reference Van Dijk, Hedden, Venkataraman, Evans, Lazar and Buckner2010) adapted for cortical surface based analysis (Seibert & Brewer, Reference Seibert and Brewer2011) was performed. The average correlation between networks and ROIs is calculated as the average of the Fisher-transformed correlations.

The present study made use of tabulated data sheets resulting from these analyses; these were part of the first wave of released ABCD data. For all ACC modalities, because we had no hypotheses related to laterality effects, left and right, rostral and caudal ACC measures were averaged to create a summary ACC score. To create an average amygdala volume score, right and left amygdala volumes were averaged.

Quality assurance

FreeSurfer-processed structural data were available on 3,048 participants. We excluded 553 participants whose data sets were rated by the ABCD consortium as moderately or severely impacted by motion, intensity inhomogeneity, white matter underestimation, pial overestimation, or magnetic susceptibility artifact. Structural MRI analyses were performed on 2,495 participants.

Processed DTI data were available on 2,787 participants. We excluded 759 participants with more than 1.50 mm average framewise displacement, resulting in a total sample of 2,028 participants.

For the resting-state analyses, fully processed data were available on 2,727 participants. We further excluded 271 participants with a mean FD of >0.55 mm as well as 50 participants who had less than 4 min of resting-state data with FD < 0.20 mm and no fieldmap data collected within two scans prior to the resting-state scan, resulting in a sample of 2,164 for the resting-state fMRI analyses (Parkes, Fulcher, Yücel, & Fornito, Reference Parkes, Fulcher, Yücel and Fornito2018).

Statistical analysis

Partial correlations were used to assess associations between the latent variables and child behavior scores correcting for child and parent sex, and child age. These statistics are meant as descriptors of the data, rather than tests of hypotheses. Therefore, no indices of inferential significance are reported. To test the hypothesized indirect effect of family environment on structure and function of the amygdala–mPFC circuit via pubertal stage, linear mediation analyses were performed in Mplus. Mediation analyses were corrected for child sex, age, and race. Brain measures were residualized for study site (dummy coded). Gray matter measures were additionally residualized for total brain volume (sum of left and right cortical volume and white matter volume). Due to the large scale of the cortical area and subcortical volume measures, these measures were converted to z scores. Separate models were run for each modality of the same structure (T1 ACC, T1 amygdala, DTI, and resting-state fMRI). Models were initially run on the full sample. To assess sex differences, we performed exploratory mediation analyses separately for boys and girls.

Because gray matter development follows an inverted U-shaped developmental trajectory with peaks of gray matter in adolescence (e.g., Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Blumenthal, Jeffries, Castellanos, Liu, Zijdenbos and Rapoport1999), we visualized the distribution of the gray matter metrics across pubertal stages (see Supplemental Figure S.2). Because the distribution of ACC cortical area and amygdala volume suggests that there could be a quadratic relation with pubertal stage, for these outcomes, quadratic mediation was also tested (quadratic association between mediator pubertal stage and gray matter outcome) using the Medcurve macro in SPSS (Hayes & Preacher, Reference Hayes and Preacher2010). Whereas linear mediation analyses in Mplus can analyze categorical mediators, quadratic mediation in Medcurve can only handle continuous mediators. Therefore, for the quadratic mediation analyses, pubertal stage was regarded as a continuous variable and the original coding (Stage 1 through 4) was used. Contrary to linear mediation, where the indirect effect is similar for any level of the independent variable, in quadratic meditation, the effect size of the indirect effect depends on the level of the independent variable (Hayes & Preacher, Reference Hayes and Preacher2010). Instantaneous indirect effects were assessed for the mean score and +/–1 SD. Bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals (10,000 samples) were calculated to allow statistical inference.

Results

Tables 1 and 2 provide sample characteristics for the sample used to create the family environment score (i.e., including individuals with poor-quality MRI data). As expected, there were significant differences in pubertal development between boys and girls, with more girls in higher stages compared to boys, χ2 (2) = 721.42, p < .001. Boys (M = –0.101, SD = 0.58) had a lower family environment score (e.g., lower quality environment) compared to girls (M = 0.00, SD = 0.56), t (3181) = 4.95, p < .001. Table 3 shows the correlations between the different latent variables (LVs) as well as the correlations between the LVs and the child behavioral measures. As can be seen, the overarching latent measure of family environment correlates similarly with all subordinate LVs (r = .76, r = .80, and r = .67 for the child-reported, parent-reported, and demographic LV, respectively). Contrary to the parent- and child-reported LVs, which show stronger correlations with the same reporter behavioral outcomes, the family environment LV shows a stable pattern of correlations with both parent- and child-reported child behavior measures.

Table 1. Sample characteristics; continuous variables

Note: LV, latent variable. CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist. SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.

Table 2. Sample characteristics; categorical variables

Table 3. Partial correlations between latent variables (LVs) and child behavior measures

Note: Correlations were corrected for child age and sex, and parent sex. FE, family environment latent variable. Child LV, latent variable representing child-reported data on parenting and family relationships. Parent LV, latent variable representing parent-reported data on family relationships. Demographics, latent variable representing family demographical information. CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist. Int, internalizing behavior. Ext, externalizing behavior. SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.

Correlations between the outcomes of interest can be found in Table 4. All measures were residualized for scanning site (dummy coded) prior to calculating correlations. Gray matter measures were additionally residualized for total brain volume. See Supplemental Table S.1 for correlations between the motor-processing brain measures. For amygdala volume, ACC cortical thickness and cortical area, sex-corrected age correlations were r = .028, p = .167, r = –.124, p < .001, and r = .036, p = .072, respectively. Correlation between cingulo-opercular network–left and right amygdala functional connectivity and age (corrected for sex) were partial r = .007, p = .724, and partial r = .019, p = .349, respectively. Finally, for ACC fractional anisotropy, the sex-corrected correlation with age was partial r = .041, p = .062.

Table 4. Correlation between MRI measures

Note: All measures are residualized for data collection site. Gray matter measures were further residualized for total brain volume. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex. CT, cortical thickness. CA, cortical area. FA, fractional anisotropy. SV, subcortical volume. CON, cingulo-opercular network. L, left. R, right. FC, functional connectivity.

Mediation analyses

The results of the linear mediation analyses are presented in Figures 1 (total sample) and 2 (sex-stratified analyses). The full model details for the analyses of the amygdala–mPFC circuit can be found in Tables 5–7. In these tables, the top half describes the effect of predictors (family environment and pubertal stage) and confounding variables on the brain metric of interest. This part of the table thus provides information on the direct effect of family environment on brain structure or functioning as well as on the second leg of the indirect effect (pubertal stage on brain structure or function). The second half of these tables describes the effect of family environment and confounding variables on pubertal stage. As such, this part of the table provides information on the first leg of the indirect effect. For full model details of the quadratic mediation analyses, please see Supplemental Table S.2. For a description of the results of the motor circuit processing analyses, please see Supplemental Text 1 and Supplemental Tables S.3–S.6. For full-model estimates of the sex-stratified analyses, please see Supplemental Tables S.7–S.21.

Figure 1. Associations between family environment and amygdala–mPFC measures are mediated by pubertal stage. Values are standardized coefficients. *p < .01. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Fam Env, family environment. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex. CT, cortical thickness. CA, cortical area. SV, subcortical volume. FA, fractional anisotropy. CON-Am FC, cingulu-opercular network–amygdala functional connectivity.

Figure 2. Mediation of association between family environment and amygdala–mPFC measures by pubertal stage for (a) girls and (b) boys. Values are standardized coefficients. *p < .01. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Fam Env, family environment. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex. CT, cortical thickness. CA, cortical area. SV, subcortical volume. FA, fractional anisotropy. CON-Am FC, cingulu-opercular network–amygdala functional connectivity.

Table 5. Mediation model parameters: Anterior cingulate cortical thickness and area

Note: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex. Fam Env, family environment. aDirect effect. bIndirect effect Step 2. cIndirect effect Step 1.

Table 6. Mediation model parameters: Anterior cingulate white matter fractional anisotropy

aDirect effect. bIndirect effect Step 2. cIndirect effect Step 1.

Table 7. Mediation model parameters: Cinculo-opercular network–amygdala connectivity

Note: CON, cingulo-opercular network. aDirect effect. bIndirect effect Step 2. cIndirect effect Step 1.

Gray matter structure

Amygdala volume

For amygdalae volume, the total, direct (effect controlled for pubertal stage), and indirect effects (effect via pubertal stage) of family environment were not significant, β = 0.029, p = .147, β = 0.031, p = .135, β = –0.002, p = .707, nor was there evidence for quadratic mediation. The quadratic unstandardized association between pubertal stage and amygdala volume was not significant (b = –0.039, p = .268).

Cortical thickness

The indirect effect of family environment on ACC cortical thickness through pubertal stage was significant, β = 0.014, p = .007, but the total and direct effects were not, β = 0.012, p = .552, β = –0.002, p = .916, respectively. Thus, a lower quality family environment was related to a thinner ACC, but only through its association with a more advanced pubertal stage.

Cortical area

No significant linear associations were found for ACC cortical area (β = –0.004, p = .843, β = –0.004, p = .872, β = –0.001, p = .885, for the total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively). However, the quadratic mediation model for ACC cortical area did provide some evidence for mediation. There was a negative quadratic association between pubertal stage and ACC cortical area (b = –0.098, p = .006), corresponding with the inverted U-shaped developmental trajectories previously reported. However, none of the instantaneous indirect effects were significant, b = –0.005, 95% CI [–0.015, 0.004], b = –0.007, 95% CI [0.019, 0.002], b = –0.010, 95% CI [–0.023, 0.001], for –1 SD, mean, and +1 SD, respectively. As the indirect effect seems to increase with increasing family environment score, these results suggest that for children from very low-stress family environments only, ACC cortical area is decreased through its association with a less advanced pubertal stage.

Sex-specific effects

The exploratory analyses stratified by sex suggest that the significant ACC cortical thickness association was driven mainly by the girls in the sample. In girls, the indirect effect of family environment through pubertal stage was significant, β = 0.0.20, p = .013, but total and direct effects were not, β = –0.006, p = .840, β = –0.026, p = .402. For boys, the total, direct, and indirect effects were not significant (β = 0.006, p = .366, β = 0.004, p = .533, β = 0.002, p = .181, respectively).

Similar to the analyses in the total sample, the stratified linear mediation analyses of ACC cortical area did not provide any significant results (ACC cortical area: girls: β = –0.028, p = .354, β = –0.028, p = .358, β = 0.000, p = .998, for total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively; boys β = –0.001, p = .987, β = 0.002, p = .970, β = –0.003, p = .755, for total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively). In girls, the quadratic association between pubertal stage and ACC cortical area was significant, b = –0.131, p = .004. However, again no significant instantaneous indirect effects were found, b = 0.013, 95% CI [–0.002, 0.034], b = 0.006, 95% CI [–0.009, 0.024], b = –0.005, 95% CI [–0.019, 0.015], for –1 SD, mean, and +1 SD, respectively. In boys, no evidence for quadratic mediation was found (quadratic association between pubertal stage and ACC cortical area: b = –0.027, p = .676).

The stratified linear analyses on amygdala volume were not significant (girls: β = –0.044, p = .393, β = –0.034, p = .546, β = –0.10, p = .586, for total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively; boys β = 0.082, p = .088, β = 0.077, p = .097, β = 0.005, p = .660, for total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively). The quadratic mediation model provided no evidence for quadratic mediation in girls (quadratic association between pubertal stage and amygdala volume: b = –0.053, p = .215) or boys (quadratic association between pubertal stage and amygdala volume: b = –0.048, p = .741).

White matter integrity: Fractional anisotropy

In the total sample, the total, direct, and indirect effects through pubertal stage of family environment on ACC fractional anisotropy were significant β = –0.066, p = .003, β = –0.054, p = .020, β = –0.013, p = .015, respectively. Thus, a lower quality family environment related to higher ACC fractional anisotropy. This association was partly mediated by a higher pubertal stage.

The stratified analyses suggest that for girls, the family environment associates with ACC fractional anisotropy only through its association with pubertal stage (β = –0.058, p = .107, β = –0.038, p = .298, β = –0.020, p = .009, for total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively), whereas for boys, only the direct effect of family environment on ACC fractional anisotropy was significant (β = –0.076, p = .005, β = –0.077, p = .007, β = 0.001, p = .868, for total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively).

Resting-state fMRI

In the total sample, the total, direct, and indirect effects of family environment on cingulo-opercular network–left amygdala functional connectivity were β = 0.070, p = .001, β = 0.062, p = .005, β = 0.008, p = .055, respectively. For cingulo-opercular network–right amygdala functional connectivity, the total, direct, and indirect effects were β = 0.038, p = .086, β = 0.030, p = .181, β = 0.007, p = .080, respectively. Thus, family environment was positively associated with cingulo-opercular network–amygdala functional connectivity. For both left and right amygdala– cingulo-opercular network functional connectivity, the indirect effect of family environment on functional connectivity via pubertal stage indicated a trend in the expected direction.

The exploratory analyses stratified by sex suggest that the total and direct effects of family environment on cingulo-opercular network–left amygdala functional connectivity were significant for girls, whereas a trend was found for the indirect effect (β = 0.101, p = .001, β = 0.090, p = .004, β = 0.011, p = .064, respectively). For boys, no significant effects were found (β = 0.044, p = .133, β = 0.039, p = .198, β = 0.005, p = .37, respectively). For cingulo-opercular network–right amygdala functional connectivity, no significant effects were found for girls nor boys (girls: β = 0.059, p = .076, β = 0.121, p = .099, β = 0.006, p = .306, for total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively; boys β = 0.020, p = .518, β = 0.011, p = .738, β = 0.009, p = .151, for total, direct, and indirect effects, respectively).

Motor-processing areas

No significant indirect effects were found for cortical structure, white matter integrity, or functional connectivity of the motor circuit. Only a significant direct and total effect of family environment on functional connectivity between the left amygdala and somatomotor–mouth network were found, β = 0.061, p = .003, β = 0.062, p = .003, respectively.

Discussion

The present study examined whether associations between the quality of a child's family environment and measures of the amygdala–mPFC circuit's structure and function were mediated by pubertal development. For ACC cortical thickness, cortical area, and fractional anisotropy, evidence was found that a more stressful family environment relates to brain structure through accelerated pubertal development. For amygdala–cingulo-opercular network resting-state functional connectivity, results indicated a trend in the expected direction. There were no direct or indirect associations between family environment and amygdala volume. Original studies reporting accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit focused on functional MRI data only. Our findings provide suggestive evidence from a population-based sample that accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit in response to family-related stress may not be limited to brain activation (similar relations are found for mPFC [ACC] gray and white matter structure) and may be (in part) the consequence of accelerated pubertal development.

Cortical gray matter follows an inverted U-shaped developmental trajectory: during childhood, gray matter increases until it peaks in adolescence and declines thereafter (e.g., Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Blumenthal, Jeffries, Castellanos, Liu, Zijdenbos and Rapoport1999). White matter has been shown to increase over age until at least the late 20s (Westlye et al., Reference Westlye, Walhovd, Dale, Bjørnerud, Due-Tønnessen, Engvig and Fjell2010). Our results suggest that a suboptimal family environment relates to higher ACC white matter integrity, which can be seen as a more mature state of brain development given the age range of the current sample. This association was mediated by a more advanced pubertal stage. Similarly, through a more advanced pubertal stage, a suboptimal family environment related to a thinner cortex, which—as gray matter decreases over pubertal development—can be seen as a more mature state. For ACC cortical area, the indirect effect is more complicated due to its quadratic nature, but coheres with the hypothesized model. As such, a suboptimal family environment may accelerate ACC gray and white matter development.

Although a large literature exists on the association between a child´s familial context and pubertal development, to our knowledge, no studies have examined the consequences of these findings for individual differences in patterns of brain structure and function. Similarly, accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit has not been ascribed to accelerated pubertal development, but is regarded as a direct effect of the environment or stress on this circuit (Callaghan & Tottenham, Reference Callaghan and Tottenham2016; Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013). In rats, early life stress (through increasing cortisol) has been associated with precocious engagement of the amygdala in response to odor-shock conditioning (Moriceau & Sullivan, Reference Moriceau and Sullivan2006). In their study on accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit in previously institutionalized children, Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al. (Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013) show that the association between institutionalized care and accelerated development of amygdala–mPFC circuit is mediated by cortisol levels. The authors suggest that the accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit may be a result of early life increased cortisol levels resulting in earlier amygdala hyperactivity, which in its turn may facilitate development of connections with the mPFC. Further, in rodents and primates, the amygdala has been suggested to play a role in regulating the onset of puberty (Adekunbi et al., Reference Adekunbi, Li, Li, Adegoke, Iranloye, Morakinyo and Byrne2017; Li et al., Reference Li, Hu, Hanley, Lin, Poston, Lightman and O'Byrne2015; Stephens, Raper, Bachevalier, & Wallen, Reference Stephens, Raper, Bachevalier and Wallen2015). This particular role of the amygdala bridges the theory that the precocious development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit is a consequence of stress (cortisol) affecting the amygdala and the theory presented here, that accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit is (in part) a consequence of accelerated pubertal development.

In a previously institutionalized sample, accelerated neural development was reported in children aged 6.5–10.4 years (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013). Given the age of the sample, one might argue that pubertal development is an unlikely driving force of such development. However, adrenarche, which is considered the first stage of pubertal development, typically occurs around 6–8 years of age (Campbell, Reference Campbell2006; McClintock & Herdt, Reference McClintock and Herdt1996), and if accelerated, may well precede the accelerated neural development reported by Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013. Adrenal hormones are continuously involved in pubertal development and affect, for example, pubic hair development and pubertal skin changes, pubertal indices measured in the Pubertal Development Scale utilized here to study pubertal stage. Adrenarche affects brain development (for a review, see Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Whittle, Vijayakumar, Dennison, Simmons and Allen2017), and although considered distinct from gonadarche, through its influence on GABA neurons adrenarche may indirectly affect gonadarche (Genazzani, Bernardi, Monteleone, Luisi & Luisi, Reference Genazzani, Bernardi, Monteleone, Luisi and Luisi2000). Like gonadarche and menarche, adrenarche has been reported to be accelerated in response to early life stress (e.g., Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Ruttle, Boyce, Armstrong and Essex2015; Ellis & Essex, Reference Ellis and Essex2007), and children experiencing early adrenarche are more likely to experience early pubarche (adrenal), menarche, and telarche (gonadal; Ibáñez, Jiménez, & de Zegher, Reference Ibáñez, Jiménez and de Zegher2006; Liimatta, Utriainen, Voutilainen, & Jääskeläinen, Reference Liimatta, Utriainen, Voutilainen and Jääskeläinen2017; Pereira, Iñiguez, Corvalan, & Mericq, Reference Pereira, Iñiguez, Corvalan and Mericq2017). The synthesis and release of both cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA), a hormonal index of adrenarcheal development, are triggered by the same hormone, ACTH. Moreover, DHEA plays a role in the stress system (Saczawa, Graber, Brooks-Gunn, & Warren, Reference Saczawa, Graber, Brooks-Gunn and Warren2013), providing a direct biological link between stress and adrenal hormones. As such, adrenarche is an early marker of pubertal onset that predates the release of gonadal hormones. We speculate that it may play a role in the accelerated neural development that has been observed across studies in response to early stress. Of course, the family environment could affect brain development via multiple pathways: cortisol could directly affect the amygdala–mPFC circuit as previously suggested, and simultaneously have a similar effect through its association with pubertal development and hormones (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, McFadyen-Ketchum, Dodge, Pettit and Bates1999; Graberc et al., Reference Graberc, Brooks-Gunn and Warren1995; Romans et al., Reference Romans, Martin, Gendall and Herbison2003). This hypothesized mechanism of action merits further scrutiny given that there is inconsistent evidence for accelerated pubertal development in previously institutionalized girls.

When the quality of parental care and the familial environment is low, it may (from an evolutionary standpoint) be adaptive to reach a reproductive age sooner. Similarly, when parental care is limited, it may be beneficial to develop some form of self-regulation skills sooner instead of relying on parents for emotion regulation. In addition, the accelerated development of emotion regulation skills can be regarded in light of evolutionary adaptiveness: an individual capable of regulating his or her emotions may be better equipped to find a partner. Nevertheless, although accelerated development may be beneficial in the short run, a long childhood period has reported benefits for development. For example, superior intelligence has been associated with prolonged cortical thickness growth in children (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Greenstein, Lerch, Clasen, Lenroot, Gogtay and Giedd2006). The earlier termination of childhood may result in suboptimal development (e.g., a higher risk for health and adjustment problems or psychopathology later in life; Kelsey, Gammon, & John, Reference Kelsey, Gammon and John1993; Winer, Powers, & Pietromonaco, Reference Winer, Powers and Pietromonaco2017).

Contrary to psychosocial acceleration theory, child development theory (Ellis, Reference Ellis2004) suggests that information from the environment is used to coordinate the length of the childhood period without focusing on reproduction. This theory poses that, if the environment is positive, the child can take its time to reap the benefits of a long childhood. When the family environment is suboptimal, earlier independence from the parents may be preferred and the child accelerates his or her pubertal development. This, however, does not mean that the child will also engage in sexual behavior and reproduction at an earlier age. Our results for ACC cortical area are more in line with this latter theory, as they suggest that pubertal and brain development is delayed in children from low-stress families, rather than accelerated in high-stress families. However, despite including the full range of pubertal development, our sample includes relatively few children in pubertal Stages 4 and 5. In order to draw strong conclusions regarding quadratic mediation, a more equal distribution of pubertal stage should be evident. Nevertheless, our results suggest the possibility of quadratic mediation, which may provide a worthwhile avenue to explore as this sample develops further.

The associations between family environment or pubertal stage and amygdala–cingulo-opercular network functional connectivity were not significant but indicated a trend in the expected direction. Although our hypotheses were derived from work examining task-based functional connectivity, here, the functional connectivity data provided the least compelling results. Because ABCD did not release amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity metrics, we selected amygdala–cingulo-opercular functional connectivity as the best available proxy for amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity. However, the cingulo-opercular network only includes part of the mPFC as well as regions outside of the mPFC, and these characteristics may be critical in explaining why our results are less strong than previously reported findings (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013; Thijssen et al., Reference Thijssen, Muetzel, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Tiemeier, Verhulst and van IJzendoorn2017). In addition, no associations between family environment or pubertal stage and amygdala volume were found. Although some studies report continued amygdala structural development through childhood (Goddings & Giedd, Reference Goddings and Giedd2014; Neufang et al., Reference Neufang, Specht, Hausmann, Güntürkün, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Fink and Konrad2009), other studies suggest that the amygdala matures prior to adolescence (Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Vaituzis, Hamburger, Lange, Rajapakse, Kaysen and Rapoport1996), which would explain why no associations with pubertal stage were found in the present study. Moreover, although evidence for changes in amygdala volume in relation to early adversity has been consistently reported (Mehta et al., Reference Mehta, Golembo, Nosarti, Colvert, Mota, Williams and Sonuga-Barke2009; Tottenham et al., Reference Tottenham, Hare, Quinn, McCarry, Nurse, Gilhooly and Casey2010), studies on normal variation in family environment and amygdala volume have provided mixed results (Kok et al., Reference Kok, Thijssen, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Verhulst, White and Tiemeier2015; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Simmons, Dennison, Vijayakumar, Schwartz, Yap and Allen2014). Alternatively, T1 MRI measures cortical thickness and surface area much more precisely than the volumes of subcortical structures. Thus, the null effect for amygdala volume might simply reflect lesser precision of MRI measurement, as compared to cortical surface measures. The mediation models of motor processing largely provided nonsignificant results, except for a direct effect of the child's family environment on left amygdala–somatomotor-mouth network functional connectivity. As the motor cortex is one of the first brain regions to reach peak cortical thickness/area (Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Blumenthal, Jeffries, Castellanos, Liu, Zijdenbos and Rapoport1999), we did not expect mediation by pubertal stage in these regions, providing some evidence for the specificity of our results.

Similar to previous literature, our results suggest that girls initiate puberty earlier than boys (Petersen & Crockett, Reference Petersen and Crockett1985), and that Caucasian children are later to initiate puberty than children from other races (Butts & Seifer, Reference Butts and Seifer2010). Our exploratory analyses stratified by sex suggest that direct and indirect associations between family environment and the amygdala–mPFC circuit were more prominent for girls than for boys. These findings are in line with studies supporting psychosocial acceleration theory, which focuses on girls rather than boys. Similarly, the exploratory analyses of Thijssen et al. (Reference Thijssen, Muetzel, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Tiemeier, Verhulst and van IJzendoorn2017) suggest that acceleration of functional development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit may be present only in girls. Because females are physically more restricted in the number of offspring they can conceive relative to males, it makes evolutionary sense that girls specifically mature sooner. Moreover, rodent studies suggest that the reorganizing effects of puberty are smaller in males than in females, which may explain our sex-stratified results (Juraska & Willing, Reference Juraska and Willing2017). Alternatively, as pubertal development is slower in boys than in girls, it is possible that associations between pubertal stage and brain development in boys simply are not yet captured in our sample but will appear at a later age. Nevertheless, in girls, precocious puberty has been associated with increased depression and anxiety later in adolescence (Mendle, Turkheimer, & Emery, Reference Mendle, Turkheimer and Emery2007; Wichstrom, Reference Wichstrom2000). As the amygdala–mPFC circuit has been implicated in emotional problems (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Lee, Hollinshead, Bakst, Roffman, Smoller and Buckner2012), the co-occurrence of accelerated pubertal and amygdala–mPFC circuit development in girls specifically may be able to account for these findings.

Research in the medical field suggests that stressful environments may accelerate aging through telomere length. Telomeres are the protective caps at the end of chromosomes that erode with every cell division until a state of senescence has been reached (López-Otín, Blasco, Partridge, Serrano, & Kroemer, Reference López-Otín, Blasco, Partridge, Serrano and Kroemer2013). Accelerated telomere erosion can thus be seen as accelerated aging. Initial studies focused on adults and elderly (Epel et al., Reference Epel, Blackburn, Lin, Dhabhar, Adler, Morrow and Cawthon2004; Tomiyama et al., Reference Tomiyama, O'Donovan, Lin, Puterman, Lazaro, Chan and Epel2012), but even in children and infants, studies suggest effects of stress on telomere erosion (Drury et al., Reference Drury, Theall, Gleason, Smyke, De Vivo, Wong and Nelson2012; Entringer et al., Reference Entringer, Epel, Lin, Buss, Shahbaba, Blackburn and Wadhwa2013; Shalev et al., Reference Shalev, Moffitt, Sugden, Williams, Houts, Danese and Caspi2013). Similar to accelerated pubertal development, the pathway from early adversity to accelerated telomere erosion may include hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activation and epigenetics (Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Epel, Adler, Bush, Obradović, Lin and Boyce2011; Tomiyama et al., Reference Tomiyama, O'Donovan, Lin, Puterman, Lazaro, Chan and Epel2012), but also associations with inflammation, oxidative stress, and mitochondrial regulation have been reported (Cai et al., Reference Cai, Chang, Li, Li, Hu, Liang and Flint2015; Epel et al., Reference Epel, Blackburn, Lin, Dhabhar, Adler, Morrow and Cawthon2004; Jurk et al., Reference Jurk, Wilson, Passos, Oakley, Correia-Melo, Greaves and Von Zglinicki2014; Tyrka et al., Reference Tyrka, Parade, Price, Kao, Porton, Philip and Carpenter2016). From a medical perspective, results of accelerated telomere erosion have been interpreted as the consequence of wear and tear on biological systems caused by stress. Belsky and Shalev (Reference Belsky and Shalev2016) argue that this line of work on accelerated telomere erosion and the line of work on accelerated pubertal development may be related, and makes a case for interpreting telomere erosion from an evolutionary rather than a medical perspective (i.e., the organism adaptively trades-off longer term health and longevity for increased probability of reproducing before dying by maturing early). The results presented here add to this line of thinking and suggest that the family environment may relate to a cascade of biological events that result in accelerated aging more generally. Future research may take an interest in studying associations between family adversity, telomere erosion, and brain development as well as in examining the role of processes such as epigenetics and inflammation in the association between family environment and brain development.

Besides the hypothesized indirect effects, some potentially interesting direct effects of the child's family environment on brain function and structure were found. The sex-stratified analyses suggested increased ACC fractional anisotropy in children from lower quality family environments for both sexes. However, whereas for girls only an indirect association via pubertal stage was found, for boys only a direct effect was found, possibly suggesting accelerated development unrelated to pubertal development. Alternatively, this finding could indicate that boys from lower quality family environments have higher fractional anisotropy values independent of developmental rate. In addition, a more positive family environment was directly related to increased somatomotor-mouth network–left amygdala functional connectivity. As we did not find significant associations between functional connectivity of the somatomotor-mouth network and the left amygdala and age or pubertal stage, this finding cannot be interpreted in light of accelerated or possible slowed development in response to adversity, but suggests that a child's family environment can have broad and diverse effects on child brain development while pubertal effects may be more specific.

Several limitations of the present study should be noted. Although the ABCD Study is a longitudinal cohort study, at present only data from the first data wave, on half of the final baseline sample of 11,875 children, are available. As such, the present study is a cross-sectional study, and any statements regarding developmental trajectories should be considered speculative. Similarly, as predictor, outcome, and mediator variables are reported at the same time, the data set in its current form is not ideal for examining mediation. In addition, the original study reporting accelerated neural development in previously institutionalized children reported a developmentally mature phenotype in adolescents aged 10.5–17.6 years (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013). If amygdala–mPFC development is considered complete by the time the adultlike fear processing phenotype (negative connectivity vs. positive connectivity) is evident, then the current study's sample of 9- to 10-year-old children would not be optimal to study accelerated development. However, the negative connectivity pattern observed in early adolescents may differ still from the negative pattern observed later in development (e.g., in adults). For example, Wu et al. (Reference Wu, Kujawa, Lu, Fitzgerald, Klumpp, Fitzgerald and Phan2016) report decreased amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity in response to emotional faces in adults versus adolescents. Unlike the shift from positive to negative functional connectivity reported in relation to fear processing, mPFC structural development is more linear and protracted. As such, the age of the ABCD sample does not pose a direct problem for our conceptual model. Although family environment has been shown to be relatively stable in children over time (Loeber et al., Reference Loeber, Drinkwater, Yin, Anderson, Schmidt and Crawford2000), our measure of family environment does not necessarily reflect early family functioning. This is particularly important, because previous studies on accelerated pubertal and amygdala–mPFC development report associations for early family environment (e.g., Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Flannery, Goff, Humphreys and Telzer2013; Romans et al., Reference Romans, Martin, Gendall and Herbison2003; Thijssen et al., Reference Thijssen, Muetzel, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Jaddoe, Tiemeier, Verhulst and van IJzendoorn2017). Moreover, behavioral changes associated with puberty have been shown to affect family relationships. Thus, we cannot be certain about the causal relationship of the association between family environment and pubertal development, as there is evidence supporting a bidirectional pattern of influence (family environment accelerating pubertal development and pubertal development affecting family relationships). However, because our family environment variable includes measures other than parent–child conflict such as socioeconomic status, the inverse relationship (i.e., pubertal development affecting the family environment score) may be less salient. Thus, although it does not capture all possible features of family function, our measure of family environment presents an important predictor of pubertal and neural development. Moreover, as it is our hypothesis that family-related stress accelerates development, our family environment variable combines different factors including family dynamics and socioeconomic factors. However, this could also be considered a limitation as our approach did not test for differential effects of different types of variables. In addition, due to the population-based nature of the sample, levels of adversity are relatively low. Relatedly, due to the large sample size of the present study, small effects (such as the results presented here) may become significant. However, at the population level, even small effect sizes are potentially meaningful. Given that the pattern of results coheres across MRI modalities, confidence in the significance of our findings is increased.

In conclusion, despite the limited range in pubertal development within this sample of 9- to 10-year-olds, our results provide suggestive cross-sectional evidence that the association between family environment and accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit's structure and structural connectivity may be in part explained by accelerated pubertal development. These associations appear stronger in girls than in boys and may play a role in mental health problems observed after precocious puberty. If accelerated development of the amygdala–mPFC circuit is the consequence of an earlier onset of puberty, other brain regions and circuits that are involved in affective regulation would be expected to portray similar trajectories of accelerated development and may be related to adolescent mental health. In a recent small study, early life stress was associated with increased gray matter reductions in a variety of cortical and subcortical brain regions from ages 14 to 17 (Tyborowska et al., Reference Tyborowska, Volman, Niermann, Pouwels, Cillessen, Toni and Roelofs2018). Such associations should be further explored in future studies. Although currently only cross-sectional data are available, ABCD is a longitudinal study from age 9–10 to adulthood. Thus, once longitudinal data are available, our cross-sectional results should be replicated longitudinally. Moreover, hormonal measures could be used in future analyses to support interpretations. Although the present study provides important insight on development in relation to family-related stress, such longitudinal studies are the necessary next steps in unraveling the effects of the family environment on child brain maturation.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000580.

Funding Statement

This project was supported by Rubicon Grant 446-16-022 of the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (to S.T.) and NIDA Grant U01DA041120 (to M.L. and P.C.). Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041025, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041093, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/nih-collaborators. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/principal-investigators.html. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators.

The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from (http://dx.doi.org/10.15154/1412097).

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrook, C. (1983). Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). University Associates in Psychiatry, 53, 418435.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA Adult Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children.Google Scholar
Adekunbi, D. A., Li, X. F., Li, S., Adegoke, O. A., Iranloye, O., Morakinyo, A. O., … Byrne, K. T. O. (2017). Role of amygdala kisspeptin in pubertal timing in female rats. PLOS ONE, 12, 115. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0183596CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolfsson, S., & Westphal, O. (1981). Early pubertal development in girls adopted from Far-Eastern countries. Pediatric Research, 15, 82. doi:10.1203/00006450-198101000-00068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barch, D. M., Albaugh, M. D., Avenevoli, S., Chang, L., Clark, D. B., Glantz, M. D., … Sher, K. J. (2017). Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development Study: Rationale and description. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 5566. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basser, P. J., Mattiello, J., & LeBihan, D. (1994). MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophysical Journal, 66, 259267.10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Ruttle, P. L., Boyce, W. T., Armstrong, J. M., & Essex, M. J. (2015). Early adversity, elevated stress physiology, accelerated sexual maturation, and poor health in females. Developmental Psychology, 51, 816822. doi:10.1037/dev0000017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Shalev, I. (2016). Contextual adversity, telomere erosion, pubertal development, and health: Two models of accelerated aging, or one? Development and Psychopathology, 28, 13671383. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000900CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: A theoretical paper. Child Development, 62, 647670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bramen, J. E., Hranilovich, J. A., Dahl, R. E., Forbes, E. E., Chen, J., Toga, A. W., … Sowell, E. R. (2011). Puberty influences medial temporal lobe and cortical gray matter maturation differently in boys than girls matched for sexual maturity. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 636646. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butts, S. F., & Seifer, D. B. (2010). Racial and ethnic differences in reproductive potential across the life cycle. Fertility and Sterility, 93, 681690. doi:10.1016/j.fertnstert.2009.10.047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, M. L., Whittle, S., Vijayakumar, N., Dennison, M., Simmons, J. G., & Allen, N. B. (2017). A systematic review of adrenarche as a sensitive period in neurobiological development and mental health. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1228. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2016.12.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cai, N., Chang, S., Li, Y., Li, Q., Hu, J., Liang, J., … Flint, J. (2015). Molecular signatures of major depression. Current Biology, 25, 11461156. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callaghan, B. L., & Tottenham, N. (2016). The stress acceleration hypothesis: Effects of early-life adversity on emotion circuits and behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 7, 7681. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, B. (2006). Adrenarche and the evolution of human life history. American Journal of Human Biology, 18, 569589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Cannonier, T., Conley, M. I., Cohen, A. O., Barch, D. M., Heitzeg, M. M., … Dale, A. M. (2018). The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 4354. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chilcoat, H. D., & Anthony, J. C. (1996). Impact of parent monitoring on initiation of drug use through late childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 91100. doi:10.1097/00004583-199601000-00017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research, 29, 162173. doi:10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., & Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage, 9, 179194. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, A. M., & Sereno, M. I. (1993). Improved localizadon of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: A linear approach. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 162176. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., Cohen, A. L., Wenger, K. K., Dosenbach, R. A. T., … Petersen, S. E. (2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1107311078. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704320104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drury, S. S., Theall, K., Gleason, M. M., Smyke, A. T., De Vivo, I., Wong, J. Y. Y., … Nelson, C. A. (2012). Telomere length and early severe social deprivation: Linking early adversity and cellular aging. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 719727. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.53CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 290–258. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., & Essex, M. J. (2007). Family environments, adrenarche, and sexual maturation: A longitudinal test of a life history model. Child Development, 78, 17991817. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01092.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., & Garber, J. (2000). Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls’ pubertal timing: Maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Development, 71, 485501. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00159CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., McFadyen-Ketchum, S., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1999). Quality of early family relationships and individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: A longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 387401. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.387CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J. A., Panizzon, M. S., Hagler, D. J. Jr., Fennema-Notestine, C., Eyler, L.T., … Kremen, W. S. (2017). Genetic and environmental influences on cortical mean diffusivity. Neuroimage, 146, 9099.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Entringer, S., Epel, E. S., Lin, J., Buss, C., Shahbaba, B., Blackburn, E. H., … Wadhwa, P. D. (2013). Maternal psychosocial stress during pregnancy is associated with newborn leukocyte telomere length. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 208. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2012.11.033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epel, E. S., Blackburn, E. H., Lin, J., Dhabhar, F. S., Adler, N. E., Morrow, J. D., & Cawthon, R. M. (2004). Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 1731217315. doi:10.1073/pnas.0407162101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erman, J., & Härkönen, J. (2017). Parental separation and school performance among children of immigrant mothers in Sweden. European Journal of Population, 33, 267292. doi:10.1007/s10680-017-9419-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., & Dale, A. M. (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 97, 1105011055. doi:10.1073/pnas.200033797CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., Liu, A., & Dale, A. M. (2001). Automated manifold surgery: Constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 20, 7080. doi:10.1109/42.906426CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., … Dale, A. M. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33, 341355. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R. B. H., & Dale, A. M. (1999). High-resolution inter-subject averaging and a surface-based coordinate system. Human Brain Mapping, 8, 272284. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)83.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, C. E., & Grafman, J. (2010). The role of the human prefrontal cortex in social cognition and moral judgment. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 299324. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Gee, D. G., Humphreys, K. L., Telzer, E., … Tottenham, N. (2014). The development of human amygdala functional connectivity at rest from 4 to 23 years: A cross-sectional study. NeuroImage, 95, 193207. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garavan, H., Bartsch, H., Conway, K., Decastro, A., Goldstein, R. Z., Heeringa, S., … Zahs, D. (2018). Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 1622. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Humphreys, K. L., & Telzer, E. H. (2013). Early developmental emergence of human amygdala–prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 110, 1563815643. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307893110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G., Gabard-Durnam, L., Telzer, E. H., Humphreys, K. L., Goff, B., Shapiro, M., … Tottenham, N. (2014). Maternal buffering of human amygdala-prefrontal circuitry during childhood but not during adolescence. Psychological Science, 25, 20672078. doi:10.1177/0956797614550878CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gee, D. G., Humphreys, K. L., Flannery, J., Goff, B., Telzer, E. H., Shapiro, M., … Tottenham, N. (2013). A developmental shift from positive to negative connectivity in human amygdala–prefrontal circuitry. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 45844593. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-12.2013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genazzani, A. R., Bernardi, F., Monteleone, P., Luisi, S., & Luisi, M. (2000). Neuropeptides, neurotransmitters, neurosteroids, and the onset of puberty. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 900, 19. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06210.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., … Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861863. doi:10.1038/13158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Vaituzis, A. C., Hamburger, S. D., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Kaysen, D., … Rapoport, J. L. (1996). Quantitative MRI of the temporal lobe, amygdala, and hippocampus in normal human development: Ages 4–18 years. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 366, 223230. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960304)366:2<223::AID-CNE3>3.0.CO;2-73.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goddings, A., & Giedd, J. N. (2014). Structural brain development during childhood and adolescence. In The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1522). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goddings, A. L., Mills, K. L., Clasen, L. S., Giedd, J. N., Viner, R. M., & Blakemore, S. J. (2014). The influence of puberty on subcortical brain development. NeuroImage, 88, 242251. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.073CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R., Meltzer, H., & Bailey, V. (1998). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionaire: A pilot study on the validity of the self-report version. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 125130.10.1007/s007870050057CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Adeyemo, B., Huckins, J. F., Kelley, W. M., & Petersen, S. E. (2016). Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 288303. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu239CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graberc, J. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (1995). The antecedents of menarcheal age: Heredity, family environment, and stressful life events. Child Development, 66, 346359. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00875.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagler, D. J., Ahmadi, M. E., Kuperman, J., Holland, D., McDonald, C. R., Halgren, E., & Dale, A. M. (2009). Automated white-matter tractography using a probabilistic diffusion tensor atlas: Application to temporal lobe epilepsy. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 15351547. doi:10.1002/hbm.20619CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagler, D. J. Jr., Hatton, S. N., Makowski, C., Cornejo, M. D., Fair, D. A., Dick, A. S., … Dale, A. M. (2018). Image processing and analysis methods for the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. bioRxiv, 457739; doi:10.1101/457739Google Scholar
Hallquist, M. N., Hwang, K., & Luna, B. (2013). The nuisance of nuisance regression: Spectral misspecification in a common approach to resting-state fMRI preprocessing reintroduces noise and obscures functional connectivity. NeuroImage, 82, 208225. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D. R. (2003). Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 494501. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01786-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harold, G. T., Aitken, J. J., & Shelton, K. H. (2007). Inter-parental conflict and children's academic attainment: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 48, 12231232. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01793.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2010). Quantifying and testing indirect effects in simple mediation models when the constituent paths are nonlinear. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 45, 627660. doi:10.1080/00273171.2010.498290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herting, M. M., Gautam, P., Spielberg, J. M., & Dahl, R. E. (2015). A longitudinal study: Changes in cortical thickness and surface area during pubertal maturation. PLOS ONE, 10, 114. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0119774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hochberg, Z., & Belsky, J. (2013). Evo-devo of human adolescence: Beyond disease models of early puberty. BMC Medicine, 11, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, D., Kuperman, J. M., & Dale, A. M. (2010). Efficient correction of inhomogeneous static magnetic field-induced distortion in echo planar imaging. NeuroImage, 50, 175183. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, A. J., Lee, P. H., Hollinshead, M. O., Bakst, L., Roffman, J. L., Smoller, J. W., & Buckner, R. L. (2012). Individual differences in amygdala-medial prefrontal anatomy link negative affect, impaired social functioning, and polygenic depression risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 1808718100. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2531-12.2012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ibáñez, L., Jiménez, R., & de Zegher, F. (2006). Early puberty-menarche after precocious pubarche: Relation to prenatal growth. Pediatrics, 117, 117121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, D. E., Tang, A., Almas, A. N., Degnan, K. A., McLaughlin, K. A., Nelson, C. A., … & Drury, S.S. (2018). Caregiving disruptions affect growth and pubertal development in early adolescence in institutionalized and fostered romanian children: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Pediatrics, 203, 345353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Rodgers, B., Jacomb, P. A., & Easteal, S. (2004). Association of adverse childhood experiences, age of menarche, and adult reproductive behavior: Does the androgen receptor gene play a role? American Journal of Medical Genetics, 125B, 105111. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.20114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jovicich, J., Czanner, S., Greve, D., Haley, E., Van Der Kouwe, A., Gollub, R., … Dale, A. (2006). Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: Effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. NeuroImage, 30, 436443. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juraska, J. M., & Willing, J. (2017). Pubertal onset as a critical transition for neural development and cognition. Brain Research, 1654(Pt. B), 8794. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2016.04.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jurk, D., Wilson, C., Passos, J. F., Oakley, F., Correia-Melo, C., Greaves, L., … Von Zglinicki, T. (2014). Chronic inflammation induces telomere dysfunction and accelerates ageing in mice. Nature Communications, 5, 114. doi:10.1038/ncomms5172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., … Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children—Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 980988. doi:10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelsey, J. L., Gammon, M. D., & John, E. M. (1993). Reproductive factors and breast cancer. Epidemiologic Reviews, 15, 3647. doi:10.1016/S0378-5122(12)70032-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Avenevoli, S., Costello, E. J., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Heeringa, S., … Zaslavsky, A. M. (2009). Design and field procedures in the US National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 18, 6883. doi:10.1002/mpr.279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kok, R., Thijssen, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Verhulst, F. C., White, T., … Tiemeier, H. (2015). Normal variation in early parental sensitivity predicts child structural brain development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2015.07.009Google ScholarPubMed
Kreidl, M., Štípková, M., & Hubatková, B. (2017). Parental separation and children's education in a comparative perspective: Does the burden disappear when separation is more common? Demographic Research, 36, 73110. doi:10.4054/DemRes.2017.36.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroenke, C. H., Epel, E., Adler, N., Bush, N. R., Obradović, J., Lin, J., … Boyce, W. T. (2011). Autonomic and adrenocortical reactivity and buccal cell telomere length in kindergarten children. Psychosomatic Medicine, 73, 533540. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e318229acfcCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J. F., Poupon, C., Clark, C. A., Pappata, S., Molko, N., & Chabriat, H. (2001). Diffusion tensor imaging: Concepts and applications. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 13, 534546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leemans, A., & Jones, D. K. (2009). The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 61, 13361349. doi:10.1002/mrm.21890CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, X. F., Hu, M. H., Hanley, B. P., Lin, Y. S., Poston, L., Lightman, S. L., & O'Byrne, K. T. (2015). The posterodorsal medial amygdala regulates the timing of puberty onset in female rats. Endocrinology, 156, 37253736. doi:10.1210/en.2015-1366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liimatta, J., Utriainen, P., Voutilainen, R., & Jääskeläinen, J. (2017). Girls with a history of premature adrenarche have advanced growth and pubertal development at the age of 12 years. Frontiers in Endocrinology, 8, 291. doi:10.3389/fendo.2017.00291CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loeber, R., Drinkwater, M., Yin, Y., Anderson, S. J., Schmidt, L. C., & Crawford, A. (2000). Stability of family interaction from ages 6 to 18. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 353369. doi:10.1023/A:1005169026208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-Otín, C., Blasco, M. A., Partridge, L., Serrano, M., & Kroemer, G. (2013). The hallmarks of aging. Cell, 153, 11941217. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, L. H., Dapretto, M., O'Hare, E. D., Kan, E., McCourt, S. T., Thompson, P. M., … Sowell, E. R. (2009). Relationships between brain activation and brain structure in normally developing children. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 25952604. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClintock, M. K., & Herdt, G. (1996). Rethinking puberty: The development of sexual attraction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 178183. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512422CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehta, M. A., Golembo, N. I., Nosarti, C., Colvert, E., Mota, A., Williams, S. C., … Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2009). Amygdala, hippocampal and corpus callosum size following severe early institutional deprivation: The English and Romanian Adoptees Study Pilot. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 943951. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02084.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendle, J. J., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. E. (2007). Detrimental psychological outcomes associated with early pubertal timing in adolescent girls. Developmental Review, 27, 151171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moed, A., Gershoff, E. T., Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., Losoya, S., Spinrad, T. L., & Liew, J. (2017). Parent–child negative emotion reciprocity and children's school success: An emotion-attention process model. Social Development, 26, 560574. doi:10.1111/sode.12217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Belsky, J., & Silva, P. A. (1992). Childhood experience and the onset of menarche: A test of a sociobiological model. Child Development, 63, 4758. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb03594.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1976). A typology of family social environments. Family Process, 15, 357371. doi:10.1111/j.1545-5300.1976.00357.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriceau, S., & Sullivan, R. M. (2006). Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction in infancy. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 10041006. doi:10.1038/nn1733CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Mplus user's guide. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 2006, 676. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52225.xGoogle Scholar
Neufang, S., Specht, K., Hausmann, M., Güntürkün, O., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Fink, G. R., & Konrad, K. (2009). Sex differences and the impact of steroid hormones on the developing human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 464473. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 12151229. doi:10.1162/089892902760807212CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkes, L., Fulcher, B., Yücel, M., & Fornito, A. (2018). An evaluation of the efficacy, reliability, and sensitivity of motion correction strategies for resting-state functional MRI. NeuroImage, 171, 415436. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.073CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira, A., Iñiguez, G., Corvalan, C., & Mericq, V. (2017). High DHEAS is associated with earlier pubertal events in girls but not in boys. Journal of the Endocrine Society, 1, 800808. doi:10.1210/js.2017-00120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., & Crockett, L. (1985). Pubertal timing and grade effects on adjustment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 191206. doi:10.1007/BF02090318CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 117133. doi:10.1007/BF01537962CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phan, K. L., Fitzgerald, D. A., Nathan, P. J., Moore, G. J., Uhde, T. W., & Tancer, M. E. (2005). Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 210219. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48, 175187.10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierpaoli, C., Jezzard, P., Basser, P. J., Barnett, A., & Di Chiro, G. (1996). Diffusion tensor MR imaging of the human brain. Radiology, 201, 637648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, J. D., Barnes, K. A., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage, 59, 21422154. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, J. D., Mitra, A., Laumann, T. O., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2014). NeuroImage methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage, 84, 320341. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Proos, L. A. (2009). Growth and development of Indian children adopted in Sweden. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 130, 646650.Google ScholarPubMed
Reid, B. M., Miller, B. S., Dorn, L. D., Desjardins, C., Donzella, B., & Gunnar, M. (2017). Early growth faltering in post-institutionalized youth and later anthropometric and pubertal development. Pediatric Research, 82, 278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443447. doi:10.1126/science.1100301CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romans, S. E., Martin, J. M., Gendall, K., & Herbison, G. P. (2003). Age of menarche: The role of some psychosocial factors. Psychological Medicine, 33, 933939. doi:10.1017/S0033291703007530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saczawa, M. E., Graber, J. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (2013). Methodological considerations in use of the cortisol/DHEA(S) ratio in adolescent populations. Psychoneuroendorcinology, 38, 28152819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satterthwaite, T. D., Wolf, D. H., Loughead, J., Ruparel, K., Elliott, M. A., Hakonarson, H., … Gur, R. E. (2012). Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: Relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. NeuroImage, 60, 623632. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children's reports of parental behavior: An inventory. Child Development, 36, 413424. doi:10.2307/1126465CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ségonne, F., Dale, A. M., Busa, E., Glessner, M., Salat, D., Hahn, H. K., & Fischl, B. (2004). A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. NeuroImage, 22, 10601075. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ségonne, F., Pacheco, J., & Fischl, B. (2007). Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 26, 518529. doi:10.1109/TMI.2006.887364CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seibert, T. M., & Brewer, J. B. (2011). Default network correlations analyzed on native surfaces. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 198, 301311. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.04.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalev, I., Moffitt, T. E., Sugden, K., Williams, B., Houts, R. M., Danese, A., … Caspi, A. (2013). Exposure to violence during childhood is associated with telomere erosion from 5 to 10 years of age: A longitudinal study. Molecular Psychiatry, 18, 576581. doi:10.1038/mp.2012.32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., Clasen, L., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., … Giedd, J. (2006). Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature, 440, 47. doi:10.1038/nature04513CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephens, S. B. Z., Raper, J., Bachevalier, J., & Wallen, K. (2015). Neonatal amygdala lesions advance pubertal timing in female rhesus macaques. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 51, 307317. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stover, P. J., Harlan, W. R., Hammond, J. A., Hendershot, T., & Hamilton, C. M. (2010). PhenX: A toolkit for interdisciplinary genetics research. Current Opinion in Lipidology, 21, 136140. doi:10.1097/MOL.0b013e3283377395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teilmann, G., Pedersen, C. B., Skakkebaek, N. E., & Jensen, T. K. (2006). Increased risk of precocious puberty in internationally adopted children in Denmark. Pediatrics, 118, e391e399. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-2939CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thijssen, S., Muetzel, R. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Tiemeier, H., Verhulst, F. C., … van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Insensitive parenting may accelerate the development of the amygdala-medial prefrontal cortex circuit. Development and Psychopathology, 29. doi:10.1017/S0954579417000141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomiyama, A. J., O'Donovan, A., Lin, J., Puterman, E., Lazaro, A., Chan, J., … Epel, E. (2012). Does cellular aging relate to patterns of allostasis? An examination of basal and stress reactive HPA axis activity and telomere length. Physiology and Behavior, 106, 4045. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.11.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tottenham, N. (2015). Social scaffolding of human amygdala-mpfc circuit development. Social Neuroscience, 10, 489499. doi:10.1080/17470919.2015.1087424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gilhooly, T., … Casey, B. J. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Developmental Science, 13, 4661. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyborowska, A., Volman, I., Niermann, H. C. M., Pouwels, J. L., Cillessen, A. H. N., Toni, I., & Roelofs, K. (2018). Early-life and pubertal stress differentially modulate grey matter development in human adolescents. Scientific Reports, 8, 111. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-27439-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyrka, A. R., Parade, S. H., Price, L. H., Kao, H. T., Porton, B., Philip, N. S., … Carpenter, L. L. (2016). Alterations of mitochondrial dna copy number and telomere length with early adversity and psychopathology. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 7886. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dijk, K. R. A., Hedden, T., Venkataraman, A., Evans, K. C., Lazar, S. W., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: Theory, properties, and optimization. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 297321. doi:10.1152/jn.00783.2009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, W. M., Viola, P., Atsumi, H., Nakajima, S., & Kikinis, R. (1996). Multi-modal volume registration by maximisation of mutual information. Medical Image Analysis, 1, 3551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wendelken, C., Ferrer, E., Ghetti, S., Bailey, S. K., Cutting, L., & Bunge, S. A. (2017). Frontoparietal structural connectivity in childhood predicts development of functional connectivity and reasoning ability: A large-scale longitudinal investigation. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 85498558. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3726-16.2017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westlye, L. T., Walhovd, K. B., Dale, A. M., Bjørnerud, A., Due-Tønnessen, P., Engvig, A., … Fjell, A. M. (2010). Life-span changes of the human brain white matter: Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 20552068. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp280CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Ford, J. M. (2012). Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 4976. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittle, S., Simmons, J. G., Dennison, M., Vijayakumar, N., Schwartz, O., Yap, M. B. H., … Allen, N. B. (2014). Developmental cognitive neuroscience positive parenting predicts the development of adolescent brain structure: A longitudinal study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 717.10.1016/j.dcn.2013.10.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wichstrom, L. (2000). Predictors of adolescent suicide attempts: A nationally representative longitudinal study of norwegian adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 603610. doi:10.1097/00004583-200005000-00014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winer, J. P., Powers, S. I., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (2017). Early pubertal timing and childhood family adversity interact to predict newlywed women's anxiety symptoms. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26, 591602. doi:10.1007/s10826-016-0575-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, M., Kujawa, A., Lu, L. H., Fitzgerald, D. A., Klumpp, H., Fitzgerald, K. D., … Phan, K. L. (2016). Age-related changes in amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotional face processing from childhood into young adulthood. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 16841695.10.1002/hbm.23129CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhuang, J., Hrabe, J., Kangarlu, A., Xu, D., Bansal, R., Branch, C. A., & Peterson, B. S. (2006). Correction of eddy-current distortions in diffusion tensor images using the known directions and strengths of diffusion gradients. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 24, 11881193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zucker, R. A., Gonzalez, R., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Paulus, M. P., Arroyo, J., Fuligni, A., … Wills, T. (2018). Assessment of culture and environment in the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development Study: Rationale, description of measures, and early data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 107120. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics; continuous variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Sample characteristics; categorical variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Partial correlations between latent variables (LVs) and child behavior measures

Figure 3

Table 4. Correlation between MRI measures

Figure 4

Figure 1. Associations between family environment and amygdala–mPFC measures are mediated by pubertal stage. Values are standardized coefficients. *p < .01. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Fam Env, family environment. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex. CT, cortical thickness. CA, cortical area. SV, subcortical volume. FA, fractional anisotropy. CON-Am FC, cingulu-opercular network–amygdala functional connectivity.

Figure 5

Figure 2. Mediation of association between family environment and amygdala–mPFC measures by pubertal stage for (a) girls and (b) boys. Values are standardized coefficients. *p < .01. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Fam Env, family environment. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex. CT, cortical thickness. CA, cortical area. SV, subcortical volume. FA, fractional anisotropy. CON-Am FC, cingulu-opercular network–amygdala functional connectivity.

Figure 6

Table 5. Mediation model parameters: Anterior cingulate cortical thickness and area

Figure 7

Table 6. Mediation model parameters: Anterior cingulate white matter fractional anisotropy

Figure 8

Table 7. Mediation model parameters: Cinculo-opercular network–amygdala connectivity

Supplementary material: File

Thijssen et al. supplementary material

Thijssen et al. supplementary material 1

Download Thijssen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 401.2 KB