Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T11:19:31.718Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predictors of executive function trajectories in adolescents with and without ADHD: Links with academic outcomes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 July 2023

Delshad M. Shroff*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA, USA
Nicholas C. Dunn
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, OH, USA
Cathrin D. Green
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
Rosanna Breaux
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA, USA
Stephen P. Becker
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, OH, USA
Joshua M. Langberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Delshad Shroff; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Changes in executive function (EF) occur during adolescence with several factors (e.g., parenting styles, socioeconomic status) influencing the development of EF abilities. These changes are important as EF has been strongly linked with a range of outcomes including academic achievement, job performance, and social–emotional well-being. However, few studies have examined variability in EF trajectories during this critical developmental period, or trajectories in samples known to have specific impairments with EF, such as adolescents diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The present study examined differential trajectories of three domains of parent-rated EF in 302 adolescents (167 males; Mage = 13.17 years) with and without ADHD (53.6% with ADHD) from grade 8 to 10. The study also explored whether adolescent ADHD, parent ADHD, and parents’ own EF predicted EF trajectories in addition to the longitudinal relation between trajectories and academic outcomes. Findings suggest that adolescence is marked by significant variability in EF development due to factors such as ADHD status, parent ADHD, and parent EF ability. Additionally, adolescents who displayed poor EF abilities throughout middle and high school had significantly lower grade point averages and poorer parent-, teacher-, and self-reported academic outcomes. Implications for interventions targeting EF deficits among adolescents with and without ADHD are discussed.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Executive function (EF) is a set of higher-order cognitive processes, such as working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility, that guides purposeful, goal-directed behavior (Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010). EF abilities have been linked with a range of outcomes such as academic achievement (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Monuteaux, Doyle, Seidman, Wilens, Ferrero, Morgan and Faraone2004; Langberg et al., Reference Langberg, Dvorsky and Evans2013), job performance (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Nevado-Montenegro and Hinshaw2012), and social–emotional well-being (Miller & Hinshaw, Reference Miller and Hinshaw2010). Significant developmental changes in EF occur during adolescence with several factors (e.g., socioeconomic status [SES], IQ) influencing the development of EF abilities (Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010; Boelema et al., Reference Boelema, Harakeh, Ormel, Hartman, Vollebergh and van Zandvoort2014). Though research documents a general developmental trajectory of improvement in EF during adolescence (e.g., Ferguson et al., Reference Ferguson, Brunsdon and Bradford2021; Shanmugan & Satterthwaite, Reference Shanmugan and Satterthwaite2016), to our knowledge only three studies have looked at whether there is variability in the development of EF during this critical developmental period (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Altamirano, Corley, Young, Rhea and Hewitt2016; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Loya and Hinshaw2013; Qian et al., Reference Qian, Shuai, Chan, Qian and Wang2013). However, these three studies used community samples and performance-based measures of EF. No study to date has examined differential trajectories of EF rather than changes at the group level on average in a comprehensively diagnosed sample of adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) relative to controls using behavioral ratings of EF. Given this backdrop, the present study sought to examine differential trajectories of EF using parent-reported behavioral ratings of three domains of EF (behavioral, cognitive, and emotional) in a sample of adolescents with and without ADHD, a clinical population known to experience significant EF deficits (e.g., Thorell, Reference Thorell2007; Willcutt et al., Reference Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington2005). Furthermore, this study explored whether adolescent ADHD status, parent ADHD status, and parents’ own EF abilities predicted these EF trajectories above and beyond relevant demographic factors (biological sex, medication status, behavior therapy status, utilization of an Individualized Education Program [IEP] or 504 plan, SES, IQ), and examined the relation between these differential trajectories of EF and several academic outcomes. Importantly, we collected ratings of academic functioning from parents and teachers, intensive daily-data over a 2-week period, and more objective assessments, such as grade point average (GPA) and assignments turned in, to provide a comprehensive assessment of academic outcomes.

Normative developmental trajectory of EF during adolescence

Whereas the foundational components of EF such as inhibition and shifting develop in early childhood, EF abilities continue to develop and strengthen during adolescence (Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010; Boelema et al., Reference Boelema, Harakeh, Ormel, Hartman, Vollebergh and van Zandvoort2014). Adolescence is a critical period for development of new, and maturation of existing, EF abilities (Luciana, Reference Luciana2016). For example, Boelema et al. (Reference Boelema, Harakeh, Ormel, Hartman, Vollebergh and van Zandvoort2014) found significant improvements in all six measured components of EF – focused attention inhibition, sustained attention, speed of processing, working memory, and shift attention – from early to late adolescence.

The transition to adolescence is associated with an increased demand for self-regulation in academic and interpersonal domains; therefore, there is a need to exert higher cognitive control (Brieant et al., Reference Brieant, King-Casas and Kim-Spoon2022; Burnett et al., Reference Burnett, Scratch and Anderson2013). Notably, EF plays an important role in shaping an adolescent’s social, emotional, and educational competencies (Bierman et al., Reference Bierman, Nix, Greenberg, Blair and Domitrovich2008; Brieant et al., Reference Brieant, King-Casas and Kim-Spoon2022; Nyongesa et al., Reference Nyongesa, Ssewanyana, Mutua, Chongwo, Scerif, Newton and Abubakar2019). Given this, it is critical to examine factors affecting EF development during this time period (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Nyongesa et al., Reference Nyongesa, Ssewanyana, Mutua, Chongwo, Scerif, Newton and Abubakar2019) and to screen and implement intervention strategies for EF problems (Tonkin, Reference Tonkin2001). Understanding factors that shape EF development will enhance interventions to improve such adaptive behaviors (Gray-Burrows et al., Reference Gray-Burrows, Taylor, O’Connor, Sutherland, Stoet and Conner2019).

Measurement of EF

Prior longitudinal work on EF trajectories has used group-based approaches and performance-based measures of EF (Nyongesa et al., Reference Nyongesa, Ssewanyana, Mutua, Chongwo, Scerif, Newton and Abubakar2019). Performance or task-based approaches have several strengths, including lack of rater bias. However, they exhibit low correlations with ratings of EF and have been criticized for having low ecological validity (Nyongesa et al., Reference Nyongesa, Ssewanyana, Mutua, Chongwo, Scerif, Newton and Abubakar2019). As such, it is also important to understand EF trajectories using self and informant reports of EF. Therefore, our study employed parent report of EF to measure behavioral manifestations of adolescent EF difficulties in daily life.

Individual and family factors that influence EF

Several individual and family factors influence the development and maturation of EF. Individual factors such as IQ (Ardila et al., Reference Ardila, Pineda and Rosselli2000, Hackman et al., Reference Hackman, Gallop, Evans and Farah2015) and family factors such as SES (Last et al., Reference Last, Lawson, Breiner, Steinberg and Farah2018; Ursache & Noble, Reference Ursache and Noble2016) and parenting (Fay-Stammbach et al., Reference Fay-Stammbach, Hawes and Meredith2014) have been found to have a significant effect on the development of EF abilities. Specifically, children and adolescents with a higher IQ and from a higher SES have better EF abilities (Last et al., Reference Last, Lawson, Breiner, Steinberg and Farah2018), whereas youth from lower SES backgrounds and whose parents use negative parenting strategies (e.g., corporal punishment, emotionally reactive responses) have worse EF (Bernier et al., Reference Bernier, Carlson and Whipple2010; Hughes & Ensor, Reference Hughes and Ensor2009).

One proposed mechanism for the relation between family factors and youth EF is parents’ own EF abilities. Initial evidence of intergenerational transmission of EF has been established from early childhood (i.e., as early as 4 months) through adolescence (e.g., Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Oddi, Laake, Murdock and Bachmann2013; Cuevas et al., Reference Cuevas, Deater-Deckard, Kim-Spoon, Watson, Morasch and Bell2014; Deater-Deckard, Reference Deater-Deckard2014; Jester et al., Reference Jester, Nigg, Puttler, Long, Fitzgerald and Zucker2009). However, most of this research has focused on toddlerhood and the preschool years, which is not surprising given that this is when some EF abilities first emerge. Only two studies to date have examined how parent EF abilities impact EF development during adolescence (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008; Jester et al., Reference Jester, Nigg, Puttler, Long, Fitzgerald and Zucker2009). Specifically, Jester et al. (Reference Jester, Nigg, Puttler, Long, Fitzgerald and Zucker2009) found that parent EF was significantly associated with adolescent EF, independent of IQ, in a sample of 434 adolescents at ages 12–14 (M = 13.7) and then again at ages 15–17 (M = 16.5). Friedman et al. (Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008) found similar results in a twin study examining three aspects of EF – inhibiting dominant responses, updating working memory representations, and shifting between task sets – in a sample of 582 adolescents ages 13–17 years. Latent variable analysis showed that individual differences in adolescent EF were entirely accounted for by genetic factors beyond the contributions of IQ, using performance-based EF tasks to measure parent and adolescent EF abilities (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008). Given a growing body of research suggesting that behavioral ratings of EF offer distinct information from EF tasks and might carry higher ecological validity (Baars et al., Reference Baars, Nije Bijvank, Tonnaer and Jolles2015; Barkley & Murphy, Reference Barkley and Murphy2011; Langberg et al., Reference Langberg, Dvorsky and Evans2013), it is critical for research to examine this familial transmission effect using self- and informant-report measures. Additionally, both of the previous studies utilized community samples; it is important to understand if EF trajectories and their predictors differ across clinical samples such as between adolescents with ADHD versus typically developing adolescents.

EF abilities among adolescents with ADHD

ADHD is a highly heritable disorder that is characterized by difficulties in organization, planning, remembering details, and paying attention (Martel et al., Reference Martel, Nikolas and Nigg2007). It is well-established that individuals with ADHD display higher levels of EF deficits in daily life activities and on performance-based tasks, relative to individuals without ADHD (see Willcutt et al., Reference Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington2005 for a review; pooled effect sizes: rs = .46–.69; Gordon & Hinshaw, Reference Gordon and Hinshaw2020; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Loya and Hinshaw2013). In fact, research has shown that on average, children with ADHD experience a 3-year lag in their EF skills, relative to their typically developing peers (Berger et al., Reference Berger, Slobodin, Aboud, Melamed and Cassuto2013). Studies by Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Loya and Hinshaw2013) and Gordon and Hinshaw (Reference Gordon and Hinshaw2020) compared the developmental trajectories of EF between 140 females with ADHD and 88 females without ADHD. They found that females with ADHD differed from controls in their trajectories of EF development, such that they displayed consistently lower EF abilities compared to females without ADHD. Additionally, for females with ADHD, change in EF over time was associated with ADHD symptom change, indicating that ADHD symptoms can increase or partially remit as a result of EF development. This finding was corroborated by another recent study in which Silverstein et al. (Reference Silverstein, Faraone, Leon, Biederman, Spencer and Adler2020) showed strong positive relations between ADHD symptoms and daily life EF deficits.

In fact, EF has been proposed to be a potential endophenotype for ADHD (Fu et al., Reference Fu, Chen, Li, Wang, Liu and Qian2021; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Weibman, Halperin and Li2019), suggesting that part of the heritability of ADHD can be linked to the familial transmission of EF. As such, this study aims to gain a better understanding of whether parent EF abilities predict the developmental trajectory of EF in adolescents with ADHD compared to typically developing adolescents. Enhancing the field’s understanding of the potentially distinct and divergent development and maturation of EF abilities for adolescents with ADHD could lead to proposing targeted approaches for improving EF in these at-risk adolescents.

EF and academic outcomes

The association between EF abilities and academic performance is robust (Spiegel et al., Reference Spiegel, Goodrich, Morris, Osborne and Lonigan2021; Zelazo et al., Reference Zelazo, Blair and Willoughby2016). Studies have shown that EF is associated with educational success throughout development, with EF directly and indirectly predicting classroom difficulties (Zelazo et al., Reference Zelazo, Blair and Willoughby2016), overall school achievement (e.g., Bull & Scerif, Reference Bull and Scerif2001; Mazzocco & Kover, Reference Mazzocco and Kover2007), reading and mathematics (e.g., Morrison et al., Reference Morrison, Ponitz, McClelland, Calkins and Bell2010: Peng et al., Reference Peng, Namkung, Barnes and Sun2016; Peng et al., Reference Peng, Barnes, Wang, Wang, Li, Swanson, Dardick and Tao2018), high school completion (Vitaro et al., Reference Vitaro, Brendgen, Larose and Trembaly2005), and college graduation (McClelland et al., Reference McClelland, Acock, Piccinin, Rhea and Stallings2013). As such, understanding whether differential developmental EF trajectories predict academic outcomes for adolescents with and without ADHD will enable us to better identify at-risk adolescents who may benefit from interventions targeting EF deficits, which can directly and indirectly influence academic success.

Present study

Given this backdrop, the present study aimed to address three aims: (1) to identify differential trajectories of EF (behavioral, emotional, and cognitive regulation) from middle school to high school (fall of 8th grade, spring of 8th grade, and fall of 10th grade) in a sample of adolescents with and without comprehensively diagnosed ADHD; (2) to investigate whether parent EF, parent ADHD, and adolescent ADHD status predicted these differential EF trajectories; and (3) to analyze the association between the identified EF trajectories and a range of objective and parent-, teacher-, and adolescent-reported academic outcomes in adolescents. Parents provided ratings of both their adolescent’s EF and their own EF as well as their own symptoms of ADHD. Examining EF deficits at multiple timepoints during adolescence will provide a better understanding of differences in development and maturation of EF abilities across this critical developmental period. Additionally, using daily ratings collected from teachers and adolescents to assess academic performance (motivation for schoolwork, effort put forth in classwork, quality of completed work, percentage of assignments turned in) ensures higher accuracy in the reporting of difficulties as they arise and are related to daily life activities. Based on prior research with adolescent participants (Boelema et al., Reference Boelema, Harakeh, Ormel, Hartman, Vollebergh and van Zandvoort2014; Torgalsbøen et al., Reference Torgalsbøen, Zeiner and Øie2021), we hypothesized that three EF trajectories would exist for behavioral, emotional, and cognitive domains of EF (low EF deficits that remain stable or improve across adolescence, moderate EF deficits that remain stable or improve across adolescence, and high EF deficits that remain stable or improve across adolescence). Further, we hypothesized that ADHD status (Gordon and Hinshaw, Reference Gordon and Hinshaw2020; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Loya and Hinshaw2013; Willcutt et al., Reference Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington2005), parent ADHD (Joyner et al., Reference Joyner, Silver and Stavinoha2009; Schroeder & Kelley, Reference Schroeder and Kelley2009), and parent EF abilities (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008; Jester et al., Reference Jester, Nigg, Puttler, Long, Fitzgerald and Zucker2009) would be significant predictors of EF trajectories for all three domains, controlling for relevant covariates (SES, medication status, behavior therapy status, utilization of an IEP or 504 plan, biological sex, IQ). Finally, we hypothesized that these trajectories would be associated with a robust multi-source multi-method assessment of academic outcomes, including homework performance, academic motivation, and assignment completion with the consistently high EF deficits group having significantly worse academic outcomes.

Method

Participants

Participants were 302 adolescents (167 males) and a primary caregiver (84.1% mothers, 13.2% fathers, 2.6% other caregivers) recruited from local schools in the Southeastern and Midwestern United States in the8th grade. At the time of recruitment, adolescents were between 13 and 14 years of age (M = 13.17, SD = 0.40). About half of the sample (53.6%) received an ADHD diagnosis based on the Children’s Interview for Psychiatric Syndromes (ChIPS; Weller et al., Reference Weller, Fristad, Weller and Rooney1999) diagnostic interview with parents, which included assessment of age of onset, duration, cross-setting impairment, and common comorbid conditions. Adolescents identified as predominantly White (81.8%), with 7.9% identifying as biracial/multiracial, 5.3% as Black, and 4.6% as Asian. Participants came from a range of socioeconomic backgrounds (M family income = $93,179, SD = $34,847).

Procedures

Adolescents and their parents were recruited as part of a larger longitudinal study of adolescents with and without ADHD [R305A160126]. Inclusion criteria included: (1) estimated full scale IQ ≥ 80; (2) enrollment in 8th grade; and (3) enrollment in regular education classes. Exclusion criteria included: (1) not meeting criteria for either the ADHD or comparison group; (2) prior diagnosis of an organic sleep disorder; and (3) meeting criteria for autism spectrum disorder, bipolar disorder, or a dissociative or psychotic disorder. Adolescents received an ADHD diagnostic evaluation during the initial assessment and were required to meet all Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) criteria for ADHD predominately inattentive presentation or combined presentation on the ChIPS to be eligible for the ADHD group. To be eligible for the comparison group, three or fewer symptoms had to be endorsed in both the inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity domains. Youth that did not meet full criteria for an ADHD diagnosis but had more than three symptoms in either the inattention or hyperactivity/impulsivity domains were excluded from the study. Parent ratings of adolescent EF abilities were collected at three of the five study timepoints (T1: fall of 8th grade, N = 302; T2: spring of 8th grade, n = 284; T3: fall of 10th grade, n = 283).

Measures

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Second Edition (BRIEF-2)

Impairments in adolescent EF were assessed using the BRIEF-2 (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2015). Parents completed the BRIEF-2 parent form at T1, T2, and T3. This rating form consists of 55 statements about different domains of adolescents’ EF skills such as “My child has trouble accepting a different way to solve a problem with things such as schoolwork, friends, or tasks” and “My child is not aware of how his/her behavior affects or bothers others.” Responses were provided on a 3-point Likert scale ranging from Never to Often. The BRIEF-2 generates three broad indices: behavioral regulation index (BRI), emotion regulation index (ERI), and cognitive regulation index (CRI), which together form the Global Executive Composite score. The BRI includes the Inhibit and Self-Monitor clinical scales, which assesses an adolescent’s ability to effectively modulate behavior. The ERI includes the Shift and Emotional Control scales and measures an adolescent’s ability to manage their emotions and emotional responses as well as appropriately adjust to changes in the environment. The CRI includes the Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Task Monitor, and Organization of Materials scales and assesses an adolescent’s ability to effectively problem solve and regulate cognitive processes. In the present study, we used the BRI, ERI, and CRI scores as a measure of adolescent EF difficulties, where higher scores reflect greater deficits in EF. Adolescent EF deficit scores are presented in Table 1; adolescents with ADHD had significantly greater EF deficits in all three domains at all three timepoints relative to adolescents without ADHD. Reliability was good in this sample for all timepoints (αs = .87–.89).

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of demographic variables and adolescent and parent EF deficits across ADHD and comparison groups

Note. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001; descriptive statistics are reported for T1 unless otherwise specified.

Barkley Deficits in Executive Functioning Scale – Short Version (BDEFS-S)

Parents completed the BDEFS-S (Barkley, Reference Barkley2011) at the T2 assessment. The BDEFS-S is a self-report form with 20 statements (e.g., “I don't seem to process information as quickly or accurately as others”) that evaluate dimensions of adult EF in daily life activities over the past 6 months. Parents report on the frequency of thoughts and behaviors that reflect EF deficits. Responses are provided on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from Never/Rarely to Very Often. For the present study, we used the summary score as a measure of parent EF. Parent EF scores are presented in Table 1; parents of adolescents with ADHD had significantly more EF difficulties than parents of adolescents without ADHD. Reliability was high in the current sample (α = .91).

Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale – Fourth Edition (BAARS-IV)

Parents completed the BAARS-IV (Barkley, Reference Barkley2011) at the T2 assessment. The BAARS-IV is a self-report form that assesses current and childhood ADHD symptoms and domains of impairment. Responses are provided on a 4-point Likert scale from Never/Rarely to Very Often. For the present study, we used the mean score for ADHD symptoms, which is presented in Table 1. Parents of adolescents with ADHD had higher levels of ADHD symptoms, although this difference was not statistically significant. Reliability was good in the current sample (α = .87).

Academic outcomes

Homework performance questionnaire (HPQ)

The HPQ (Power et al., Reference Power, Dombrowski, Watkins, Mautone and Eagle2007) consists of 22 items that assess student’s homework performance, and each item is rated on a 7-point scale ranging from 0 (Never/Rarely, 0%–10% of the time) to 6 (Almost Always/Always, 91%–100% of the time). Parents and teachers reported on the frequency of behaviors with items worded such that higher scores indicate less impairment at T3. For the current study, the total mean score was used. Reliability was high in the current sample for parent report and moderate for teacher report (αs = .92 and .71, respectively).

Daily diary ratings

At T3, adolescents and teachers completed daily ratings on adolescent motivation for schoolwork, effort put forth in classwork, and the quality of completed work, for a 2-week period; teachers also reported on the percentage of assignments the adolescents turned in daily. Motivation, effort, and work quality were rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Not at all motivated, Barely any effort, and Poor quality, respectively) to 4 (Very motivated, A lot of effort, and Great quality, respectively). The percentage of assignments turned in was rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (0%–10%) to 5 (75%–100%). The daily diary ratings were averaged across ten school days to create the motivation, effort, work quality, and assignments turned in variables used in this study. Reliability was high for all daily diary ratings within subjects (αs = .92–.95 for teachers, αs = .95–.99 for adolescents). However, inter-rater reliability for daily diary ratings were low in the current sample (αs = .36–.43).

Grade point average

Following the end of their 10th grade year (i.e., corresponding to T3), grades for the four core subject areas: English, Science, Math, and History were collected and used to calculate GPA on a 4-point scale (As = 4.0, Bs = 3.0, Cs = 2.0, Ds or below = 1.0). Since not all high schools allowed GPA to go above a 4.0 (i.e., using a weighted system for honors/advanced placement classes), values were capped at 4.0.

Demographic variables

Parents reported on adolescent biological sex and family income on a demographic form. Parents reported on adolescent medication status, behavior therapy status, and utilization of an IEP or 504 plan on the Services for Children and Adolescents – Parent Interview (Jensen et al., Reference Jensen, Hoagwood, Roper, Arnold, Odbert, Crowe, Molina, Hechtman, Hinshaw, Hoza, Newcorn, Swanson and Wells2004). IQ was assessed using the two-subtest estimate on the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-II (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2011).

Analytic plan

Descriptive statistics were created for relevant variables and correlations between the outcome variables, predictor variable, and variables shown in the literature to be associated with EF were examined. Next, a repeated measures ANOVA was run to examine change in EF on average from fall of 8th grade to fall of 10th grade. Finally, we ran growth mixture models (GMM) to explore the differential trajectories of adolescent EF from fall of 8th grade to fall of 10th grade, which accounted for unequal spacing between time points (i.e., timepoints were set at 0, 1, 4; approximately 6 months between time 1 and time 2 and approximately 18 months between time 2 and time 3). GMM examine multiple unobserved (latent) classes that can differ in intercepts and slopes, and allow for class-specific variations in these parameters (Jung & Wickrama, Reference Jung and Wickrama2008; Lubke & Muthén, Reference Lubke and Muthén2007; Ram & Grimm, Reference Ram and Grimm2009). GMM were run using the “3-step” approach for model estimation (Vermunt, Reference Vermunt2010), which allows for the incorporation of predictors and/or outcomes while still protecting the class formation determined in the first step (i.e., unconditional model) from the potential influence of covariates (Muthén, Reference Muthén2003) by using the auxiliary variable option in Mplus.

When determining the number of classes in the unconditional model, an increasing number of classes were examined to determine the best fit for the data. Model fit was determined using Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), Lo–Mendell–Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR), bootstrapped parametric likelihood ratio test (BLRT), entropy, classification probabilities (how distinct each class is from the other classes), and signs of model instability (e.g., class membership of less than 10%), consistent with prior recommendations (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2000; Tein et al., Reference Tein, Coxe and Cham2013). Better model fit was indicated by having the majority of model fit indicators in a model’s favor as indicated by AIC and BIC decreasing, LMR and BLRT remaining significant, entropy values approaching 1 indicating clear delineation of classes (Celeux & Soromenho, Reference Celeux and Soromenho1996), and classification probabilities remaining ≥ .70. Once the best fitting model was determined, models were examined to determine if trajectories had significant slopes (i.e., indicating either improvement or worsening in EF difficulties; non-significant slope indicated stable EF difficulties). Given prior research, family income, IQ, biological sex, adolescent medication status, behavior therapy status, IEP/ 504 plan status were included as covariates in the second step, after distinct trajectories were identified. Missing data were addressed using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors in all models. Next, we examined if adolescent ADHD status, parent ADHD status, and parent EF difficulties were predictors of membership in the identified latent classes using the R3STEP auxiliary function, which runs multinomial logistic regressions. Finally, we examined if latent class membership was associated with differential academic outcomes based on parent-, teacher-, and self-report at T3 using the DE3STEP auxiliary function.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Descriptive statistics and correlations for baseline variables are presented in Table 2. All three domain of adolescent EF deficits (BRI, ERI, and CRI) were significantly correlated with parent EF deficits, parent ADHD, adolescent ADHD status, and SES at all 3 timepoints. In contrast, IQ was only significantly correlated with the CRI domain of adolescent EF deficits at all 3 timepoints. Additionally, biological sex was correlated with adolescent EF deficits in the BRI domain only at T1 and with adolescent EF deficits in the CRI domain at all 3 timepoints. A repeated measures ANOVA demonstrated a statistically significant decrease in adolescent EF deficits, F (2, 244) = 39.56, p < .001, on average from fall of 8th grade (M = 57.62, SD = 12.66) to fall of 10th grade (M = 53.45, SD = 11.27).

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and correlations between study variables

Note. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; EF = executive function; BRI = Behavior Regulation Index; ERI = Emotion Regulation Index; CRI = Cognitive Regulation Index; higher sores on EF variables indicate greater deficits. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .00.

Trajectories of adolescent EF abilities

Behavior regulation index

A three-class model was the best fitting model for adolescent BRI trajectories, exhibiting strong model fit indices for all indicators, except for the LMR, and acceptable discrimination of classes as indicated by the classification probabilities and entropy (Table 3). As can be seen in Figure 1a, the first trajectory had BRI difficulties within normal limits in fall of 8th grade (M = 41.85, SE = 1.45) that remained stable from 8th to 10th grade (Low Stable BRI; 55 adolescents; 18% of sample; non-significant slope: b = −0.50, SE = 0.39, p = .198). The second trajectory also had BRI difficulties within normal limits in fall of 8th grade (M = 47.60, SE = 1.27) that remained stable from 8th to 10th grade (Moderate Stable BRI; 107 adolescents; 35% of sample; non-significant slope: b = 0.54, SE = 0.72, p = .448). The third trajectory had BRI scores just below the clinical level in fall of 8th grade (M = 64.74, SE = 1.22) that improved from 8th to 10th grade (High Improving BRI; 141 adolescents; 47% of sample; significant slope: b = −1.63, SE = 0.26, p < .001). The Low Stable BRI class had significantly less members who were attending behavior therapy at T1, T2, and T3 relative to the two other classes, ps < .030. Additionally, the Low Stable BRI class had significantly less members who were receiving an IEP/504 plan than the two other classes at T3, ps < .001. Classes did not significantly differ on any other covariates.

Figure 1. (a) Three-class model for behavior regulation index trajectories. (b) Three-class model for emotion regulation index trajectories. (c) Three-class model for cognitive regulation index trajectories. Note. Executive function difficulties are reported as T-scores; values within normal limits range from 40 to 60, T-scores between 60 and 65 are considered borderline clinical, and T-scores of 65 or above indicate clinical concern.

Table 3. Model fit statistics for unconditional executive function trajectories models

Note. Bolded row represents the best fitting growth mixture model. AIC = Akaike Information Criterion; BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; LMR = Lo–Mendell–Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test; BLRT = bootstrapped parametric likelihood ratio test. The LMR and BLRT compare the fit of one model to the previous (k-1 factor) model.

Emotion regulation index

A three-class model was the best fitting model for adolescent ERI trajectories, with strong model fit indices for all indicators and acceptable discrimination of classes as indicated by the classification probabilities and entropy (Table 3). The first trajectory had ERI difficulties within normal limits in fall of 8th grade (M = 42.88, SE = 0.60) and had their EF abilities improve from 8th to 10th grade (Low Improving ERI; 47 adolescents; 16% of sample; significant slope: b = −0.55, SE = 0.15, p < .001). The second trajectory also had ERI difficulties within normal limits in fall of 8th grade (M = 45.51, SE = 0.92), that remained stable from 8th to 10th grade (Moderate Stable ERI; 183 adolescents; 60% of sample; non-significant slope: b = −0.29, SE = 0.20, p = .148). The third trajectory had borderline clinical levels of ERI difficulties in fall of 8th grade (M = 62.04, SE = −1.23) that improved from 8th to 10th grade (High Improving ERI; 73 adolescents; 24% of sample; significant slope: b = −1.23, SE = 0.22, p < .001). These trajectories are presented in Figure 1b. The Low Improving ERI class had significantly less members who were attending behavior therapy at T1 and T2 relative to the High Improving ERI class, ps < .015. Additionally, the Low Stable ERI class had significantly less members who were receiving an IEP/504 plan than the two other classes at T3, ps < .001. Classes did not significantly differ on any other covariates.

Cognitive regulation index

A three-class model was also the best fitting model for adolescent CRI trajectories, with strong model fit indices for all indicators except LMR and acceptable discrimination of classes as indicated by the classification probabilities and entropy (Table 3). As can be seen in Figure 1c, the first trajectory had CRI difficulties within normal limits in fall of 8th grade (M = 41.64, SE = 1.05) that remained stable from 8th to 10th grade (Low Stable CRI; 52 adolescents; 17% of sample; non-significant slope: b = −0.76, SE = 1.09, p = .486). The second trajectory also had CRI difficulties within normal limits in fall of 8th grade (M = 49.97, SE = 3.51), with these difficulties remaining stable from 8th to 10th grade (Moderate Stable CRI; 82 adolescents; 27% of sample; non-significant slope: b = −0.18, SE = 0.43, p = .668). The third trajectory had clinical levels of CRI difficulties in fall of 8th grade (M = 68.67, SE = 2.74) that significantly improved from 8th to 10th grade (High Improving CRI; 168 adolescents; 56% of sample; significant slope: b = −1.27, SE = 0.20, p < .001). The Low Stable CRI group had significantly less members on medication at T1, T2, and T3 relative to the two other groups ps < .001. Additionally, the Low Stable CRI group had significantly less members attending behavior therapy and receiving an IEP/504 plan at T3 relative to the two other groups ps < .001. Classes did not significantly differ on any other covariates.

Predictors of latent class membership

ADHD status significantly predicted membership in the three identified BRI latent class trajectories. Adolescents with ADHD were significantly more likely to be in the High Improving than Low Stable and Moderate Stable classes (odds ratio = 104.30 and 31.66, respectively), ps < .001. Parent ADHD symptoms also significantly predicted membership in the three identified BRI latent class trajectories. Adolescents in the High Improving class had parents with significantly higher ADHD symptoms than the Low Stable and Moderate Stable (odds ratio = 35.94 and 17.53, respectively) classes, p = .009 and .012, respectively. However, parent EF abilities did not predict class membership when accounting for adolescent ADHD status, parent ADHD symptoms, income, medication status, behavior therapy status, IEP/ 504 plan status, IQ, and sex, ps > .244.

For the ERI, only adolescent ADHD status significantly predicted membership in the three identified latent classes. Specifically, adolescents with ADHD were significantly more likely to be in the High Improving than Low Improving and marginally more likely to be in the Moderate Stable classes, odds ratio = 43.63 and 9.46; p = .032 and .062, respectively. In contrast, parent ADHD symptoms and EF abilities did not predict class membership, accounting for adolescent ADHD status, parent ADHD symptoms, and relevant covariates (i.e., income, medication status, behavior therapy status, IEP/ 504 plan status, IQ, sex), ps > .113.

ADHD status also significantly predicted membership in the three identified CRI latent class trajectories, such that adolescents with ADHD were significantly more likely to be in the High Improving than Low Stable and Moderate Stable classes, odds ratio = 37.11 and 20.06, respectively, ps < .001. Similarly, parent EF difficulties significantly predicted membership in the three identified BRI latent class trajectories, such that adolescents in the High Improving group had parents with significantly more EF difficulties than the Low Stable and Moderate Stable classes, odds ratio = 14.93 and 17.53, respectively, ps < .001. Parent ADHD symptoms did not predict class membership, accounting for adolescent ADHD status, parent EF difficulties, and relevant covariates (i.e., income, medication status, behavior therapy status, IEP/ 504 plan status, IQ, sex), ps > .229.

Association between latent class membership and academic outcomes

Means and standard errors for the academic outcomes of the three latent classes for the BRI, ERI, and CRI are presented in Tables 4, 5, and 6, respectively.

Table 4. Means and standard errors for academic outcomes of three latent classes for the behavior regulation index

Note. HPQ = Homework Performance Questionnaire; PR = parent report; TR = teacher report; AR = adolescent report; GPA = grade point average. Means followed by a common letter are not significantly different. Means followed by a different letter are significantly different; for homework performance and grade point average, different letters denote significant difference at an alpha level of 0.001; for motivation for schoolwork, effort put into classwork, work quality, and assignments turned in, different letters denote significant difference at an alpha level of 0.05. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 5. Means and standard errors for academic outcomes of three latent classes for the emotion regulation index

Note. HPQ = Homework Performance Questionnaire; PR = parent report; TR = teacher report; AR = adolescent report; GPA = grade point average. Means followed by a common letter are not significantly different. Means followed by a different letter are significantly different; for homework performance and grade point average, different letters denote significant difference at an alpha level of 0.001; for motivation for schoolwork, effort put into classwork, work quality, and assignments turned in, different letters denote significant difference at an alpha level of 0.05. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 6. Means and standard errors for academic outcomes of three latent classes for the cognitive regulation index

Note. HPQ = Homework Performance Questionnaire; PR = parent report; TR = teacher report; AR = adolescent report; GPA = grade point average. Means followed by a common letter are not significantly different. Means followed by a different letter are significantly different; for homework performance and grade point average, different letters denote significant difference at an alpha level of 0.001; for motivation for schoolwork, effort put into classwork, work quality, and assignments turned in, different letters denote significant difference at an alpha level of 0.05. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Homework performance

Members of the High Improving latent class displayed significantly poorer homework performance in 10th grade relative to the two other latent classes based on parent and teacher report, χ2 = 5.29–158.44, all ps < .021. Additionally, the Moderate Stable class was significantly lower than the Low Stable class for parent and teacher-reported HPQ for BRI, χ2 = 6.27 and 6.29, ps = .012, respectively, and parent report for CRI, χ2 = 11.61, p = .001.

Motivation for schoolwork and effort put into classwork

With regard to motivation, members of the High Improving latent class had significantly lower motivation in 10th grade than the other two classes based on self-report, χ2 = 3.88–8.77, ps < .050, respectively. For teacher-reported motivation, the High Improving class had significantly lower motivation than the other two classes for the CRI, χ2 = 5.05 and 7.63, p = .025 and .006, than the Moderate Stable class for the BRI, χ2 = 12.20, p < .001; groups did not differ based on the ERI for teacher-reported motivation.

Similarly, members of the High Improving latent class self-reported significantly less effort in 10th grade than the Low Improving latent class for BRI, χ2 = 6.67, p = .010, than the other two latent classes for ERI and CRI, χ2 = 3.96–13.09, ps < .047. For teacher-reported effort, members of the High Improving latent class were significantly lower than the Moderate Stable class for BRI and CRI, χ2 = 7.82 and 7.33, p = .005 and .007, respectively; they did not differ for ERI.

Work quality and percentage of assignments turned in

With regard to work quality, members of the High Improving latent class had significantly poorer quality work in 10th grade than the Low Stable/Low Improving class based on self-report for BRI and ERI, χ2 = 6.19 and 8.82, p = .013 and .003, than both classes for CRI, χ2 = 8.23 and 20.22, p = .004 and p < .001. Similarly, for teacher-reported work quality, the High Improving latent class had significantly lower quality work in 10th grade than the Moderate Stable class for BRI, χ2 = 13.51, p < .001, than both classes for the CRI, χ2 = 4.93 and 15.29, p = .026 and p < .001; they were unrelated to ERI class membership for self-reported work quality.

Members of the High Improving latent class turned in significantly fewer assignments relative to members of the other two latent classes for BRI, χ2 = 6.40 and 7.17, p = .011 and .007, and CRI, χ2 = 15.00 and 16.53, ps < .001. They were unrelated to ERI class membership for teacher-reported work quality.

Grade point average

Finally, when considering GPA in 10th grade, members of the High Improving latent class had significantly lower GPAs on average relative to the other two latent classes for BRI, ERI, and CRI, χ2 = 17.74–67.79, ps < .001. Additionally, members of the Moderate Stable latent class had significantly lower GPAs on average than members of the Low Stable latent class for CRI, χ2 = 4.68, p = .030.

Discussion

This study sought to examine trajectories of EF abilities (measured across behavioral, emotional, and cognitive domains) during adolescence, factors that predicted these differential trajectories, and the association of these trajectories with academic outcomes in a sample of adolescents with and without ADHD. The results of this study identified three distinct trajectories of EF development across behavioral, emotional, and cognitive domains from early to mid-adolescence. Consistent with normative development (Nyongesa et al., Reference Nyongesa, Ssewanyana, Mutua, Chongwo, Scerif, Newton and Abubakar2019), the first two trajectories include adolescents who display low levels of EF difficulties (within normal limits) that remain stable or slightly improve across adolescence and adolescents who display moderate levels of EF difficulties (within normal limits) that remain stable across adolescence. The third trajectory included adolescents who demonstrated high EF difficulties (indicating clinical or subclinical concern) that improve across adolescence. Furthermore, these results suggest that factors such as adolescent ADHD status, parent ADHD, and parent EF ability predict risk for such ongoing deficits, as indicated by membership in the High Improving trajectory. Further, adolescents in the High Improving group (showing clinical EF deficits in 8th grade but no longer displaying clinical or subclinical deficits by 10th grade) still had significantly lower GPAs and poorer parent, teacher, and self-reported academic outcomes in 10th grade relative to adolescents who never displayed clinical deficits. This underscores the importance of intervening early as well as continuing to support these adolescents even after their EF difficulties are in the normal limits range. In contrast, adolescents without ADHD, and those with parents without ADHD and with low EF difficulties (within normal limits), were more likely to fall in the Low Stable/Improving or Moderate Stable trajectories and to have better academic outcomes. Strengths of this study include having a subsample of adolescents diagnosed with ADHD, use of a multi-informant and multi-method assessment of academic outcomes (including use of daily assessment from teachers and adolescents), and assessing EF abilities over three timepoints throughout the adolescent developmental period. These findings and their clinical implications are discussed below.

Variability in and predictors of EF abilities during adolescence

Results of the present study support and extend the limited prior research examining trajectories of EF abilities across adolescence (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Loya and Hinshaw2013; Qian et al., Reference Qian, Shuai, Chan, Qian and Wang2013). Specifically, prior research on the development of EF abilities across adolescence suggests that, on average, all aspects of EF improve during the adolescent developmental period (e.g., Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010; Boelema et al., Reference Boelema, Harakeh, Ormel, Hartman, Vollebergh and van Zandvoort2014). Although understanding development on average is important, it is critical to identify individual variation in such trajectories to identify at-risk youth and intervene accordingly. Consistent with this literature, we found that, on average, adolescents in our sample showed improvements in EF abilities across mid-adolescence. However, for adolescents who exhibit clinical deficits in EF during early adolescence, they either continued to display EF difficulties in the subclinical range (CRI) or just below the subclinical range (ERI and BRI) in mid-adolescence. Additionally, visual inspection of the study data revealed that a small percentage (14%) of adolescents who displayed clinical deficits in EF in 8th grade continued to display clinical levels of EF deficits in 10th grade, but these adolescents were not captured as a separate class due to the relatively small sample size in the current study. These findings underscore the importance of examining individual developmental pathways rather than change among a group on average, but also suggest that at-risk youth can be identified early given the high stability of difficulty in this at-risk subsample. As such, youth who score in the clinical range of EF difficulties in middle school should be referred for intervention, given evidence that they are likely to stay high in EF deficits into high school.

Accordingly, understanding risk factors for which youth fall into this subsample is critical. Prior research has linked individual and family factors such as IQ and SES to the development of EF abilities (e.g., Hackman et al., Reference Hackman, Gallop, Evans and Farah2015, Hughes & Ensor, Reference Hughes and Ensor2009). Our study sought to examine individual and family factors, specifically parent ADHD and EF and adolescent ADHD status that are more readily addressed via intervention. Our findings support and add to the limited research with community samples linking parent EF and ADHD status to adolescent EF (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008; Jester et al., Reference Jester, Nigg, Puttler, Long, Fitzgerald and Zucker2009), which found adolescent EF to be significantly associated with parent EF/genetic factors, independent of IQ between the ages of 12–17 years using performance-based measures of EF. However, in our sample, this finding was only supported for the BRI and CRI, as difficulties in these two indices map on more directly with deficits in parent EF abilities. Given emerging evidence that behavioral ratings of EF are ecologically valid and offer distinct, complementary information to performance-based measures (Nyongesa et al., Reference Nyongesa, Ssewanyana, Mutua, Chongwo, Scerif, Newton and Abubakar2019), our examination of these relations using behavioral ratings of parent and adolescent EF is an important addition to the literature. Additionally, this is the first study to examine this association in adolescents with ADHD, a clinical sample characterized by EF difficulties. Our findings indicate that parents who have high levels of EF difficulties are more likely to have adolescents who continue to display high levels of EF deficits throughout adolescence, as well as adolescents with moderate deficits that improve during adolescence. Given this, it will be important for future research to examine how assessing or targeting parent EF abilities may lead to improvements in youth EF abilities over time. Based on our current findings, adding components to help parents address their own potential difficulties with aspects of EF, such as available in current adult interventions (e.g., Hepark et al., Reference Hepark, Janssen, de Vries, Schoenberg, Donders, Kan and Speckens2019; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, McIntyre, English, Dennis, Beckham and Kollins2017), may promote accelerated attainment of positive clinical outcomes. Further, interventions could have parents and adolescents work together on their own EF abilities, where parents could model engagement in the intervention.

Similarly, we found that youth with ADHD were significantly more likely to display High Improving EF difficulties (and less likely to display Low Stable/Improving or Moderate Stable EF difficulties) relative to adolescents without ADHD. This is consistent with prior research suggesting a developmental lag in EF skills and significant EF difficulties on average among youth with ADHD (e.g., Willcutt et al., Reference Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington2005). Although ADHD is a neurodevelopmental disorder, interventions can help reduce the frequency of symptom occurrence and reduce impairment associated with the disorder (see Evans et al., Reference Evans, Owens, Wymbs and Ray2018), with medication being better at reducing symptoms but behavior therapy leading to the greatest impact on impairment (Sibley et al., Reference Sibley, Kuriyan, Evans, Waxmonsky and Smith2014). In particular, interventions targeting organization, time management, and planning difficulties among adolescents with ADHD may prove particularly useful in addressing impairment and provide opportunities to rehearse skills that will improve EF abilities with time.

Association between EF trajectories and academic outcomes

This study also adds to the literature by examining associations among various developmental trajectories and academic outcomes. Membership in the High Improving and, to a lesser extent, the Moderate Stable EF trajectories was significantly associated with worse academic outcomes based on an objective measure (i.e., GPA) and parent, teacher, and adolescent report, including poorer homework performance, work quality, motivation, and effort, and less teacher-reported assignments turned in. These findings are consistent with prior research showing the robust association between EF abilities and academic performance (e.g., Allan et al., Reference Allan, Hume, Allan, Farrington and Lonigan2014; Zelazo et al., Reference Zelazo, Blair and Willoughby2016), and importantly, extend these findings to daily adolescent and teacher ratings of academic performance during the school week.

Collectively, these findings suggest that higher levels of EF deficits in early adolescence may hinder implementation of adaptive self-regulatory strategies that are critical for academic success during the transition from middle school to high school. These findings are noteworthy as academic demands increase rapidly during middle school including greater demands on organizational and planning abilities over extended periods of time with less external supports (Eccles, Reference Eccles, Learner and Steinberg2004; Rudolph et al., Reference Rudolph, Lambert, Clark and Kurlakowsky2001), which requires the development of better self-regulation in response (Burnett et al., Reference Burnett, Scratch and Anderson2013). These demands are only heightened during high school with more challenging courses, higher levels of independence for completing assignments, large exams, and long-term projects occurring throughout the semester (Jacobson et al., Reference Jacobson, Williford and Pianta2011). As such, adaptation to the academic demands of middle and then high school is significantly impacted by children’s performance-based EF skills (Jacobson et al., Reference Jacobson, Williford and Pianta2011; Langberg et al., Reference Langberg, Dvorsky and Evans2013). Accordingly, and as these trajectories indicate, it is highly important to assess for and intervene on EF difficulties during middle school to improve the trajectory of academic performance in the critical transition from middle to high school. Since EF has been shown to predict high school completion and college performance and graduation (Dvorsky & Langberg, Reference Dvorsky and Langberg2019; McClelland et al., Reference McClelland, Acock, Piccinin, Rhea and Stallings2013; Vitaro et al., Reference Vitaro, Brendgen, Larose and Trembaly2005), the High Improving trajectory may be at a greater risk for negative outcomes (e.g., high school dropout) if such EF difficulties are not addressed. Taken together, our results indicate that adolescents with subclinical and clinical levels of EF deficits may benefit from targeted interventions for improving EF deficits during the middle school years.

Clinical implications

Given our finding that youth with ADHD were more likely to fall in the High Improving trajectory, with membership in these being associated with poorer academic outcomes, existing skills-based interventions that provide repeated opportunities to improve EF abilities may offer a promising direction for improving academic outcomes for these at-risk adolescents. Interventions for students with ADHD exist at both the middle school (e.g., Langberg et al., Reference Langberg, Dvorsky, Molitor, Bourchtein, Eddy, Smith, Oddo and Eadeh2018) and high school (e.g., Sibley, Reference Sibley2016) level, and have resulted in improvements in objective (i.e., GPA) and parent/teacher-reported outcomes (e.g., Breaux et al., Reference Breaux, Langberg, Molitor, Dvorsky, Bourchtein, Smith and Green2019). These interventions target EF deficits by providing the necessary strategies to meet the increasing academic demands that adolescents face during the transition to middle and high school (e.g., breaking larger assignments into chunks and plan for when to complete the various components). Moreover, some of these interventions also work with parents to support adolescents in acquiring these strategies and skills, such as by providing incentives for meeting targeted goals, and discussing strategies for better organization. Given our finding that parent EF ability predicted differential EF trajectories, such interventions could also add components specifically targeting parent EF to ensure that parents have the necessary tools to support their adolescents. Alternatively, there are standalone interventions targeting EF abilities in adults (e.g., Hepark et al., Reference Hepark, Janssen, de Vries, Schoenberg, Donders, Kan and Speckens2019; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, McIntyre, English, Dennis, Beckham and Kollins2017). Similarly, the impact of such adult-focused interventions on parenting behaviors and youth EF abilities is an important area for future research.

Finally, our findings highlight the clinical utility and ecological validity of using parent-reported behavioral ratings to capture impairment and change in adolescent EF over time. Parent-reported behavioral ratings may provide an efficient and effective means of early identification and intervention for adolescents with subclinical and clinical levels of EF deficits, and may protect these at-risk adolescents from worse academic outcomes. It will be critical for future research to explore whether teacher-reported ratings of EF deficits in adolescents can prove equally effective, given that such ratings would be easier for schools to implement.

Limitations

The findings and clinical implications of this study should be interpreted within the context of several limitations. First, several questions on the BRIEF-2 ask about abilities that directly overlap with DSM-5 criteria for mental health disorders (including ADHD). It is important to acknowledge that this might potentially limit our findings and therefore, an examination of the study aims which utilizes performance- and report-based measures of EF in future research is warranted. Second, despite a strength of the study being our assessment of adolescent EF abilities across three timepoints, and during the transition from middle to high school, our knowledge about changes in EF abilities is somewhat limited by the unequal spacing across time points (i.e., two timepoints in 8th grade, none in 9th grade, and one in 10th grade). In particular, 9th grade is when many students enter high school, so understanding predictors of which students are and are not able to rise to the increased challenges during this time would be beneficial. Third, parent EF was measured at our second time point; however, since EF abilities are relatively stable during middle adulthood (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Altamirano, Corley, Young, Rhea and Hewitt2016) this is less of a concern. Fourth, since our data included measures of adolescent EF abilities at three timepoints, only linear models could be estimated. Future research should include a larger number of timepoints to capture a potentially more accurate representation of trajectories of EF development across adolescence (e.g., quadratic). Finally, our sample was predominately white and from a middle to high SES. Given that EF abilities differ based on SES (e.g., Last et al., Reference Last, Lawson, Breiner, Steinberg and Farah2018), and that EF abilities explain some of the socioeconomic and racial differences in academic achievement (Nesbitt et al., Reference Nesbitt, Baker-Ward and Willoughby2013), it is critical for future research to include more diverse samples.

Conclusion

This study provides important insight into EF development across adolescence in a sample of adolescents with and without ADHD. Findings underscore that although the majority of youth experience improvements in EF abilities across the adolescent developmental period, youth who exhibit clinically significant deficits in early adolescence continue to display subclinical or just below subclinical deficits, with adolescents with ADHD and parents with ADHD and poor EF abilities being more at-risk for displaying this at-risk developmental trajectory. Further, findings indicate that clinical EF difficulties in 8th grade are associated with worse academic outcomes in 10th grade, even for adolescents who improve in EF abilities from 8th to 10th grade. Together these findings highlight that intervention efforts to target EF abilities of adolescents, particularly with ADHD, and their parents could help reduce risk for negative academic outcomes.

Funding statement

This research was supported by the Institute of Education Science (IES), U.S. Department of Education (grant R305A160126), PI: Joshua M. Langberg and Stephen P. Becker.

Competing interests

None.

References

Allan, N. P., Hume, L. E., Allan, D. M., Farrington, A. L., & Lonigan, C. J. (2014). Relations between inhibitory control and the development of academic skills in preschool and kindergarten: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 50(10), 23682379. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037493 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ardila, A., Pineda, D., & Rosselli, M. (2000). Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15(1), 3136. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(98)00159-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baars, M. A., Nije Bijvank, M., Tonnaer, G. H., & Jolles, J. (2015). Self-report measures of executive functioning are a determinant of academic performance in first-year students at a university of applied sciences. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1131. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01131 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (2011). Barkley deficits in executive functioning scale (BDEFS). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A., & Murphy, K. R. (2011). The nature of executive function (EF) deficits in daily life activities in adults with ADHD and their relationship to performance on EF tests. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33(2), 137158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-011-9217-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, I., Slobodin, O., Aboud, M., Melamed, J., & Cassuto, H. (2013). Maturational delay in ADHD: Evidence from CPT. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 691. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00691 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326339. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 16411660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F., Morgan, C. L., & Faraone, S. V. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 72(5), 757. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.757 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bierman, K. L., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Blair, C., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2008). Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 821843. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000394 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boelema, S. R., Harakeh, Z., Ormel, J., Hartman, C. A., Vollebergh, W. A., & van Zandvoort, M. J. (2014). Executive functioning shows differential maturation from early to late adolescence: Longitudinal findings from a TRAILS study. Neuropsychology, 28(2), 177187. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breaux, R. P., Langberg, J. M., Molitor, S. J., Dvorsky, M. R., Bourchtein, E., Smith, Z. R., & Green, C. D. (2019). Predictors and trajectories of response to the Homework, Organization, and Planning Skills (HOPS) intervention for adolescents with ADHD. Behavior Therapy, 50(1), 140154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2018.04.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridgett, D. J., Oddi, K. B., Laake, L. M., Murdock, K. W., & Bachmann, M. N. (2013). Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: Effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity. Emotion, 13(1), 4763. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029536 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brieant, A., King-Casas, B., & Kim-Spoon, J. (2022). Transactional relations between developmental trajectories of executive functioning and internalizing and externalizing symptomatology in adolescence. Development and psychopathology, 34(1), 213224. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273293. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN1903_3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnett, A. C., Scratch, S. E., & Anderson, P. J. (2013). Executive function outcome in preterm adolescents. Early Human Development, 89(4), 215220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.01.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Celeux, G., & Soromenho, G. (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. Journal of Classification, 13(2), 195212. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01246098 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuevas, K., Deater-Deckard, K., Kim-Spoon, J., Watson, A. J., Morasch, K. C., & Bell, M. A. (2014). What’s mom got to do with it? Contributions of maternal executive function and caregiving to the development of executive function across early childhood. Developmental Science, 17(2), 224238. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12073 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K. (2014). Family matters: Intergenerational and interpersonal processes of executive function and attentive behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 230236. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414531597 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dvorsky, M. R., & Langberg, J. M. (2019). Predicting impairment in college students with ADHD: The role of executive functions. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(13), 16241636. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054714548037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccles, J. S. (2004). Schools, academic motivation, and stage-environment fit. In Learner, R. M., & Steinberg, L. (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 125154). John Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780471726746.ch5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, S. W., Owens, J. S., Wymbs, B. T., & Ray, A. R. (2018). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 47(2), 157198. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2017.1390757 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J., & Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Development Perspectives, 8(4), 258264. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12095 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, H. J., Brunsdon, V. E., & Bradford, E. E. (2021). The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-80866-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Altamirano, L. J., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., Rhea, S. A., & Hewitt, J. K. (2016). Stability and change in executive function abilities from late adolescence to early adulthood: A longitudinal twin study. Developmental Psychology, 52(2), 326340. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000075 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 201225. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fu, G. H., Chen, W., Li, H. M., Wang, Y. F., Liu, L., & Qian, Q. J. (2021). A potential association of RNF219-AS1 with ADHD: Evidence from categorical analysis of clinical phenotypes and from quantitative exploration of executive function and white matter microstructure endophenotypes. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 27(5), 603616. https://doi.org/10.1111/cns.13629 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2015). BRIEF: Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Gordon, C. T., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2020). Executive functions in girls with and without childhood ADHD followed through emerging adulthood: Developmental trajectories. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 49(4), 509523. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2019.1602840 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray-Burrows, K., Taylor, N., O’Connor, D., Sutherland, E., Stoet, G., & Conner, M. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the executive function-health behaviour relationship. Health Psychology and Behavioral Medicine, 7(1), 253268. https://doi.org/10.1080/21642850.2019.1637740 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., Gallop, R., Evans, G. W., & Farah, M. J. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science, 18(5), 686702. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12246 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hepark, S., Janssen, L., de Vries, A., Schoenberg, P. L., Donders, R., Kan, C. C., & Speckens, A. E. (2019). The efficacy of adapted MBCT on core symptoms and executive functioning in adults with ADHD: A preliminary randomized controlled trial. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(4), 351362. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054715613587 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C. H., & Ensor, R. A. (2009). How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2009(123), 3550. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.234 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, L. A., Williford, A. P., & Pianta, R. C. (2011). The role of executive function in children’s competent adjustment to middle school. Child Neuropsychology, 17(3), 255280. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.535654 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, P. S., Hoagwood, K. E., Roper, M., Arnold, L. E., Odbert, C., Crowe, M., Molina, B. S. G., Hechtman, L., Hinshaw, S. P., Hoza, B., Newcorn, J., Swanson, J., & Wells, K. (2004). The services for children and adolescents-parent interview: Development and performance characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(11), 13341344. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000139557.16830.4e CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jester, J. M., Nigg, J. T., Puttler, L. I., Long, J. C., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Zucker, R. A. (2009). Intergenerational transmission of neuropsychological executive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 70(1), 145153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joyner, K. B., Silver, C. H., & Stavinoha, P. L. (2009). Relationship between parenting stress and ratings of executive functioning in children with ADHD. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27(6), 452464. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282909333945 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, T., & Wickrama, K. A. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 302317. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00054.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langberg, J. M., Dvorsky, M. R., & Evans, S. W. (2013). What specific facets of executive function are associated with academic functioning in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(7), 11451159. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9750-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langberg, J. M., Dvorsky, M. R., Molitor, S. J., Bourchtein, E., Eddy, L. D., Smith, Z. R., Oddo, L. E., & Eadeh, H. M. (2018). Overcoming the research-to-practice gap: A randomized trial with two brief homework and organization interventions for students with ADHD as implemented by school mental health providers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86(1), 39. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000265 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Last, B. S., Lawson, G. M., Breiner, K., Steinberg, L., & Farah, M. J. (2018). Childhood socioeconomic status and executive function in childhood and beyond. PloS One, 13(8), e0202964. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0202964 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubke, G., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Performance of factor mixture models as a function of model size, covariate effects, and class-specific parameters. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(1), 2647. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510709336735 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luciana, M. (2016). Executive function in adolescence: A commentary on regulatory control and depression in adolescents: Findings from neuroimaging and neuropsychological research. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 45(1), 8489. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1123638 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, Y., Weibman, D., Halperin, J. M., & Li, X. (2019). A review of heterogeneity in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 42. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martel, M., Nikolas, M., & Nigg, J. T. (2007). Executive function in adolescents with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(11), 14371444. https://doi.org/10.1097/chi.0b013e31814cf953 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzocco, M. M., & Kover, S. T. (2007). A longitudinal assessment of executive function skills and their association with math performance. Child Neuropsychology, 13(1), 1845. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040600611346 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., Piccinin, A., Rhea, S. A., & Stallings, M. C. (2013). Relations between preschool attention span-persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2), 314324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.07.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, M., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2010). Does childhood executive function predict adolescent functional outcomes in girls with ADHD? Journal of abnormal child psychology, 38, 315326. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9369-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, M., Loya, F., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2013). Executive functions in girls with and without childhood ADHD: Developmental trajectories and associations with symptom change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(9), 10051015. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12074 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, M., Nevado-Montenegro, A. J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2012). Childhood executive function continues to predict outcomes in young adult females with and without childhood-diagnosed ADHD. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 40, 657668. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9599-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, J. T., McIntyre, E. M., English, J. S., Dennis, M. F., Beckham, J. C., & Kollins, S. H. (2017). A pilot trial of mindfulness meditation training for ADHD in adulthood: Impact on core symptoms, executive functioning, and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(13), 11051120. https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547135133 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, F. J., Ponitz, C. C., & McClelland, M. M. (2010). Self-regulation and academic achievement in the transition to school. In Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. (Eds.), Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 203224). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/12059-011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, B. (2003). Statistical and substantive checking in growth mixture modeling: Comment on Bauer and Curran (2003). Psychological Methods, 8(3), 369377. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.369 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. K. (2000). Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: Growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(6), 882891. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb02070.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesbitt, K. T., Baker-Ward, L., & Willoughby, M. T. (2013). Executive function mediates socio-economic and racial differences in early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(4), 774783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.07.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyongesa, M. K., Ssewanyana, D., Mutua, A. M., Chongwo, E., Scerif, G., Newton, C. R., & Abubakar, A. (2019). Assessing executive functions in adolescence: A scoping review of existing measures and their psychometric robustness. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 311. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00311 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C. C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., Dardick, W., & Tao, S. (2018). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 4876. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000124 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M., & Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(4), 455473. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000079 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, T. J., Dombrowski, S. C., Watkins, M. W., Mautone, J. A., & Eagle, J. W. (2007). Assessing children’s homework performance: Development of multi-dimensional, multi-informant rating scales. Journal of School Psychology, 45(3), 333348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2007.02.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qian, Y., Shuai, L., Chan, R. C., Qian, Q. J., & Wang, Y. (2013). The developmental trajectories of executive function of children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(5), 14341445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ram, N., & Grimm, K. J. (2009). Methods and measures: Growth mixture modeling: A method for identifying differences in longitudinal change among unobserved groups. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(6), 565576. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025409343765 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudolph, K. D., Lambert, S. F., Clark, A. G., & Kurlakowsky, K. D. (2001). Negotiating the transition to middle school: The role of self-regulatory processes. Child Development, 72(3), 929946. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00325 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schroeder, V. M., & Kelley, M. L. (2009). Associations between family environment, parenting practices, and executive functioning of children with and without ADHD. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18(2), 227235. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-008-9223-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanmugan, S., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2016). Neural markers of the development of executive function: Relevance for education. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 713. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sibley, M. H. (2016). Parent-teen therapy for executive function deficits and ADHD: Building skills and motivation. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sibley, M. H., Kuriyan, A. B., Evans, S. W., Waxmonsky, J. G., & Smith, B. H. (2014). Pharmacological and psychosocial treatments for adolescents with ADHD: An updated systematic review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(3), 218232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.02.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverstein, M. J., Faraone, S. V., Leon, T. L., Biederman, J., Spencer, T. J., & Adler, L. A. (2020). The relationship between executive function deficits and DSM-5-defined ADHD symptoms. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24(1), 4151. https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547188043 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiegel, J. A., Goodrich, J. M., Morris, B. M., Osborne, C. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2021). Relations between executive functions and academic outcomes in elementary school children: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 147(4), 329351. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000322 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tein, J. Y., Coxe, S., & Cham, H. (2013). Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 20(4), 640657. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2013.824781 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorell, L. B. (2007). Do delay aversion and executive function deficits make distinct contributions to the functional impact of ADHD symptoms? A study of early academic skill deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(11), 10611070. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01777.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonkin, R. S. (2001). Early intervention with an adolescent twist. Paediatrics & Child Health, 6(7), 424428. https://doi.org/10.1093/pch/6.7.424 Google ScholarPubMed
Torgalsbøen, B. R., Zeiner, P., & Øie, M. G. (2021). Pre-attention and working memory in ADHD: A 25-year follow-up study. Journal of Attention Disorders, 25(7), 895905. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054719879491 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ursache, A., & Noble, K. G. (2016). Socioeconomic status, white matter, and executive function in children. Brain and Behavior, 6(10), e00531. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.531 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermunt, J. K. (2010). Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Political Analysis, 18(4), 450469. https://doi.org/10.1093/pan/mpq025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Larose, S., & Trembaly, R. E. (2005). Kindergarten disruptive behaviors, protective factors, and educational achievement by early adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(4), 617629. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.4.617 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (2nd ed.). Pearson.Google Scholar
Weller, E. B., Fristad, M. A., Weller, R. A., & Rooney, M. T. (1999). Children’s interview for psychiatric syndromes: ChIPS. American Psychiatric Pub.Google Scholar
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 13361346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., Blair, C. B., & Willoughby, M. T. (2016). Executive function: Implications for education (NCER 2017-2000). National Center for Education Research; Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of demographic variables and adolescent and parent EF deficits across ADHD and comparison groups

Figure 1

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and correlations between study variables

Figure 2

Figure 1. (a) Three-class model for behavior regulation index trajectories. (b) Three-class model for emotion regulation index trajectories. (c) Three-class model for cognitive regulation index trajectories. Note. Executive function difficulties are reported as T-scores; values within normal limits range from 40 to 60, T-scores between 60 and 65 are considered borderline clinical, and T-scores of 65 or above indicate clinical concern.

Figure 3

Table 3. Model fit statistics for unconditional executive function trajectories models

Figure 4

Table 4. Means and standard errors for academic outcomes of three latent classes for the behavior regulation index

Figure 5

Table 5. Means and standard errors for academic outcomes of three latent classes for the emotion regulation index

Figure 6

Table 6. Means and standard errors for academic outcomes of three latent classes for the cognitive regulation index