Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T17:24:22.006Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A person-centered approach to resilience and vulnerability in emerging adulthood: Predictions from parenting and personality in adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 August 2022

Donna A. de Maat*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Nicole Lucassen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Rebecca L. Shiner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Colgate University, Hamilton, NY, USA
Peter Prinzie
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: Donna A. de Maat, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In this person-centered study, we identified different profiles of resilience and vulnerability in emerging adulthood in response to previously experienced stressful life events. Additionally, we examined whether mothers’ and fathers’ parenting and participants’ personality traits in adolescence predicted these profiles. Data from the Flemish Study on Parenting, Personality, and Development (N = 346 families) were used. At T1 (2004; Mage = 11 years), T2 (2007), and T3 (2009), mothers and fathers reported on their parenting and their child’s personality. At T4 (2018; Mage = 25 years), emerging adults retrospectively self-reported the occurrence and impact of 22 stressful life events and rated current behavior problems and subjective well-being. Latent profile analysis revealed three profiles: Competent (71%; low stress, low behavior problems, high subjective well-being), Vulnerable (21%; average stress, high behavior problems, low subjective well-being), and Resilient (9%; high stress, average behavior problems, average subjective well-being). Emerging adults in the Resilient profile had experienced higher levels of maternal positive parenting and were less emotionally stable and conscientious than those in the Competent profile. Furthermore, emerging adults in the Vulnerable profile were less emotionally stable than their peers in the Competent profile. These findings reveal new insights into the heterogeneous patterns of emerging adults’ adaptation following stressful life events.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

The experience of stressful life events in childhood can have long-term adverse effects. Stressful life events are described as discrete experiences, such as the death of a family member, parental separation, or a romantic break up, that are likely to disrupt an individual’s usual activities and cause readjustment (Carlson, Reference Carlson and Michalos2014; Holmes & Rahe, Reference Holmes and Rahe1967). Children exposed to stressful life events are at increased risk for negative developmental outcomes including a variety of emotional and behavioral problems as they enter adolescence and adulthood (e.g., Burt & Masten, Reference Burt, Masten, Grant and Potenza2009; Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Murphy and Prather2019; LeMoult et al., Reference LeMoult, Humphreys, Tracy, Hoffmeister, Ip and Gotlib2020; Luecken & Gress, Reference Luecken, Gress, Reich, Zautra and Hall2010; March-Llanes et al., Reference March-Llanes, Marqués-Feixa, Mezquita, Fañanás and Moya-Higueras2017). Yet, many children growing up in conditions of stress do not experience such negative outcomes and show positive development. These individuals are said to show “resilience” to stress (Masten, Reference Masten2011). Resilience reflects the dynamic process of positive adaptation despite exposure to risk or adversity. Hence, two components should be present to identify resilience in young people: exposure to risk that increases the probability of negative outcomes and relatively positive adaptation that is better than expected, given exposure to the risk being studied (Infurna & Luthar, Reference Infurna and Luthar2018; Masten, Reference Masten2018; Rutter, Reference Rutter2012).

Theoretical frameworks of resilience suggest that both contextual and individual factors may contribute to the relative positive adaptation in the context of stress (e.g., Infurna & Luthar, Reference Infurna and Luthar2018; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016). Resilience theorists emphasize the need to identify specific contextual and individual resources that are potentially modifiable in interventions. One contextual factor that is malleable and that has been considered central to children’s resilience for many decades is parenting (Masten & Palmer, Reference Masten, Palmer and Bornstein2019; Masten, Reference Masten2018). From the start of research on resilience, researchers also recognized the potential importance of children’s personality as an individual resource factor (e.g., Garmezy, Reference Garmezy and Stevenson1985; Masten & Tellegen, Reference Masten and Tellegen2012; Werner & Smith, Reference Werner and Smith1982). Given increasing evidence indicating that personality traits can change both naturally and through intervention (e.g., Borghuis et al., Reference Borghuis, Denissen, Oberski, Sijtsma, Meeus, Branje, Koot and Bleidorn2017; Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Luo, Briley, Chow, Su and Hill2017; Shiner et al., Reference Shiner, Van den Berg, Van Aken, Laceulle, Skinner and Zimmer-Gembeck2021), it is particularly worthwhile to study what specific personality traits are important for building resilience.

Although previous work examined the contributions of parenting and personality characteristics to resilience in childhood and adolescence (e.g., Fritz et al., Reference Fritz, de Graaff, Caisley, Van Harmelen and Wilkinson2018; Masten, Reference Masten2018; Zolkoski & Bullock, Reference Zolkoski and Bullock2012), less is known about how these resource factors are related to resilience in the transition to adulthood, characterized as “emerging adulthood” (Arnett, Reference Arnett2000, Reference Arnett, Zukauskiene and Sugimura2014). Identifying antecedents of resilience in emerging adulthood is essential, as this developmental period is marked by unique challenges and strengths emerging from normative life transitions (Arnett, Reference Arnett2000, Reference Arnett, Zukauskiene and Sugimura2014; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Obradović, Burt, Arnett and Tanner2006). For example, emerging adults begin to establish romantic relationships, engage in the workforce, and explore their identity. The increase in freedom and autonomous functioning during this transition period opens a window of opportunity for positive change, but can also result in adaptational problems, such as mental health problems (Burt & Paysnick, Reference Burt and Paysnick2012; O’Connor et al., Reference O'Connor, Sanson, Toumbourou, Hawkins, Letcher, Williams, Olsson and Arnett2016). Therefore, it is important to identify resource factors that promote positive development in emerging adulthood, especially for those at risk for negative outcomes.

Even though research on the antecedents of resilience in the period of emerging adulthood has accelerated in the past decades (e.g., Chen et al., Reference Chen, Yu, Siliezar, Drage, Dezil, Miller and Brody2020; Donnellan et al., Reference Donnellan, Conger, McAdams and Neppl2009; Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Yu, Sadiq and Brody2021; Farrell et al., Reference Farrell, Simpson, Carlson, Englund and Sung2017; Feldman et al., Reference Feldman, Conger, Burzette and Keyes2018), many questions remain (Leung et al., Reference Leung, Chan and Ho2020; O'Connor et al., Reference O'Connor, Sanson, Toumbourou, Hawkins, Letcher, Williams, Olsson and Arnett2016). First, previous studies mostly focused on either internal, individual assets (e.g., personality traits) or external, contextual resources (e.g., social support) as potential protective factors. Yet, to get a more comprehensive picture of the pathways to adulthood resilience, it is essential to consider both individual and contextual factors, as both can facilitate resilience (Fritz et al., Reference Fritz, de Graaff, Caisley, Van Harmelen and Wilkinson2018; Zolkoski & Bullock, Reference Zolkoski and Bullock2012). Second, research examining family level protective factors such as parenting often only includes mothers’ parenting, whereas many children in Western societies are raised by both a father and a mother (United Nations, 2019). Third, there is limited information on individual patterns of resilience and vulnerability in the context of stress. Originally, variable-centered studies have clarified relations between resource factors and specific domains of adjustment in the context of stress, overlooking individual, naturally occurring patterns of adjustment that can be elucidated by person-centered approaches (Masten, Reference Masten2001). An advantage of a person-centered approach is that it enables the examination of resilience as a multidimensional construct within the individual, rather than through associations between variables within a given sample. This allows researchers to identify subgroups of resilient individuals and to scrutinize what characterizes these particular subgroups (Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013). The available person-centered studies on resilience in emerging adulthood used a priori classifications based on cutoff scores on adversity and adaptation to create subgroups of individuals (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Hubbard, Gest, Tellegen, Garmezy and Ramirez1999, Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004; Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012). Applying these a priori classifications does not allow for identification of unobserved subgroups who may present with heterogeneous patterns of adaptation across multiple developmental domains. Latent profile analysis (LPA) enables the identification of such hidden profiles, and, moreover, can handle missing data and offers model fit statistics (Lanza & Cooper, Reference Lanza and Cooper2016). Hence, person-centered approaches such as LPA provide a useful statistical method for addressing the claim that, in studying resilience, adaptation in one domain of functioning only gains meaning in relation to the individual’s functioning in other domains (Infurna & Luthar, Reference Infurna and Luthar2018; Russotti et al., Reference Russotti, Warmingham, Handley, Rogosch and Cicchetti2020).

In the current person-centered study, we aimed to identify profiles of resilience in emerging adulthood and to examine how these profiles are related to multiple parenting behaviors and personality traits assessed in adolescence. This study may shed more light on how emerging adults adapt after stress exposure and why some emerging adults show more resilience than others, which is beneficial for theory building on the etiology of resilience. In addition, examining the protective role of relatively malleable factors as parenting behaviors and personality traits can provide valuable targets for intervention strategies aimed at promoting resilience in the transition to adulthood.

Parenting and resilience in emerging adulthood

At the contextual level, positive and effective parenting is among the most robust predictors of resilient adaptation in the face of stress (Masten & Palmer, Reference Masten, Palmer and Bornstein2019; Masten, Reference Masten2018). Supportive parent-child relationships, in addition to close relationships with peers, may be an important resource during difficult times in adolescence and emerging adulthood (Burt & Paysnick, Reference Burt and Paysnick2012). By providing warmth, support, and a safe environment to explore emotions, parents may facilitate healthy communication and coping in adolescents, and subsequently support emotional well-being in adulthood (Dumas et al., Reference Dumas, Lawford, Tieu and Pratt2009).

Positive parenting and other potential resources have been differentiated into two broad categories: Promotive factors that generate positive effects independent of the level of risk, and protective factors that only benefit children with high levels of risk, mitigating the effects of risk in an interactive fashion (Masten & Tellegen, Reference Masten and Tellegen2012; Sameroff, Reference Sameroff2000). With respect to the beneficial effects of parenting, empirical findings demonstrate that higher levels of positive parenting (e.g., warmth, support, involvement) and lower levels of negative parenting (e.g., hostility, psychological control, coercive control) during adolescence were generally related to better adjustment in adulthood, including lower levels of anxiety, depression (Yap et al., Reference Yap, Pilkington, Ryan and Jorm2014) and risk-taking behavior (Aquilino & Supple, Reference Aquilino and Supple2001), and higher academic attainment (Loeb et al., Reference Loeb, Kansky, Tan, Costello and Allen2021) and subjective well-being (Aquilino & Supple, Reference Aquilino and Supple2001). Likewise, in a person-centered study by Masten et al. (Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004), emerging adults characterized by resilience (high stress, good adaptation) and by competence (low stress, good adaptation) had received higher levels of positive parenting (i.e., warmth, closeness, structure) in childhood than those characterized by maladjustment (high stress, poor adaptation). In the same longitudinal cohort, it was found that individuals that showed consistently high adaptation from adolescence to adulthood were distinguished by higher levels of childhood positive parenting (Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Burt and Masten2006). These findings suggest that effective parenting in childhood and adolescence can promote positive adaptation in emerging adulthood, regardless of the level of stress exposure.

In addition, parenting can act as a protective factor by buffering the negative impact of adversity on developmental outcomes in emerging adulthood. For example, more nurturant-involved parenting and less angry or hostile parenting in adolescence weakened the link between economic stress and less competent functioning in emerging adulthood (Conger & Conger, Reference Conger and Conger2002; Masarik & Conger, Reference Masarik and Conger2017). Likewise, parental positivity and positive parenting behaviors fostered resilience among emerging adults in times of economic stress (Neppl et al., Reference Neppl, Jeon, Schofield and Donnellan2015). Positive parenting in childhood has also been associated with increased resilience in adulthood following exposure to childhood maltreatment, protecting these at-risk children from mental health problems (Collishaw et al., Reference Collishaw, Pickles, Messer, Rutter, Shearer and Maughan2007; Lind et al., Reference Lind, Brown, Sheerin, York, Myers, Kendler and Amstadter2018). Such findings imply that positive, supportive parenting may also act as a protective factor for emerging adults exposed to high levels of stress or adversity.

Surprisingly, research on the role of parenting in resilience often solely examined parenting behaviors of mothers (Cabrera et al., Reference Cabrera, Hennigar, Alonso, McDorman and Reich2021). When fathers were included in research, mothers’ and fathers’ parenting behaviors were either not analyzed independently (e.g., Collishaw et al., Reference Collishaw, Pickles, Messer, Rutter, Shearer and Maughan2007; Lind et al., Reference Lind, Brown, Sheerin, York, Myers, Kendler and Amstadter2018) or not included simultaneously in one model (e.g., Conger & Conger, Reference Conger and Conger2002; with the exception of Neppl et al., Reference Neppl, Jeon, Schofield and Donnellan2015). Yet, evidence clearly indicates that fathers are also important to children’s problem behavior and positive outcomes (e.g., Day & Padilla-Walker, Reference Day and Padilla-Walker2009; Salgado et al., Reference Salgado, González and Yáñez2021) and that fathers’ positive parenting complements the effects of mothers’ parenting (e.g., Cabrera et al., Reference Cabrera, Hennigar, Alonso, McDorman and Reich2021; Lansford et al., Reference Lansford, Laird, Pettit, Bates and Dodge2014). Given the independent contributions of mothers and fathers to their children’s development, it is essential to consider both parents when analyzing links between parenting and positive adaptation in emerging adulthood.

Personality and resilience in emerging adulthood

At the individual level, children’s personality is one of the most well-studied child characteristics that can promote positive outcomes and resilience. Personality refers to an individual’s typical patterns of thinking, feeling, and behaving and can be conceptualized according to the comprehensive Big Five framework (Caspi & Shiner, Reference Caspi, Shiner, Lerner, Eisenberg and Damon2006; McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae, Costa, Pervin and John1999). Specifically, the Big Five personality traits are extraversion (i.e., sociability, expressivity), agreeableness (i.e., empathy, consideration of other’s needs), conscientiousness (i.e., impulse control, planfulness), emotional stability (vs. neuroticism, i.e., low negative emotionality and high self-esteem), and openness to experience (i.e., intellect, autonomy, imagination).

Empirical findings indicate that children’s personality traits can directly shape positive development. For example, higher levels of Big Five personality traits in childhood and adolescence have been related to positive outcomes in adulthood, such as higher subjective well-being (Anglim et al., Reference Anglim, Horwood, Smillie, Marrero and Wood2020; Gale et al., Reference Gale, Booth, Mõttus, Kuh and Deary2013), self-efficacy (Deutz et al., Reference Deutz, van Eldik, Over de Vest, Ringoot, de Haan and Prinzie2021), and academic and social competence (Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012) as well as lower internalizing and externalizing problems (Van Eldik et al., Reference Van Eldik, de Haan, Arends and Prinzie2020). In person-centered analyses (Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012), emerging adults characterized by resilience (high stress, good adaptation) showed higher childhood conscientiousness, agreeableness, openness, and emotional stability than those characterized by maladjustment (high stress, poor adaptation). Yet, personality traits did not differ between the Resilient and the Competent group (low stress, good adaptation), suggesting that personality might be a promotive factor, with positive effects on development independent of the level of adversity.

Additionally, children’s personality may counteract the impact of adversity on developmental outcomes. That is, personality traits may influence the reactions and support that children evoke from others, their interpretation of negative life experiences, and their ability to cope with stress (Connor-Smith & Flachsbart, Reference Connor-Smith and Flachsbart2007; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Kratsiotis, Niven and Holman2020; Shiner et al., Reference Shiner, Van den Berg, Van Aken, Laceulle, Skinner and Zimmer-Gembeck2021). Although research on personality and resilience in emerging adulthood is scarce, the available studies showed that higher levels of childhood conscientiousness and agreeableness buffered the link between childhood adversity and poor adaptation in emerging adulthood (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Oshri and Kwon2015; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Obradović, Burt, Arnett and Tanner2006; Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012). Hence, children’s personality traits may function as protective factors in the face of stress, contributing to resilience over time.

The current study

In the current study, we aimed to (1) identify different profiles of resilience and vulnerability in response to stressful life events among emerging adults; and (2) examine to what extent the identified profiles were related to contextual factors (i.e., mothers’ and fathers’ positive and negative parenting) and individual factors (i.e., personality) assessed in adolescence. We extended research on resilience in several ways. First, in contrast to most studies that relied on cross-sectional or short-term longitudinal designs to investigate positive adaptation in emerging adulthood, we employed a long-term longitudinal, prospective design to identify pathways to resilience over time. Specifically, we examined whether parenting and personality in adolescence (M ages = 11–16 years) predicted profiles of adaptation in emerging adulthood (M age = 25 years). More insight into longitudinal predictors of positive adaptation in emerging adulthood can offer valuable insights for intervention strategies to promote positive development and resilience. Second, we examined the role of both mothers’ and fathers’ parenting as resource factors for resilience to better match the reality of most children’s lives, expanding previous work in which often only mothers were included. Third, we used a multi-informant design, including parent-report of adolescents’ personality traits and parenting behaviors in combination with emerging adults’ self-report of stress and (mal)adaptation, to avoid shared rater bias. Fourth, we focused on a wide range of outcomes, since broad and multifaceted measures better approximate the general notion of resilience than do narrower, unidimensional outcomes (Infurna & Luthar, Reference Infurna and Luthar2018; Klika & Herrenkohl, Reference Klika and Herrenkohl2013) and resilience may depend on the outcome studied (Fergus & Zimmerman, Reference Fergus and Zimmerman2005; Zolkoski & Bullock, Reference Zolkoski and Bullock2012). In the current study, we examined both adaptive and maladaptive outcomes in emerging adulthood, in contrast to previous person-centered resilience studies that included either maladaptive (Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013) or adaptive outcomes (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Hubbard, Gest, Tellegen, Garmezy and Ramirez1999, Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004; Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012) or a single indicator of global health (Moreno et al., Reference Moreno, García-Moya, Rivera and Ramos2016). More specifically, we examined subjective well-being (i.e., life satisfaction and positive affect) as an indicator of emerging adults’ adaptation. This fits previous literature in which positive adaptation in emerging adulthood is described as positive social and psychological adjustment, with life satisfaction as one of its key dimensions (Hawkins et al., Reference Hawkins, Letcher, Sanson, Smart and Toumbourou2009; O'Connor et al., Reference O'Connor, Sanson, Toumbourou, Hawkins, Letcher, Williams, Olsson and Arnett2016). The transition to adulthood is characterized by potential for both positive development and problem outcomes such as risk behaviors and mental health problems (Arnett et al., Reference Arnett, Zukauskiene and Sugimura2014; Smart & Sanson, Reference Smart and Sanson2005). Consequently, researchers on emerging adulthood emphasize that it is important to study adaptive outcomes in combination with maladaptive outcomes, such as depression, alcohol abuse, or delinquent behavior (O’Connor et al., Reference O’Connor, Sanson, Hawkins, Olsson, Frydenberg, Toumbourou and Letcher2012, Reference O'Connor, Sanson, Toumbourou, Hawkins, Letcher, Williams, Olsson and Arnett2016; Oesterle et al., Reference Oesterle, Hill, Hawkins and Abbott2008). Therefore, we additionally examined multiple types of behavior problems (i.e., anxious/depressed behavior, withdrawn behavior, somatic complaints, aggressive behavior, rule-breaking behavior, and intrusive behavior) as indicators of maladaptation. As evidence suggests that adaptation and maladaptation in emerging adulthood are separate but correlated constructs (e.g., O’Connor et al., Reference O’Connor, Sanson, Hawkins, Olsson, Frydenberg, Toumbourou and Letcher2012, Reference O'Connor, Sanson, Toumbourou, Hawkins, Letcher, Williams, Olsson and Arnett2016), it is essential to consider both to obtain a more complete picture of emerging adults’ functioning.

With respect to aim 1, we expected to identify multiple profiles of resilience and vulnerability among emerging adults based on indicators of stressful life events, behavior problems, and subjective well-being. More specifically, in line with previous person-centered studies (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Hubbard, Gest, Tellegen, Garmezy and Ramirez1999, Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004; Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013; Moreno et al., Reference Moreno, García-Moya, Rivera and Ramos2016; Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012) we hypothesized that there would be at least a profile characterized by resilience (high stress, good adaptation), a profile characterized by maladjustment (high stress, poor adaptation), and a profile characterized by competence (low stress, good adaptation). However, no hypotheses regarding the exact number or type of profiles were posited, given that previous studies have not established the profiles using LPA. Regarding aim 2, we hypothesized that emerging adults in profiles characterized by resilience (high stress, good adaptation) and by competence (low stress, good adaptation) experienced higher levels of positive parenting and lower levels of negative parenting by mothers and fathers in adolescence than emerging adults in a profile characterized by maladjustment (high stress, poor adaptation). Concerning personality traits, we hypothesized that emerging adults in profiles characterized by resilience (high stress, good adaptation) and by competence (low stress, good adaptation) would show higher extraversion, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and emotional stability in adolescence than emerging adults in a profile characterized by maladjustment (high stress, poor adaptation). Group differences in openness were exploratively examined, due to inconsistent findings in previous work on the association between openness and (mal)adaptation (e.g., Anglim et al., Reference Anglim, Horwood, Smillie, Marrero and Wood2020; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt and Watson2010; Malouff et al., Reference Malouff, Thorsteinsson and Schutte2005).

Method

Design and study population

Our study hypotheses, design, and analysis plan were preregistered on the Open Science Framework; see https://osf.io/v38hr. The current study is part of the ongoing longitudinal Flemish Study on Parenting, Personality, and Development, which started in 1999 and consists of nine waves of data collection so far. The recruitment and sample characteristics of the study have been described in detail elsewhere (Prinzie et al., Reference Prinzie, Onghena, Hellinckx, Grietens, Ghesquiere and Colpin2003). A proportional stratified sample of elementary-school-aged children and their families in Belgium was randomly selected. Parents of 682 (85.3% response rate) children agreed to participate in the study. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants and the board of the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven approved the study.

We used data from the fourth (T1; 2004), fifth (T2; 2007), sixth (T3; 2009) and ninth (T4; 2018) measurement waves, as these waves comprised the period of adolescence (M age T1 = 10.82 years; M age T2 = 13.82 years; M age T3 = 15.82 years) and emerging adulthood (M age T4 = 24.82 years). A total of 561 families still participated during adolescence, that is, at T1, T2, and/or T3, and hence were eligible for the current study (82.3% retention rate). Families were included in the identification of resilience and vulnerability profiles (study aim 1) if data were available on at least one profile indicator of stressful life events, behavior problems, or subjective well-being in emerging adulthood. Of the 561 families that participated in adolescence, 346 families had available information on at least one profile indicator in emerging adulthood (n = 215, 38.3% lost to follow-up). For the analysis of profile differences regarding parenting and personality (study aim 2), we included families with data on at least one profile indicator and data on at least half of the parenting (i.e., two out of four) and personality (i.e., three out of five) constructs (see Measures). For the same 346 families data were available on at least half of the parenting and personality constructs. Thus, these 346 families comprised the final sample.

Families in the final sample (n = 346) did not differ from families that were not included (n = 215) regarding the age of children (t (558) = −0.75, p = .455), the age of mothers (t (537) = −0.57, p = .567) and fathers (t (524) = 0.54, p = .589), the education level of mothers (χ2 (3) = 3.33, p = .343) and fathers (χ2 (3) = 6.77, p = .080), and internalizing and externalizing problems of children at ages 6, 7 and 8 years (t (530–539) = −0.004 to 1.38, ps = .169–.997). However, the final sample contained relatively more girls compared to the excluded sample (χ2 (1) = 12.53, p < .001).

Characteristics of the study sample are presented in Table 1. About 57% of the children were girls. All parents were native Belgians and most were married or lived together (85%). The majority of the mothers (64%) and fathers (57%) had pursued either non-university higher education (vocational or professional degrees, comparable to US “college”) or university education (bachelor’s or master’s degrees).

Table 1. Child and family characteristics of study sample (N = 346)

a Non-university higher education = “college.”

Measures

Indicators of resilience and vulnerability profiles: emerging adulthood (T4)

Stressful life events

At T4, emerging adults retrospectively reported on the lifetime occurrence of stressful events (e.g., illness of a loved one, parental separation, being bullied, financial difficulties). We constructed a stressful life event scale (22 items; see Table S1 in Supplementary Material for an overview of items), as we needed a relatively short questionnaire that covers the entire lifetime and measures the perceived stress caused by the events, given that the impact of life events on psychological outcomes may depend on perceived event characteristics (Luhmann et al., Reference Luhmann, Fassbender, Alcock and Haehner2021). We therefore selected concrete events that are typically perceived as negative or undesirable from established scales that consider lifetime stressful events and show good psychometric properties, that is, the Questionnaire of Stressful Life Events (Butjosa et al., Reference Butjosa, Gómez-Benito, Myin-Germeys, Barajas, Baños, Usall, Grau, Granell, Sola, Carlson, Dolz, Sánchez, Haro and Ochoa2017), the Stressful Life Events Screening Questionnaire (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Corcoran, Turner, Yuan and Green1998), and the Social Readjustment Rating Scale (Holmes & Rahe, Reference Holmes and Rahe1967; Scully et al., Reference Scully, Tosi and Banning2000). Two different stress scores were created to assess both the objective number of life events experienced and the subjective impact of events. First, participants were asked to indicate whether a stressful life event had occurred (0 = no, 1 = yes) and at what age this happened. We computed a total score by summing the number of life events that occurred before 16 years of age, that is, no later than the assessment of our resource factors at age 11, 14, and 16 years. Higher scores represented exposure to more stressful life events (theoretical range: 0–22). Second, for each life event that was experienced, participants retrospectively rated the perceived stress (“How much stress did this event cause you?” from 0 = no stress to 10 = a lot of stress). A total stress score was computed by summing the amount of stress caused by the life events that occurred before 16 years of age. Higher scores indicated more perceived stress (theoretical range: 0–220).

Behavior problems

Current behavior problems were assessed by emerging adults’ ratings on the Adult Self-Report (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003). The six syndrome scales underlying the internalizing and externalizing broadband scales were used: anxious/depressed behavior (17 items; α = .92), withdrawn behavior (9 items; α = .78), somatic complaints (12 items; α = .80), aggressive behavior (15 items; α = .78), rule-breaking behavior (14 items; α = .71), and intrusive behavior (6 items; α = .72). All items were answered on a 3-point scale (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, 2 = very true or often true). We computed six problem scores by summing the relevant item scores, with higher scores indicating more problems.

Subjective well-being

Emerging adults reported on their life satisfaction using the Satisfaction With Life Scale (Arrindell et al., Reference Arrindell, Meeuwesen and Huyse1991; Diener et al., Reference Diener, Emmons, Larsen and Griffin1985) and on their positive affect using the positive affect subscale of the Positive Affect and Negative Affect Schedule (Peeters et al., Reference Peeters, Ponds and Vermeeren1996; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988). The five items of the Satisfaction With Life Scale (α = .84) were answered on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). A total score was computed by summing all item scores, with higher scores indicating higher life satisfaction. Concerning the positive affect subscale of the Positive Affect and Negative Affect Schedule (α = .89), emerging adults rated the extent to which they had experienced ten positive mood states during the past week on a 5-point scale (1 = very slightly or not at all to 5 = extremely). We computed a positive affect score by summing all item scores, with higher scores indicating higher levels of positive affect.

Resource factors: adolescence (T1, T2, and T3)

Positive parenting

Mothers and fathers reported on their positive parenting behaviors using the warmth/involvement (11 items) and reasoning/induction (6 items) subscales of the Parenting Practices Questionnaire (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Mandleco, Frost Olsen and Hart1995; Schalenbourg & Verschueren, Reference Schalenbourg and Verschueren2003). Answers on the Parenting Practices Questionnaire are given on a 5-point scale (1 = never to 5 = always). We computed three scores for warmth/involvement at T1, T2, and T3 (αmothers = .83–.87; αfathers = .85–.86) and three scores for reasoning/induction at T1, T2, and T3 (αmothers = .82–.91; αfathers = .85–.89) for mothers and fathers separately by summing the relevant item scores. Higher scores represented more positive parenting.

Negative parenting

Likewise, mothers and fathers reported on their overreactive parenting as a representation of negative parenting behavior, using the Parenting Scale (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, O’Leary, Wolff and Acker1993; Prinzie et al., Reference Prinzie, Onghena and Hellinckx2007). The nine items tapping overreactivity describe discipline encounters (e.g., “When my child misbehaves…”) followed by two options that act as opposite anchor points for a 7-point scale, on which 1 indicates a high probability of using a more positive discipline strategy (e.g., “I speak to my child calmly”) and 7 indicates a high probability of using a more negative, that is, overreactive, discipline strategy (“I raise my voice or yell”). We computed three overreactivity scores at T1, T2, and T3 (αmothers = .77–.79; αfathers = .75–.81) for mothers and fathers separately by summing the item scores. Higher scores represented more overreactive parenting.

Personality

Mothers and fathers reported on their adolescent’s Big Five personality traits using the Hierarchical Personality Inventory for Children (HiPIC; Mervielde & De Fruyt, Reference Mervielde, De Fruyt, De Raad and Perugini2002). The HiPIC consists of 144 items that are answered on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = almost not characteristic to 5 = very characteristic). The five personality traits of the HiPIC are: Extraversion (32 items), Benevolence (similar to Agreeableness; 40 items), Conscientiousness (32 items), Emotional stability (16 items), and Imagination (similar to Openness; 24 items). For each personality trait, we computed three scores for mothers at T1, T2, and T3 (α range = .90–.95) and three scores for fathers at T1, T2, and T3 (α range = .89–.95) by summing the relevant item scores.

We estimated latent constructs to form stable indicators of the levels of positive parenting, negative parenting, and personality traits across adolescence. Positive parenting constructs for mothers and fathers were modeled, using warmth/involvement and reasoning/induction scores at T1, T2, and T3 as indicators. Likewise, negative parenting constructs for mothers and fathers were modeled with overreactivity scores at T1, T2, and T3 as indicators. Additionally, we modeled five personality constructs (i.e., extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and openness), using mothers’ and fathers’ ratings of the corresponding personality traits at T1, T2, and T3 as indicators. Missing data were handled by full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation. Confirmatory factor analyses indicated good model fit for all latent constructs, range χ2 (1–3) = 0.07–3.62, p = .306–.787; CFI = .999–1.00, TLI = .997–1.01, RMSEA = .00–.02, SRMR = .00–.02 (Byrne, Reference Byrne2012). Model fit of the latent constructs of negative parenting could not be evaluated, since these were saturated models.

Covariates

As prior research indicates that child age, child sex, and parental education may be associated with the experience of stressful life events as well as with behavior problems and subjective well-being (e.g., Hatch & Dohrenwend, Reference Hatch and Dohrenwend2007; Lucas & Gohm, Reference Lucas, Gohm, Diener and Suh2000; World Health Organization, 2016) we considered the inclusion of these demographic characteristics as covariates in the analyses. Parents reported on their highest level of educational attainment (1 = primary school, 2 = secondary school, 3 = non-university higher education (“college”), 4 = university) in middle childhood (M age child = 7.82 years). We used the level of the parent with the highest education level as our measure of family education attainment.

Statistical Analyses

Missing data

The amount of missing data on profile indicators ranged between 0.0% and 26.9% (M = 3.4%) and on parenting and personality constructs between 0.0% and 2.9% (M = 0.7%) in the final sample (N = 346). In nearly all families (n = 335, 96.8%) both mother and father reports on parenting behaviors and personality traits were present. For a small number of families, only mother reports (n = 10, 2.9%) or only father reports (n = 1, 0.3%) were available. We tested patterns in missing data by performing Little’s MCAR test (Little, Reference Little1988). Since the data were completely missing at random (χ2 (131) = 141.13, p = .257), multiple imputation was performed in Mplus to handle missing data after the parenting and personality constructs were created. We employed multiple imputation instead of FIML as was originally preregistered, since FIML was not compatible with our statistical analyses (Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2021). More specifically, missing data on profile indicators, parenting constructs, and personality constructs were imputed using 10 imputed data sets and 100 iterations. We used information on profile indicators, parenting and personality constructs, and auxiliary variables (i.e., emerging adults’ age and sex, and family educational level) as predictors in the multiple imputation model (Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2021).

Main analyses

First, we checked the data for the assumption of multivariate normality and multivariate outliers. Second, we calculated descriptive statistics and correlations between the study variables. Third, we performed LPA using the recommended three-step approach (R3STEP command; Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2014; Vermunt, Reference Vermunt2010) in Mplus 8.5 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998–2017) to identify underlying profiles of adaptation in emerging adulthood and to examine whether the profiles differed on maternal and paternal positive and negative parenting and personality traits. In Step 1 of the three-step approach, the latent profiles were evaluated. We used the indicators of stress (i.e., number of life events, stress caused by life events), behavior problems (i.e., anxious/depressed behavior, withdrawn behavior, somatic complaints, aggressive behavior, rule-breaking behavior, and intrusive behavior), and subjective well-being (i.e., life satisfaction and positive affect) as profile indicators. Standardized values (z-scores) of the profile indicators were used, as this increases interpretation of the latent profiles and may help model convergence (Spurk et al., Reference Spurk, Hirschi, Wang, Valero and Kauffeld2020). In Step 2, the individuals were assigned to their most likely profile using the posterior profile probabilities. In Step 3, associations between external variables (i.e., positive and negative parenting and personality traits) and the profiles were examined by performing multinomial logistic regression (MLR) analyses. This method accounted for the average uncertainty in profile assignment, by using the estimated average classification errors for each profile (Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2014; Vermunt, Reference Vermunt2010). We tested two MLR models with profile membership as outcome variable, including the four parenting behaviors (Model 1) and the five personality traits (Model 2) as predictors. To test our hypotheses, we used a profile characterized by resilience as the reference group, so that this profile was compared against the other identified profiles. If the hypothesized profiles characterized by resilience, maladjustment, or competence were not identified or if profiles characterized by other patterns were identified, we would exploratively test differences in parenting behaviors and personality traits by comparing all identified profiles to each other. Additionally, we computed the percentages of behavior problems in the borderline (93rd–97th percentiles) and clinical (>97th percentile) range per profile, based on the norms for the Adult Self Report problem scales (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2015; not preregistered).

A one-profile model was evaluated first. Subsequently, we increased the number of profiles one by one, examining whether the addition of each profile resulted in statistically and conceptually superior solutions. Relative model fit was assessed by the Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, Reference Akaike1987), the Bayesian information criterion (BIC; Schwarz, Reference Schwarz1978), the sample size-adjusted BIC (SSABIC; Sclove, Reference Sclove1987), and entropy. The lower the values of the AIC, BIC, and SSABIC, the better the model fit (Tein et al., Reference Tein, Coxe and Cham2013). Entropy indicates how well a model categorizes individuals into profiles, with better categorization for values closer to 1 (Celeux & Soromenho, Reference Celeux and Soromenho1996). To compare models, the Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin (VLMR) likelihood ratio test and the adjusted Lo-Mendell-Rubin (adjusted LMR) likelihood ratio test were used with p-values < .05 indicating that the estimated model fits the data better than a model with one profile less (Tein et al., Reference Tein, Coxe and Cham2013). As recommended by Nylund-Gibson and Choi (Reference Nylund-Gibson and Choi2018), we considered statistical fit indices, entropy values, and the substantive interpretability and utility of profiles (Do profiles contain a decent number of individuals, that is, at least 5% of the sample? Do the profiles theoretically make sense?). A common procedure is to accept the model with the largest number of profiles, smallest BIC value, and an adjusted LMR with p < .05, in conjunction with the theoretical interpretability and utility of the profiles (Nylund-Gibson & Choi, Reference Nylund-Gibson and Choi2018).

In all analyses, we used significance levels of p < .05. As the predictors in the MLR were standardized, odds ratios (OR) could be used as indices of effect size for individual predictors. Effect sizes were defined as very small (OR < 1.68 or inverted > 0.60; equivalent to Cohen’s d < 0.20), small (1.68 or inverted 0.60; Cohen’s d = 0.20), medium (OR 3.47 or inverted 0.29, Cohen’s d = 0.50), or large (OR 6.71 or inverted 0.15, Cohen’s d = 0.80; Chen et al., Reference Chen, Cohen and Chen2010; not preregistered). Additionally, McFadden’s pseudo R2 values (ρ2; McFadden, Reference McFadden and Zarembka1974) were computed to indicate the total variance explained in the profiles by the set of predictors.

We tested whether the proposed covariates should be included in the MLR models. The age and sex of emerging adults and the family educational attainment were included as covariates if these sociodemographic variables were related (p < .05) to latent profile membership in the final LPA model (Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2014).

Results

Preliminary analyses

Results of assumption checking indicated that the study variables were normally distributed, except for somatic complaints (skewness = 2.2; kurtosis = 6.8). Therefore, a maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard error was used in all analyses (Byrne, Reference Byrne2012, pp. 99–100). No outliers were detected in the data. Means and standard deviations of the variables and correlations between the variables are presented in Table 2. On average, individuals experienced 1.82 out of 22 stressful life events (range: 0–8 events). Most individuals (72.0%) reported to be exposed to 0 (23.1%), 1 (25.2%) or 2 (23.7%) life events. The occurrence per stressful life event ranged from 0.3% (n = 1; “Have you ever had alcohol, drug, or addiction problems?”) to 29.2% (n = 101; “Have you ever been bullied?”). The perceived stress levels caused by a life event ranged from 1 to 10, with average stress levels per event ranging from 1.50 (“Have you ever been suspended or expelled from school?”) to 7.00 (“Have you ever had alcohol, drug, or addiction problems?”). Bivariate correlations indicated that exposure to a higher number of stressful life events was weakly correlated with higher levels of behavior problems and lower levels of positive affect and life satisfaction in emerging adulthood. Most of the behavior problems were negatively correlated with levels of positive affect and life satisfaction (small to large effects). The correlations between all behavior problems, between all personality traits, and between all maternal and paternal parenting behaviors were in the expected directions, representing small to large effect sizes. Furthermore, positive parenting behaviors were positively correlated with all personality traits and negative parenting behaviors correlated negatively with most of the personality traits (small to medium effects)

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and Spearman’s correlations for study variables (N = 346)

Note. m = mother; f = father. Resource factors (variables 11–19) concerned standardized. Latent variables.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Latent profiles of resilience and vulnerability in emerging adulthood

Fit indices resulting from the latent profile models containing one through five profiles are provided in Table 3. Based on the lower AIC, BIC, and SSABIC values, high entropy value, and VLMR and adjusted LMR tests (p < .05), a three-profile model was retained as the best model to fit the data. In addition, the smallest class contained more than 5% of the sample and the profiles were supported by theory. Although the four-profile model displayed lower values on the AIC, BIC and SSABIC statistics, this model did not show a better fit to the data than the three-profile model based on the VLMR and adjusted LMR tests. Moreover, the smallest class size in the four-profile model was too low (<5% of sample). Finally, we tested a five-profile model, however, this model did not show a better fit to the data than the four-profile model and two out of five profiles were theoretically difficult to interpret. As such, the three-profile model was preserved as the statistically and conceptually superior solution.

Table 3. Model fit statistics of latent profile analysis

Note. The bold three-profile solution represents the most optimal model. AIC = Akaike Information Criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; SSABIC = sample size-adjusted Bayesian information criterion; VLMR = Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio; Adj. LMR = adjusted Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio test.

Description of the three-profile solution

The means and standard deviations of the indicator variables in each profile are provided in Table 4. Additionally, the profiles are graphically represented using standardized scores in Figure 1. Profile 1 comprised 70.5% of the total sample (n = 244) and was characterized by emerging adults with lower levels of stress, lower levels of behavior problems, and higher levels of subjective well-being. Following the labeling in previous studies on resilience profiles (e.g., Masten et al., Reference Masten, Hubbard, Gest, Tellegen, Garmezy and Ramirez1999, Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004; Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013; Moreno et al., Reference Moreno, García-Moya, Rivera and Ramos2016; Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012), this group was labeled “Competent.” As shown in Table 5, scores in the (borderline) clinical range on behavior problems for this profile ranged from 0.0% (aggressive behavior) to 1.6% (intrusive behavior). Profile 2 (20.5%, n = 71) consisted of emerging adults with average levels of stress, higher levels of behavior problems, and lower levels of subjective well-being. This profile was referred to as “Vulnerable.” For emerging adults in this profile, scores in the (borderline) clinical range on behavior problems ranged from 8.4% (intrusive behavior) to 46.5% (anxious/depressed behavior). Profile 3 represented the smallest proportion (9.0%, n = 31) of emerging adults in the sample. This group was typified by higher levels of stress, average levels of behavior problems, and average levels of subjective well-being and was labeled “Resilient.” In this profile, scores in the (borderline) clinical range on behavior problems ranged from 0.0% (aggressive behavior) to 6.5% (rule-breaking behavior and somatic complaints).

Figure 1. Patterns of Indicator Variables Across the Three Profiles Using Standardized Z-Scores.

Table 4. Means and standard deviations of indicator and demographic variables per profile

Note. Unstandardized values are presented.

a Post hoc comparisons (z-tests); only significant differences are shown between the Competent (C) profile, Vulnerable (V) profile, and Resilient (R) profile.

b Non-university higher education = “college.”

Table 5. Percentages of individuals with borderline and clinical scores on the Adult Self-Report subscales per profile

Note. Percentages in the borderline (93rd–97th percentiles) and clinical (>97th percentiles) range are based on the multicultural norms for the Adult Self-Report (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2015).

Post hoc comparisons (z-tests) indicated that emerging adults in the Resilient profile reported a greater number of stressful life events and more perceived stress than their peers in the Competent and Vulnerable profile (see Table 4). Furthermore, individuals in the Resilient and Vulnerable profile experienced more behavior problems in emerging adulthood than those in the Competent profile. Moreover, individuals in the Vulnerable profile reported lower levels of subjective well-being than their peers in the Resilient and Competent profile. Concerning demographic variables, results showed that only family educational attainment was related to profile membership (Resilient vs. Competent: B = −1.36, SE = 0.37, p < .001; Resilient vs. Vulnerable: B = −1.33, SE = 0.40, p = .001; Vulnerable vs. Competent: B = −0.03, SE = 0.21, p = .874). Family educational attainment was lower for emerging adults in the Resilient profile compared to those in the Competent (p < .001) and Vulnerable (p = .001) profiles. The identified profiles did not differ concerning emerging adults’ mean age (ps range: .336–.934) or sex distribution (ps range: .645–.886). Therefore, we included only family educational attainment as a covariate in the MLR models.

Associations of resource factors with latent profiles

Results of the MLR models revealed few differences in resource factors across the identified profiles (see Table 6). Concerning parenting behaviors in adolescence, the results showed that emerging adults in the Resilient profile had experienced higher levels of positive parenting by mothers compared to those in the Competent profile (B = 0.84, p = .038, OR = 2.33). This difference reflected a small effect size. No other differences in parenting behaviors by mothers or fathers were found (ps range: .100–.969). About 6% of the total variance in the profiles was explained by the four parenting behaviors.

Table 6. Results of multinomial logistic regression with the latent profile construct as dependent variable

Note. m = mother; f = father. OR = Odds ratio; CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. ρ2 = McFadden’s pseudo R2. Significant coefficients (p < .05) are displayed in bold. In Model 1 and 2, the predictor terms were simultaneously included. Family educational attainment was included as a covariate.

Regarding personality traits, emerging adults in the Resilient profile had lower levels of conscientiousness as compared to the Competent profile (B = −1.40, p = .042, OR = 0.25), indicating a medium effect. Moreover, emerging adults in the Resilient (B = −1.33, p = .028, OR = 0.26) and the Vulnerable (B = −1.14, p = .004, OR = 0.32) profile were less emotionally stable during adolescence than those in the Competent profile. These differences represented medium and small effect sizes, respectively. The profiles did not differ on levels of extraversion, agreeableness, and openness (ps range: .071–.956). Finally, about 9% of the total variance in the profiles was explained by the five personality traits.

Discussion

This study aimed to identify different profiles of resilience and vulnerability in response to stressful life events in a community sample of emerging adults. In addition, we examined to what extent parenting behaviors and personality traits assessed in adolescence were related to the identified profiles. Results of LPA with stressful life events, behavior problems, and subjective well-being as indicators revealed three distinct profiles: A Competent profile (70.5%; low stress, good adaptation), a Vulnerable profile (20.5%; average stress, poor adaptation), and a Resilient profile (9.0%; high stress, good adaptation). Emerging adults in the Resilient profile had experienced higher levels of positive parenting by mothers compared to those in the Competent profile. With respect to personality traits, emerging adults in the Resilient and Vulnerable profile tended to be less emotionally stable during adolescence than those in the Competent profile. Furthermore, emerging adults in the Resilient profile were less conscientious than their peers in the Competent profile.

The identification of the three profiles was partially in line with previous person-centered work on resilience. We used the same labeling as in prior work, in order to build a more cumulative science and to avoid the “jangle fallacy” (Kelley, Reference Kelley1927) in which similar constructs are given different names. More specifically, similar Competent and Resilient groups were identified in childhood and adolescence (Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013; Moreno et al., Reference Moreno, García-Moya, Rivera and Ramos2016) and in emerging adulthood (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Hubbard, Gest, Tellegen, Garmezy and Ramirez1999, Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004; Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012) by previous studies using traditional a priori classifications. Interestingly, in our study the Resilient group experienced more behavior problems than the Competent group, whereas when groups are created a priori by using cutoff scores, these groups have the same level of behavior problems by definition. This finding raises questions and fits the ongoing debate concerning the conceptualization of resilience: What is resilience and how does it present? (Infurna, Reference Infurna2020; Masten, Reference Masten2011). Perhaps the effects of stress were still noticeable in our Resilient group, although they were doing “better than expected” based on the high level of stress experienced. Moreover, this result supports the longstanding recognition that resilient people are not invincible but rather may have some circumscribed areas of difficulty (Masten, Reference Masten2001, Reference Masten2018; Rutter, Reference Rutter2012).

In contrast to our expectations and to the previously mentioned person-centered studies, we did not find a profile characterized by Maladjustment (high stress, poor adaptation). However, a Vulnerable profile (average stress, poor adaptation) was identified in this sample, representing emerging adults who seemed to experience elevated behavior problems and relatively low subjective well-being with only normative levels of stress. This finding was surprising, as in most previous studies (e.g., Masten et al., Reference Masten, Hubbard, Gest, Tellegen, Garmezy and Ramirez1999, Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004; Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012) this vulnerable group was too small for statistical analyses. Such individuals are likely to be underrepresented in community samples, also described as the “empty cell” phenomenon (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Hubbard, Gest, Tellegen, Garmezy and Ramirez1999; Masten & Tellegen, Reference Masten and Tellegen2012). An explanation for the identification of a Vulnerable but not a Maladjusted profile in the current study might be related to characteristics of our study sample. That is, on average, the emerging adults in this community sample in Belgium experienced relatively low levels of stressful life events. Whereas there was sufficient variability in the number of stressful life events and the stress caused by these events, few individuals experienced extremely high levels of adversity. In addition, we included other types of stressors than used in the previous person-centered studies among emerging adults (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Hubbard, Gest, Tellegen, Garmezy and Ramirez1999, Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004; Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012). That is, those studies included relatively more chronic adversity (e.g., living with a violent alcoholic parent in chronic poverty) whereas we primarily focused on discrete stressors (e.g., parental separation). Hence, it might be that we did not capture chronic adversity in the configural profiles, which may have reduced the ability to identify certain profiles such as a Maladjusted group. Overall, the results of our LPA indicate that emerging adults show heterogeneous patterns of adaptation after stressful life events and highlight the fact that some individuals are more vulnerable, while others are more resilient to life stress.

When examining the associations between mothers’ and fathers’ parenting and the identified profiles, we found little support for parenting behaviors as resource factors for resilience in emerging adulthood. Fathers’ parenting behaviors were not related to the profiles of resilience and vulnerability, in contrast to earlier work indicating that fathers’ parenting behaviors may foster resilience in emerging adulthood in the face of economic stress (Conger & Conger, Reference Conger and Conger2002; Masarik & Conger, Reference Masarik and Conger2017; Neppl et al., Reference Neppl, Jeon, Schofield and Donnellan2015). However, our results suggested that mothers’ parenting behaviors may be a resource factor for later resilience. Specifically, emerging adults in the Resilient profile had experienced higher levels of positive parenting by mothers in adolescence compared to those in the Competent profile. This result conflicts with previous findings by Masten et al. (Reference Masten, Burt, Roisman, Obradović, Long and Tellegen2004) showing that emerging adults in the Resilient group were similar to those in the Competent group regarding mothers’ positive parenting (i.e., warmth, closeness, structure) assessed in childhood, despite their differences in stressful life experiences. It is possible that, in the current study, maternal warmth and involvement helped Resilient emerging adults to adapt well despite exposure to life stress, while their low-risk, Competent peers perhaps did not need this resource factor to show positive adaptation. Hence, our finding supports the notion that positive and supportive parenting may be a protective factor in the face of risk for some emerging adults (Masten & Palmer, Reference Masten, Palmer and Bornstein2019; Masten, Reference Masten2018).

With respect to Big Five personality traits, our results showed that emerging adults in the Competent profile were more emotionally stable during adolescence than those in the Vulnerable profile. This finding is in line with previous work suggesting that greater emotional stability in youth is related to a variety of positive outcomes in adulthood, including lower levels of internalizing and externalizing problems (e.g., Van Eldik et al., Reference Van Eldik, de Haan, Arends and Prinzie2020) and higher levels of subjective well-being and self-efficacy (e.g., Anglim et al., Reference Anglim, Horwood, Smillie, Marrero and Wood2020; Deutz et al., Reference Deutz, van Eldik, Over de Vest, Ringoot, de Haan and Prinzie2021; Gale et al., Reference Gale, Booth, Mõttus, Kuh and Deary2013). Even at low levels of stress, emotional stability seems to be an important individual trait acting as a promotive factor for positive outcomes.

Moreover, emerging adults in the Resilient profile were less emotionally stable and less conscientious compared to those in the Competent profile. This result is somewhat surprising, given that individuals with higher levels of emotional stability and conscientiousness tend to use more effective coping strategies that may foster resilience (Barańczuk, Reference Barańczuk2019; Connor-Smith & Flachsbart, Reference Connor-Smith and Flachsbart2007; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Kratsiotis, Niven and Holman2020). In previous person-centered studies, individuals in the Resilient group generally did not differ from those in the Competent group in personality traits (Shiner & Masten, Reference Shiner and Masten2012) or other personal assets (i.e., self-control, self-concept; Miller-Lewis et al., Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013). The differences we found in the current study might be explained by the bidirectional interplay between personality and stressful life events that has been posited by dynamic personality theories (Endler & Parker, Reference Endler and Parker1992; Magnusson, Reference Magnusson and Pervin1990; Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Wood, Caspi, John, Robins and Pervin2008). For example, individuals who are more emotionally stable and conscientious, like those in the Competent profile, may have a lower risk to experience certain life events, such as a romantic break up or academic failure. Also, evidence indicates that exposure to stressful life events might influence personality development, for example, by decreasing youth’s level of emotional stability, conscientiousness (Bleidorn et al., Reference Bleidorn, Hopwood and Lucas2018; Jeronimus et al., Reference Jeronimus, Riese, Sanderman and Ormel2014; Shiner et al., Reference Shiner, Allen and Masten2017), or self-control (Blair, Reference Blair2010; Laceulle et al., Reference Laceulle, van Aken, Ormel and Nederhof2015; Ong et al., Reference Ong, Hong, Tsai and Tan2019). It is possible that the relatively high amount of stress experienced by those in the Resilient profile may have had negative effects on their emotional stability and conscientiousness. Future studies are needed to increase our understanding of the complex processes through which individuals’ personalities and their stressful life events affect their development over time.

The finding that emerging adults in the Resilient profile were distinguished by a combination of positive parenting of mothers and lower emotional stability and conscientiousness might seem puzzling. However, this pattern of findings aligns with the growing understanding that parenting behaviors and children’s personalities interact in predicting developmental outcomes, exacerbating or buffering their effects (e.g., Loginova, & Slobodskaya, Reference Loginova and Slobodskaya2021; Pascual-Sagastizabal et al., Reference Pascual-Sagastizabal, del Puerto-Golzarri and Azurmendi2021; Rioux et al., Reference Rioux, Castellanos-Ryan, Parent, Vitaro, Tremblay and Seguin2016). More specifically, in our study positive parenting behaviors of mothers may have buffered the potential negative effects of lower emotional stability and lower conscientiousness in the Resilient group. In other words, despite their more vulnerable personality, these adolescents may still have developed well because of their warm and supporting mothers. These complex findings point to the importance of considering both individual and family level resources and their interactive effects in resilience research. As argued by several resilience frameworks (e.g., Infurna & Luthar, Reference Infurna and Luthar2018; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016; Ungar & Theron, Reference Ungar and Theron2020), resilience is not merely the result of one factor in an individual’s life, but rather is facilitated by multiple dynamic, interacting individual level systems (e.g., psychology, genes, neurobiology), which are embedded in larger environmental systems (e.g., family, culture, policy). Consequently, disentangling the underlying mechanisms that lead to resilience and how to best foster it requires multidisciplinary research that accounts for the multiple systems involved in resilience (Ungar & Theron, Reference Ungar and Theron2020).

Practical implications

Our findings underscore that youth may respond differentially to the experience of stressful life events. Some individuals may develop behavior problems after life event exposure and may need help, while others seem to adapt well despite the experience of multiple stressful events. Since stressful life event exposure was not a universal risk factor for all individuals in this study, it is valuable to focus on strengthening individual and contextual characteristics that may promote adaptive coping and successful development in the face of stress. The higher levels of emotional stability and conscientiousness of individuals in the Competent profile, who showed best overall functioning, suggest that strengthening these personality traits in adolescence can optimize behavioral outcomes in emerging adulthood. Therapeutic interventions, such as mindfulness interventions and social skill training, have been shown to be effective in increasing emotional stability and conscientiousness (Roberts et al., Reference Roberts, Luo, Briley, Chow, Su and Hill2017). Given that maternal positive parenting was a protective factor in the context of stress, preventative parenting interventions focused on parental warmth and involvement may help children in high-stress environments to avoid negative adult outcomes (Sandler et al., Reference Sandler, Ingram, Wolchik, Tein and Winslow2015; Yap et al., Reference Yap, Morgan, Cairns, Jorm, Hetrick and Merry2016).

Strengths, limitations, and suggestions for future research

The present study builds upon previous work on resilience in emerging adulthood by using statistically advanced person-centered analyses to identify individual, naturally occurring patterns of stress and (mal)adaptation across multiple developmental domains. We adopted a prospective, longitudinal design with a large time interval from adolescence (age 11–16 years) to emerging adulthood (age 25 years) to investigate potential resource factors at both the contextual level, that is, paternal and maternal parenting, and the individual level, that is, personality traits, to obtain a more comprehensive picture of the resources and assets that may promote resilience.

The strengths of the current study should be interpreted in consideration of some limitations. First, the study design was not fully prospective given that the occurrence of stressful life events and their perceived impact were retrospectively reported in emerging adulthood. Despite findings indicating that retrospective reports of major, easily defined adverse childhood experiences have acceptable psychometric properties and similar associations with adult outcomes as prospective assessments (Hardt & Rutter, Reference Hardt and Rutter2004; Reuben et al., Reference Reuben, Moffitt, Caspi, Belsky, Harrington, Schroeder, Hogan, Ramrakha, Poulton and Danese2016), it is unknown to what extent individuals’ ratings of stress in the current study were affected by recall bias. It is recommended that future studies use prospective measures of stressful life events (e.g., by caregiver report) in addition to retrospective reports to test the robustness of our findings. Likewise, we constructed a new stressful life event scale instead of using an existing scale, as we aimed to assess both the lifetime occurrence of events typically seen as undesirable, and the perceived stress caused by these events. Future research is needed to investigate the psychometric properties of this life event scale.

Second, although we used a multi-informant design with reports of mothers, fathers, and their children, we relied on questionnaires due to the relatively large sample size of the present study. It can be worthwhile to use a combination of assessment methods (e.g., combining questionnaires with behavioral observations of parenting) to minimize the influence of parents’ and children’s social desirability tendencies and mood states on their ratings (Podsakoff et al., Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie and Podsakoff2012). Further, it is unclear to what extent parents’ judgments regarding their child’s personality traits correspond to adolescents’ own perceptions. In future work, it would be interesting to consider both parent reports and adolescents’ self-reports of their personality traits. However, the availability of resources plays a critical role in the selection of research instruments and methods.

Third, caution should be exercised with generalizing these findings to other samples given that our study consisted of predominantly White, European families from middle-class backgrounds. Families in our sample were characterized by relatively high levels of education and low levels of stress, and thus could be considered low risk. Patterns of resilience and vulnerability as well as the relevance of certain parenting and personality characteristics for development are potentially different in higher-risk or culturally different samples (Fergus & Zimmerman, Reference Fergus and Zimmerman2005; Valentino et al., Reference Valentino, Nuttall, Comas, Borkowski and Akai2012; Zolkoski & Bullock, Reference Zolkoski and Bullock2012). To generalize our results to other settings, it is essential that future studies examine patterns of resilience and the role of protective factors across families in varying contexts.

Fourth, despite the already mentioned advantages of LPA, this analysis technique has limitations as well. Although LPA is well suited for revealing meaningful patterns of stress and adaptation simultaneously within individuals, person-centered analyses such as LPA may be unduly influenced by features of the sample or by the nature of the selected indicators (Spurk et al., Reference Spurk, Hirschi, Wang, Valero and Kauffeld2020). Therefore, the current inductive findings warrant replication in other samples. In addition, LPA makes several statistical assumptions, such as local independence (i.e., all indicator variables are uncorrelated within each latent profile) and variance homogeneity (i.e., all indicator variable variances are constrained to equality across profiles), which are reason for careful interpretation of the current results (Peugh & Fan, Reference Peugh and Fan2013; Spurk et al., Reference Spurk, Hirschi, Wang, Valero and Kauffeld2020).

Fifth, since we found limited evidence for parental overreactivity and (paternal) warmth as family resource factors, it is likely that other parenting and family aspects are involved in the development of resilience in emerging adulthood. It would be valuable to examine the role of other parenting behaviors, such as helicopter parenting (i.e., parents being overly involved and protective in their children’s life), which has been related to emerging adults’ self-efficacy and well-being (Reed et al., Reference Reed, Duncan, Lucier-Greer, Fixelle and Ferraro2016; Schiffrin et al., Reference Schiffrin, Erchull, Sendrick, Yost, Power and Saldanha2019). Additionally, future studies could consider other potential individual resource factors that may be malleable and responsive to intervention efforts, such as adolescents’ mindset and coping strategies (Compas et al., Reference Compas, Jaser, Bettis, Watson, Gruhn, Dunbar, Williams and Thigpen2017). A more nuanced understanding of what resource factors in childhood and adolescence promote resilience can guide possible targets for interventions before youth enter the emerging adulthood period. Such early intervention strategies may help young people to strengthen their abilities and skills so that they are ready to tackle the challenges of emerging adulthood.

Conclusion

The current study provides new insights into the heterogeneous patterns of emerging adults’ adaptation after stressful life events. We identified three latent profiles characterized by competence, vulnerability, and resilience, demonstrating that some individuals are more vulnerable, whereas others are more resilient to life stress. Additionally, the profiles were associated with maternal warmth and with the personality traits emotional stability and conscientiousness assessed in adolescence. Our findings suggest that maternal warmth is an important protective factor in the context of stress, possibly having the potential to buffer the effects of vulnerable personality traits. In intervention strategies, it is essential to take into account that youth may respond differentially to the experience of stressful life events. Finally, further research into malleable resource factors such as parenting and personality characteristics is needed to guide interventions that help youth better deal with stress, thereby optimizing their development into emerging adulthood.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422000578

Funding statement

The present study was supported by the Erasmus Initiatives “Vital Cities and Citizens” of the Erasmus University Rotterdam (DdM).

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2015). Multicultural supplement to the manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317332. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294359 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anglim, J., Horwood, S., Smillie, L. D., Marrero, R. J., & Wood, J. K. (2020). Predicting psychological and subjective well-being from personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146, 279323. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000226 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aquilino, W. S., & Supple, A. J. (2001). Long-term effects of parenting practices during adolescence on well-being outcomes in young adulthood. Journal of Family Issues, 22, 289308. https://doi.org/10.1177/019251301022003002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnett, J. J., Zukauskiene, R., & Sugimura, K. (2014). The new life stage of emerging adulthood at ages 18-29 years: Implications for mental health. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1, 569576. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00080-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, D. S., O’Leary, S. G., Wolff, L. S., & Acker, M. M. (1993). The parenting scale: A measure of dysfunctional parenting and discipline situations. Psychological Assessment, 5(2), 137144. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.5.2.137 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arrindell, W. A., Meeuwesen, L., & Huyse, F. J. (1991). The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS): Psychometric properties in a non-psychiatric medical outpatients sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 117123. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(91)90094-R CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2, 329341. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.915181 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2021). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Using the BCH method in Mplus to estimate a distal outcome model and an arbitrary secondary model. Mplus Web Notes, 21, 122. https://www.statmodel.com/examples/webnotes/webnote21.pdf Google Scholar
Barańczuk, U. (2019). The five factor model of personality and emotion regulation: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 139, 217227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.11.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, C. (2010). Stress and the development of self-regulation in context. Child Development Perspectives, 4(3), 181188. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00145.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleidorn, W., Hopwood, C. J., & Lucas, R. E. (2018). Life events and personality trait change. Journal of Personality, 86, 8396. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12286 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borghuis, J., Denissen, J. A., Oberski, D., Sijtsma, K., Meeus, W. J., Branje, S., Koot, H. M., & Bleidorn, W. (2017). Big five personality stability, change, and codevelopment across adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 641657. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000138 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, K. B., & Masten, A. S. (2009). Development in the transition to adulthood: Vulnerabilities and opportunities. In Grant, J. E., & Potenza, M. N. (Eds.), Young adult mental health (pp. 518). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, K. B., & Paysnick, A. A. (2012). Resilience in the transition to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 493505. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000119 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butjosa, A., Gómez-Benito, J., Myin-Germeys, I., Barajas, A., Baños, I., Usall, J.,Grau, N., Granell, L., Sola, A., Carlson, J., Dolz, M., Sánchez, B., Haro, J. M., the GENIPE group, & Ochoa, S. (2017). Development and validation of the Questionnaire of Stressful Life Events (QSLE). Journal of Psychiatric Research, 95, 213223, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.08.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, B. M. (2012). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Routledge.Google Scholar
Cabrera, N. J., Hennigar, A., Alonso, A., McDorman, S. A., & Reich, S. M. (2021). The protective effects of maternal and paternal factors on children’s social development. Adversity and Resilience Science, 2, 8598. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42844-021-00041-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, D. (2014). Stressful life events. In Michalos, A. C. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of quality of life and well-being research. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5_2880 Google Scholar
Carlson, M., Oshri, A., & Kwon, J. (2015). Child maltreatment and risk behaviors: The roles of callous/unemotional traits and conscientiousness. Child Abuse & Neglect, 50, 234243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In Lerner, W. R. M., Eisenberg, N., & Damon, W. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (6th ed., pp. 300365). Wiley.Google Scholar
Celeux, G., & Soromenho, G. (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. Journal of Classification, 13, 195212. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01246098 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, E., Yu, T., Siliezar, R., Drage, J. N., Dezil, J., Miller, G. E., & Brody, G. H. (2020). Evidence for skin-deep resilience using a co-twin control design: Effects on low-grade inflammation in a longitudinal study of youth. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 88, 661667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.070 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, H., Cohen, P., & Chen, S. (2010). How big is a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies. Communications in Statistics—simulation and Computation®, 39, 860864. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610911003650383 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, S., Murphy, M. L., & Prather, A. A. (2019). Ten surprising facts about stressful life events and disease risk. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 577597. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102857 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collishaw, S., Pickles, A., Messer, J., Rutter, M., Shearer, C., & Maughan, B. (2007). Resilience to adult psychopathology following childhood maltreatment: Evidence from a community sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31, 211229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.02.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compas, B. E., Jaser, S. S., Bettis, A. H., Watson, K. H., Gruhn, M. A., Dunbar, J. P., Williams, E., & Thigpen, J. C. (2017). Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 939991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000110 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conger, R. D., & Conger, K. J. (2002). Resilience in Midwestern families: Selected findings from the first decade of a prospective, longitudinal study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 64, 361373. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00361.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor-Smith, J. K., & Flachsbart, C. (2007). Relations between personality and coping: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 10801107. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1080 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Day, R. D., & Padilla-Walker, L. M. (2009). Mother and father connectedness and involvement during early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 900904. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016438 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutz, M. H., van Eldik, W. M., Over de Vest, V. T., Ringoot, A., de Haan, A. D., & Prinzie, P. (2021). An 11-year prospective study of personality X parenting interactions as predictors of self-efficacy in young adults: Diathesis-stress or differential susceptibility? BMC Psychology, 9, 113. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00676-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 7175. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donnellan, M. B., Conger, K. J., McAdams, K. K., & Neppl, T. K. (2009). Personal characteristics and resilience to economic hardship and its consequences: Conceptual issues and empirical illustrations. Journal of Personality, 77, 16451676. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00596.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dumas, T. M., Lawford, H., Tieu, T. T., & Pratt, M. W. (2009). Positive parenting in adolescence and its relation to low point narration and identity status in emerging adulthood: A longitudinal analysis. Developmental Psychology, 45, 15311544. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017360 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrlich, K. B., Yu, T., Sadiq, A., & Brody, G. H. (2021). Neighborhood poverty, allostatic load, and changes in cellular aging in African American young adults: The moderating role of attachment [Special issue]. Attachment & Human Development, 23, 114. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2021.1976934 Google Scholar
Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1992). Interactionism revisited: Reflections on the continuing crisis in the personality area. European Journal of Personality, 6, 177198. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410060302 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrell, A. K., Simpson, J. A., Carlson, E. A., Englund, M. M., & Sung, S. (2017). The impact of stress at different life stages on physical health and the buffering effects of maternal sensitivity. Health Psychology, 36, 3544. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000424 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, B. J., Conger, R. D., & Burzette, R. G. (2018). Traumatic events, psychiatric disorders, and pathways of risk and resilience during the transition to adulthood. In Keyes, C. (Ed.), Special issue: Risk and resilience in human development (pp. 259290). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203764374 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fergus, S., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2005). Adolescent resilience: A framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annual Review of Public Health, 26, 399419. https://doi.org/10.1146/annur CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fritz, J., de Graaff, A. M., Caisley, H., Van Harmelen, A. L., & Wilkinson, P. O. (2018). A systematic review of amenable resilience factors that moderate and/or mediate the relationship between childhood adversity and mental health in young people. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 117. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00230 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gale, C. R., Booth, T., Mõttus, R., Kuh, D., & Deary, I. J. (2013). Neuroticism and extraversion in youth predict mental wellbeing and life satisfaction 40 years later. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 687697. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.06.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garmezy, N. (1985). Stress-resistant children: The search for protective factors. In Stevenson, J. E. (Ed.), Recent research in developmental psychopathology: Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry book supplement (pp. 213233). Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, L. A., Corcoran, C., Turner, K., Yuan, N., & Green, B. L. (1998). Assessing traumatic event exposure: General issues and preliminary findings for the Stressful Life Events Screening Questionnaire. Journal of Traumatic Stress: Official Publication of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, 11, 521542. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024456713321 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardt, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: Review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 260273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00218.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatch, S. L., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (2007). Distribution of traumatic and other stressful life events by race/ethnicity, gender, SES and age: A review of the research. American Journal of Community Psychology, 40, 313332. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-007-9134-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, M., Letcher, P., Sanson, A., Smart, D., & Toumbourou, J. (2009). Positive development in emerging adulthood. Australian Journal of Psychology, 61, 8999. https://doi.org/10.1080/00049530802001346 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213218. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, D. J., Kratsiotis, I. K., Niven, K., & Holman, D. (2020). Personality traits and emotion regulation. Emotion, 20(1), 6367. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000644 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Infurna, F. J. (2020). What does resilience signify? An evaluation of concepts and directions for future research. Gerontologia, 66, 323331. https://doi.org/10.1159/000507365 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Infurna, F. J., & Luthar, S. S. (2018). Re-evaluating the notion that resilience is commonplace: A review and distillation of directions for future research, practice, and policy. Clinical Psychology Review, 65, 4356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeronimus, B. F., Riese, H., Sanderman, R., & Ormel, J. (2014). Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: A five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 751764. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, T. L. (1927). Interpretation of educational measurements. World Book Company.Google Scholar
Klika, J. B., & Herrenkohl, T. I. (2013). A review of developmental research on resilience in maltreated children. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 14, 222234. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838013487808 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking, big, personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 768821. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020327 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laceulle, O. M., van Aken, M. A. G., Ormel, J., & Nederhof, E. (2015). Stress-sensitivity and reciprocal associations between stressful events and adolescent temperament. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 7683. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lansford, J. E., Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (2014). Mothers' and fathers' autonomy-relevant parenting: Longitudinal links with adolescents' externalizing and internalizing behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43, 18771889. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-0079-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanza, S. T., & Cooper, B. R. (2016). Latent class analysis for developmental research. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 5964. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12163 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeMoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., Tracy, A., Hoffmeister, J. A., Ip, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Meta-analysis: Exposure to early life stress and risk for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 59, 842855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.10.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, D. Y., Chan, A. C., & Ho, G. W. (2020). Resilience of emerging adults after adverse childhood experiences: A qualitative systematic review. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 119. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838020933865 Google ScholarPubMed
Lind, M. J., Brown, R. C., Sheerin, C. M., York, T. P., Myers, J. M., Kendler, K. S., & Amstadter, A. B. (2018). Does parenting influence the enduring impact of severe childhood sexual abuse on psychiatric resilience in adulthood? Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 49, 3341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-017-0727-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, R. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 11981202. https://doi.org/10.2307/2290157 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loeb, E. L., Kansky, J., Tan, J. S., Costello, M. A., & Allen, J. P. (2021). Perceived psychological control in early adolescence predicts lower levels of adaptation into mid-adulthood. Child Development, 92, e158e172. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13377 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loginova, S. V., & Slobodskaya, H. R. (2021). Interactions between child personality and parenting in relation to child well-being: Support for diathesis-stress and differential susceptibility patterns. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.558224 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, R. E., & Gohm, C. L. (2000). Age and sex differences in subjective well-being across cultures. In Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 291317). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Luecken, L. J., & Gress, J. L. (2010). Early adversity and resilience in emerging adulthood. In Reich, J. W., Zautra, A. J., & Hall, J. S. (Eds.), Handbook of adult resilience (pp. 238257). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Luhmann, M., Fassbender, I., Alcock, M., & Haehner, P. (2021). A dimensional taxonomy of perceived characteristics of major life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121, 633668. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000291 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnusson, D. (1990). Personality development from an interactional perspective. In Pervin, L. (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 193222). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., & Schutte, N. S. (2005). The relationship between the five-factor model of personality and symptoms of clinical disorders: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 101114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-005-5384-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
March-Llanes, J., Marqués-Feixa, L., Mezquita, L., Fañanás, L., & Moya-Higueras, J. (2017). Stressful life events during adolescence and risk for externalizing and internalizing psychopathology: A meta-analysis. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 14091422. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-0996-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masarik, A. S., & Conger, R. D. (2017). Stress and child development: A review of the Family Stress Model. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 8590. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. The American Psychologist, 56, 227238. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.56.3.227 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2011). Resilience in children threatened by extreme adversity: Frameworks for research, practice, and translational synergy. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 493506. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000198 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2018). Resilience theory and research on children and families: Past, present, and promise. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 10, 1231. https://doi.org/10.1111/jftr.12255 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., Burt, K. B., Roisman, G. I., Obradović, J., Long, J. D., & Tellegen, A. (2004). Resources and resilience in the transition to adulthood: Continuity and change. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 10711094. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579404040143 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2016). Resilience in development: Progress and transformation. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention (pp. 271333). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy406 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Hubbard, J. J., Gest, S. D., Tellegen, A., Garmezy, N., & Ramirez, M. (1999). Competence in the context of adversity: Pathways to resilience and maladaptation from childhood to late adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 11(1), 143169. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579499001996 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., Obradović, J., & Burt, K. (2006). Resilience in emerging adulthood: Developmental perspectives on continuity and transformation. In Arnett, J. J., & Tanner, J. L. (Eds.), Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century (pp. 173190). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11381-007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Palmer, A. R. (2019). Parenting to promote resilience in children. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: The practice of parenting (pp. 156188). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429401695-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (2012). Resilience in developmental psychopathology: Contributions of the project competence longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 345361. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941200003X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality. In Pervin, L. A., & John, O. P. (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 139153). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McFadden, D. (1974). Conditional logit analysis of qualitative choice behavior. In Zarembka, P. (Ed.), Frontiers in econometrics (pp. 105142). Academic Press. ∼http://eml.berkeley.edu/∼mcfadden/travel.html Google Scholar
Mervielde, I., & De Fruyt, F. (2002). Assessing children’s traits with the Hierarchical Personality Inventory for Children. In De Raad, B., & Perugini, M. (Eds.), Big Five assessment (pp. 127146). Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.Google Scholar
Miller-Lewis, L. R., Searle, A. K., Sawyer, M. G., Baghurst, P. A., & Hedley, D. (2013). Resource factors for mental health resilience in early childhood: An analysis with multiple methodologies. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 7, 123. https://doi.org/10.1186/1753-2000-7-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno, C., García-Moya, I., Rivera, F., & Ramos, P. (2016). Characterization of vulnerable and resilient Spanish adolescents in their developmental contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 122. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00983 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Neppl, T. K., Jeon, S., Schofield, T. J., & Donnellan, M. B. (2015). The impact of economic pressure on parent positivity, parenting, and adolescent positivity into emerging adulthood. Family Relations, 64, 8092. https://doi.org/10.1111/fare.12098 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nylund-Gibson, K., & Choi, A. Y. (2018). Ten frequently asked questions about latent class analysis. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 4, 440461. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000176 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, M., Sanson, A., Toumbourou, J. W., Hawkins, M. T., Letcher, P., Williams, P., & Olsson, C. (2016). Positive development and resilience in emerging adulthood. In Arnett, J. J. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of emerging adulthood (pp. 601614). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199795574.013.19 Google Scholar
O’Connor, M., Sanson, A., Hawkins, M. T., Olsson, C., Frydenberg, E., Toumbourou, J. W., & Letcher, P. (2012). The relationship between positive development and psychopathology during the transition to adulthood: A person-centred approach. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 701712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.10.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obradović, J., Burt, K. B., & Masten, A. S. (2006). Pathways of adaptation from adolescence to young adulthood: Antecedents and correlates. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 340344. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oesterle, S., Hill, K., Hawkins, J., & Abbott, R. (2008). Positive functioning and alcohol-use disorders from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69, 100111. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2008.69.100 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ong, X. L., Hong, R. Y., Tsai, F.-F., & Tan, S. H. (2019). Becoming more or less mature? The decline of self-control in middle childhood. Journal of Personality, 87, 799812. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12434 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascual-Sagastizabal, E., del Puerto-Golzarri, N., & Azurmendi, A. (2021). Differential susceptibility or diathesis-stress: Testing the moderating role of temperament and cortisol levels between fathers’ parenting and children’s aggressive behavior. Brain Sciences, 11, 114. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11081088 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peeters, F. P. M. L., Ponds, R. H. W. M., & Vermeeren, M. T. G. (1996). Affectiviteit en zelfbeoordeling van depressie en angst [Affectivity and self-report of depression and anxiety]. Tijdschrift voor Psychiatrie, 38, 240250.Google Scholar
Peugh, J., & Fan, X. (2013). Modeling unobserved heterogeneity using latent profile analysis: A Monte Carlo simulation. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 20, 616639. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2013.824780 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2012). Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 539569. https://doi.org/10.1146/annur CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinzie, P., Onghena, P., & Hellinckx, W. (2007). Reexamining the parenting scale. Reliability, factor structure, and concurrent validity of a scale for assessing the discipline practices of mothers and fathers of elementary-school-aged children. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23(1), 2431. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.1.24 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinzie, P., Onghena, P., Hellinckx, W., Grietens, H., Ghesquiere, P., & Colpin, H. (2003). The additive and interactive effects of parenting and children’s personality on externalizing behavior. European Journal of Personality, 17(2), 95117. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.467 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, K., Duncan, J. M., Lucier-Greer, M., Fixelle, C., & Ferraro, A. J. (2016). Helicopter parenting and emerging adult self-efficacy: Implications for mental and physical health. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25, 31363149. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-016-0466-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuben, A., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Belsky, D. W., Harrington, H., Schroeder, F., Hogan, S., Ramrakha, S., Poulton, R., & Danese, A. (2016). Lest we forget: Comparing retrospective and prospective assessments of adverse childhood experiences in the prediction of adult health. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 11031112. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12621 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rioux, C., Castellanos-Ryan, N., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Seguin, J. R. (2016). Differential susceptibility to environmental influences: Interactions between child temperament and parenting in adolescent alcohol use. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 265275. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000437 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, B. W., Luo, J., Briley, D. A., Chow, P. I., Su, R., & Hill, P. L. (2017). A systematic review of personality trait change through intervention. Psychological Bulletin, 143(2), 117141. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000088 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2008). Development of personality traits in adulthood. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W., & Pervin, L. A. (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 375398). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Frost Olsen, S., & Hart, C. H. (1995). Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: Development of a new measure. Psychological Reports, 7(3), 819830. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.819 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russotti, J., Warmingham, J., Handley, E., Rogosch, F., & Cicchetti, D. (2020). Characterizing competence among a high-risk sample of emerging adults: Prospective predictions and biological considerations. Development and Psychopathology, 32, 19371953. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001467 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2012). Resilience as a dynamic concept. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 335344. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salgado, M., González, L., & Yáñez, A. (2021). Parental involvement and life satisfaction in early adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.628720 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. J. (2000). Developmental systems and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 297312. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400003035 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandler, I., Ingram, A., Wolchik, S., Tein, J. Y., & Winslow, E. (2015). Long-term effects of parenting-focused preventive interventions to promote resilience of children and adolescents. Child Development Perspectives, 9(3), 164171. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12126 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schalenbourg, K., & Verschueren, K. (2003). Probleemgedrag van kleuters op school en de relatie met ouderlijke opvoeding [Problem behaviour of infants at school and the link with parenting styles]. Kind en Adolescent, 24, 170187. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03060891 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiffrin, H. H., Erchull, M. J., Sendrick, E., Yost, J. C., Power, V., & Saldanha, E. R. (2019). The effects of maternal and paternal helicopter parenting on the self-determination and well-being of emerging adults. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28, 33463359. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01513-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461–464. https://doi.org/10.1214/aos/1176344136 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sclove, L. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 52, 333343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02294360 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scully, J. A., Tosi, H., & Banning, K. (2000). Life event checklists: Revisiting the social readjustment rating scale after 30 years. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 60, 864876. https://doi.org/10.1177/00131640021970952 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiner, R. L., Allen, T. A., & Masten, A. S. (2017). Adversity in adolescence predicts personality trait change from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Research in Personality, 67, 171182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.10.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiner, R. L., & Masten, A. S. (2012). Childhood personality as a harbinger of competence and resilience in adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 507528. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000120 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiner, R. L., Van den Berg, T., Van Aken, M. A. G., & Laceulle, O. M. (2021). The development of personality and coping in childhood and adolescence. In Skinner, E. A., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (Eds.), Handbook on the development of coping. Cambridge University Press. [Chapter submitted for publication].Google Scholar
Smart, D., & Sanson, A. (2005). What is life like for young Australians today, and how well are they faring? Family Matters, 70, 4653. https://search.informit.org/doi/10.3316/ielapa.930330714888306 Google Scholar
Spurk, D., Hirschi, A., Wang, M., Valero, D., & Kauffeld, S. (2020). Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 120, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2020.103445 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tein, J. Y., Coxe, S., & Cham, H. (2013). Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 20, 640657. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2013.824781 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungar, M., & Theron, L. (2020). Resilience and mental health: How multisystemic processes contribute to positive outcomes. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7, 441448. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30434-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United Nations (2019). Database on household size and composition 2019. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. Retrieved from https://www.un.org/development/desa/pd/data/household-size-and-composition Google Scholar
Valentino, K., Nuttall, A. K., Comas, M., Borkowski, J. G., & Akai, C. E. (2012). Intergenerational continuity of child abuse among adolescent mothers: Authoritarian parenting, community violence, and race. Child Maltreatment, 17, 172181. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559511434945 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Eldik, W. M., de Haan, A. D., Arends, L. R., & Prinzie, P. (2020). Moderation of associations between interparental stress and (mal)adaptation by adolescents' personality: Contrasting differential susceptibility and diathesis-stress models. Journal of Personality, 89, 617633. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12603 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vermunt, J. K. (2010). Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Political Analysis, 18, 450469. https://doi.org/10.1093/pan/mpq025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible: A study of resilient children. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2016). Growing up unequal: Gender and socioeconomic differences in young people’s health and well-being. World Health Organization, Regional Office for Europe. Retrieved from https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/326320 Google Scholar
Yap, M. B. H., Morgan, A. J., Cairns, K., Jorm, A. F., Hetrick, S. E., & Merry, S. (2016). Parents in prevention: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of parenting interventions to prevent internalizing problems in children from birth to age 18. Clinical Psychology Review, 50, 138158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yap, M. B. H., Pilkington, P. D., Ryan, S. M., & Jorm, A. F. (2014). Parental factors associated with depression and anxiety in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 156, 823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.11.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zolkoski, S. M., & Bullock, L. M. (2012). Resilience in children and youth: A review. Children and Youth Services Review, 34, 22952303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2012.08.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Child and family characteristics of study sample (N = 346)

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and Spearman’s correlations for study variables (N = 346)

Figure 2

Table 3. Model fit statistics of latent profile analysis

Figure 3

Figure 1. Patterns of Indicator Variables Across the Three Profiles Using Standardized Z-Scores.

Figure 4

Table 4. Means and standard deviations of indicator and demographic variables per profile

Figure 5

Table 5. Percentages of individuals with borderline and clinical scores on the Adult Self-Report subscales per profile

Figure 6

Table 6. Results of multinomial logistic regression with the latent profile construct as dependent variable

Supplementary material: File

de Maat et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download de Maat et al. supplementary material(File)
File 21 KB