Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T02:07:34.617Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neural response to errors among mothers with a history of recurrent depression and their adolescent daughters

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2024

Simon Morand-Beaulieu*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada
Iulia Banica
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada Ontario Shores Centre for Mental Health Sciences, Whitby, ON, Canada
Clara Freeman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada Minneapolis Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Minneapolis, MN, Canada
Paige Ethridge
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada Department of Psychology, Glenrose Rehabilitation Hospital, Alberta Health Services, Edmonton, AB, Canada
Aislinn Sandre
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada Department of Biobehavioral Sciences, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Anna Weinberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, QC, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Simon Morand-Beaulieu; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Depression is transmitted within families, but the mechanisms involved in such transmission are not clearly defined. A potential marker of familial risk is the neural response to errors, which may play a role in depression symptoms and is known to be partially heritable. Here, 97 mother-daughter dyads completed a Flanker task while electroencephalography markers of error monitoring were recorded: the error-related negativity (ERN) and response-locked delta and theta power. We assessed whether these measures of neural response to errors 1) were associated with history of recurrent major depressive disorder (MDD) and current depression symptoms among mothers, 2) were correlated among mother-daughter dyads, and 3) were associated with maternal history of recurrent MDD and maternal symptoms of depression among daughters. A history of recurrent MDD was associated with blunted delta and increased theta among mothers. Across mothers, delta and theta were negatively and positively associated, respectively, with current depression symptoms. Mothers’ and daughters’ ERN were positively correlated. Finally, current maternal depression symptoms were negatively associated with delta power in daughters. These results suggest that neural responses to errors may be implicated in the intergenerational transmission of depression. These results also support the relevance of delta oscillations to understanding pathways to depression.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Depression is transmitted within families. Depression symptoms often correlate in parent-child dyads (e.g., Griffith et al., Reference Griffith, Young and Hankin2021; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Rice, Harold, Collishaw and Thapar2011; Loechner et al., Reference Loechner, Sfärlea, Starman, Oort, Thomsen, Schulte-Körne and Platt2020). Multiple studies have also demonstrated that a parental history of depression confers risk for depression in offspring (Goodman, Reference Goodman2020; Gotlib et al., Reference Gotlib, Buthmann and Miller2020, Reference Gotlib, Goodman and Humphreys2023) with some additional evidence showing particularly strong effects from mothers to their daughters (Connelly & O'Connell, Reference Connelly and O'Connell2022). Depression is more prevalent in women than in men (Salk et al., Reference Salk, Hyde and Abramson2017; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Wu, Xin, Gao, Han, Zhao and Li2024), and this gender imbalance begins in early adolescence (Cyranowski et al., Reference Cyranowski, Frank, Young and Shear2000; Morken et al., Reference Morken, Viddal, von Soest and Wichstrøm2023). Because depression has a major impact on individual functioning as well as significant societal costs (König et al., Reference König, König and Konnopka2019; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Wang, Wittchen, Preedy and Watson2010), identifying mechanisms that may support the intergenerational transmission of risk for depression – especially among mother-daughter dyads – is important to develop methods to limit such transmission.

One process that could constitute a risk marker for the development and transmission of depression is the neural response to errors. When someone makes an error in a speeded-response task, a negative deflection can be seen in the electroencephalogram (EEG). That deflection – the error-related negativity (ERN) – appears approximately 0–100 ms after an error and is usually measured at frontocentral electrodes (Falkenstein et al., Reference Falkenstein, Hohnsbein, Hoormann and Blanke1991; Ullsperger et al., Reference Ullsperger, Danielmeier and Jocham2014). It is primarily generated by the anterior cingulate cortex, a brain region that is known to be involved in performance and conflict monitoring (Brázdil et al., Reference Brázdil, Roman, Daniel and Rektor2005; Keil et al., Reference Keil, Weisz, Paul-Jordanov and Wienbruch2010; Ko et al., Reference Ko, Ptito, Monchi, Cho, Van Eimeren, Pellecchia and Strafella2009; O'Connell et al., Reference O'Connell, Dockree, Bellgrove, Kelly, Hester, Garavan and Foxe2007). The ERN is thought to reflect neural processes that are triggered by error detection or response conflict (Gehring et al., Reference Gehring, Liu, Orr and Carp2012; Holroyd & Coles, Reference Holroyd and Coles2002; Kieffaber et al., Reference Kieffaber, Osborne, Norton and Hilimire2023). It can be used to assess individual differences pertaining to how the brain monitors and adjusts behavior in response to errors and conflicting situations (Olvet & Hajcak, Reference Olvet and Hajcak2008; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Weinberg, Endrass, Meyer and Hajcak2013).

A large body of literature suggests that the ERN is a viable risk marker for some forms of psychopathology, and particularly anxiety and related disorders. For instance, an enhanced ERN has been observed in individuals with anxiety disorders (Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Klawohn, Grützmann, Kaufmann, Heinzel, Bey and Kathmann2019; Riesel, Reference Riesel2019; Weinberg, Kotov, et al., 2015), is associated with trait anxiety (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Moran, Schroder, Donnellan and Yeung2013; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Härpfer, Thoma, Kathmann and Klawohn2022), and appears to prospectively predict anxiety symptoms (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Danielson, Danzig, Bhatia, Black, Bromet and Klein2017, Reference Meyer, Mehra and Hajcak2021). Yet, despite criterion overlap and high degrees of comorbidity between anxiety and depression (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Sampson, Berglund, Gruber, Al-Hamzawi, Andrade and Wilcox2015; Zbozinek et al., Reference Zbozinek, Rose, Wolitzky-Taylor, Sherbourne, Sullivan, Stein and Craske2012), the association between the ERN and depression is less clear. Consistent with the close association between depression and anxiety, several studies have also reported an enhanced ERN in individuals with depression (Aarts et al., Reference Aarts, Vanderhasselt, Otte, Baeken and Pourtois2013; Chiu & Deldin, Reference Chiu and Deldin2007; Holmes & Pizzagalli, Reference Holmes and Pizzagalli2008, Reference Holmes and Pizzagalli2010). However, others have found no difference between depressed individuals and controls (Hanna et al., Reference Hanna, Liu, Isaacs, Ayoub, Brosius, Salander and Gehring2018; Olvet et al., Reference Olvet, Klein and Hajcak2010; Schrijvers et al., Reference Schrijvers, De Bruijn, Maas, Vancoillie, Hulstijn and Sabbe2009; Weinberg, Kotov, et al., 2015), and still others have reported a blunted ERN in youth (Dell'Acqua et al., Reference Dell’Acqua, Brush, Burani, Santopetro, Klawohn, Messerotti Benvenuti and Hajcak2023; Ladouceur et al., Reference Ladouceur, Slifka, Dahl, Birmaher, Axelson and Ryan2012) and adults (Fissler et al., Reference Fissler, Winnebeck, Schroeter, Gummbersbach, Huntenburg, Gärtner and Barnhofer2017; Schoenberg, Reference Schoenberg2014; Schrijvers et al., Reference Schrijvers, de Bruijn, Maas, De Grave, Sabbe and Hulstijn2008; Weinberg, Liu, et al., 2016) with depression. The high degrees of comorbidity between anxiety and depression may be responsible for some inconsistency. While the ERN is usually enhanced in anxiety, comorbid depression may have a blunting effect on the ERN in people with anxiety disorders (Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Klein and Hajcak2012; Weinberg, Kotov, et al., 2015).

Task differences may also account for some discrepancies across studies. The most frequently used tasks in studies involving the ERN and depression are the Stroop and the flanker tasks. Both tasks involve interference control, but they differ in several aspects, such as the involvement of verbal vs. spatial interference and the type of information that needs to be filtered during task performance (Yeung et al., Reference Yeung, Lee and Chan2020). An enhanced ERN has been associated with depression when a Stroop task is used (Holmes & Pizzagalli, Reference Holmes and Pizzagalli2008, Reference Holmes and Pizzagalli2010). This is less frequently observed in studies that have used the Flanker task (Weinberg, Dieterich, et al., 2015); in fact, studies using a flanker task more typically find evidence of a blunted ERN associated with depression (Ladouceur et al., Reference Ladouceur, Slifka, Dahl, Birmaher, Axelson and Ryan2012; Schrijvers et al., Reference Schrijvers, de Bruijn, Maas, De Grave, Sabbe and Hulstijn2008; Weinberg, Meyer, et al., 2016). Moreover, recent data suggests that a blunted ERN elicited during a flanker task can prospectively predict depression symptoms in both children (Lawler et al., Reference Lawler, Hruschak, Aho, Liu, Ip, Lajiness-O'Neill and Fitzgerald2021) and adults (Sandre et al., Reference Sandre, Banica and Weinberg2023). Several studies reporting null effects have also assessed the ERN using the Flanker task (Hanna et al., Reference Hanna, Liu, Isaacs, Ayoub, Brosius, Salander and Gehring2018; Olvet et al., Reference Olvet, Klein and Hajcak2010; Schrijvers et al., Reference Schrijvers, De Bruijn, Maas, Vancoillie, Hulstijn and Sabbe2009; Weinberg, Kotov, et al., 2015), but it is possible that some of these studies lacked the necessary power to detect group differences. In addition, differences in reliability across the different tasks used to assess the ERN (Clayson, Reference Clayson2020; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Weinberg, Endrass, Meyer and Hajcak2013), or even across different variations of the Flanker task (Clayson et al., Reference Clayson, Rocha, McDonald, Baldwin and Larson2024), may also account for some of the aforementioned discrepancies.

Evidence that a blunted ERN can prospectively predict increases in depression symptoms is consistent with other evidence that the ERN may be a stable marker of vulnerability that could be useful in understanding transmission within families. For instance, this blunted ERN does not appear to be state-dependent, but instead can be observed in currently euthymic individuals with remitted depression (Weinberg, Liu, et al., 2016), who are at increased risk for subsequent episodes. The magnitude of the ERN is also subject to genetic contributions in adolescent twin samples (Anokhin et al., Reference Anokhin, Golosheykin and Heath2008; Burwell et al., Reference Burwell, Malone and Iacono2016), and abnormalities in the magnitude of the ERN have been observed in unaffected family members of individuals with anxiety and related disorders (Carrasco et al., Reference Carrasco, Harbin, Nienhuis, Fitzgerald, Gehring and Hanna2013; Riesel et al., Reference Riesel, Endrass, Kaufmann and Kathmann2011, Reference Riesel, Klawohn, Grützmann, Kaufmann, Heinzel, Bey and Kathmann2019). Additionally, there is evidence that neural measures of error-monitoring are correlated between parents and their children. From early childhood and across adolescence, positive associations between mothers’ and daughters’ ERN have been reported (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Fisher, Hicks, Zucker and Durbin2018; Suor et al., Reference Suor, Calentino, Granros and Burkhouse2022). Finally, and of interest to the current study, a blunted ERN has been observed in the never-depressed children (ages 9 to 17) of mothers with recurrent depression (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Bress, Hajcak and Gibb2018). Combined, these data suggest that the magnitude of the ERN may be familial and related to risk for depression. However, studies that have assessed familiality of the ERN have typically been in unselected samples, and studies that have assessed high-risk offspring have typically not collected data from parents.

In addition to the ERN, time-frequency decompositions of neural oscillations may provide relevant information related to error monitoring processes and their associations with depression and risk for depression. While the time-domain ERN only indexes phase-locked neural activity, decomposition of time-frequency total power provides information about both phase-locked and non-phased locked neural activity (Buzzell et al., Reference Buzzell, Niu, Aviyente and Bernat2022). Increased response-locked power in the delta and theta frequency bands has been observed during response monitoring tasks (Sandre & Weinberg, Reference Sandre and Weinberg2019; Völker et al., Reference Völker, Fiederer, Berberich, Hammer, Behncke, Kršek and Ball2018; Yordanova et al., Reference Yordanova, Falkenstein, Hohnsbein and Kolev2004). However, theta power appears to increase following both errors and correct responses (although to a lesser degree), whereas delta power appears specifically increased following errors (Yordanova et al., Reference Yordanova, Falkenstein, Hohnsbein and Kolev2004). This suggests that oscillations in these bands index partially dissociable processes. Theta may reflect generic response monitoring processes (Yordanova et al., Reference Yordanova, Falkenstein, Hohnsbein and Kolev2004), signaling that cognitive control needs to be implemented (Buzzell et al., Reference Buzzell, Barker, Troller-Renfree, Bernat, Bowers, Morales and Fox2019; Cavanagh & Frank, Reference Cavanagh and Frank2014; Cavanagh & Shackman, Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2015). In contrast, the functional significance of error-related delta is less understood. It has been suggested that delta could reflect more elaborative processes, such as strategic adjustments in behavior, after the theta response has signaled the need for cognitive control (Sandre & Weinberg, Reference Sandre and Weinberg2019). Error-related delta has also been associated with the motivational salience of errors (Umemoto et al., Reference Umemoto, Cole, Allison, Dolan, Lazarov, Auerbach and Schneier2021). Furthermore, while error-related theta is modulated by dopaminergic activity (Pezzetta et al., Reference Pezzetta, Wokke, Aglioti and Ridderinkhof2022), this is not the case with delta, which has been associated with norepinephrine activity (Pezzetta et al., Reference Pezzetta, Ozkan, Era, Tieri, Zabberoni, Taglieri and Aglioti2023). These two neurotransmitters are known to contribute to the pathophysiology of depression, although they could be associated with different symptom profiles (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Zhao and Guo2018; Nutt, Reference Nutt2008; Werner & Coveñas, Reference Werner and Coveñas2010). Given these specificities, assessing error-related delta and theta, in addition to the ERN, may improve our understanding of the association between neural responses to error and depression and risk for depression (Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Ethridge, Oumeziane, Foti, Gable, Miller and Bernat2022). However, the few studies that have examined associations between power in delta and theta in depression have reported mixed findings. One study reported decreased error-related theta in pediatric depression (Dell'Acqua et al., Reference Dell’Acqua, Brush, Burani, Santopetro, Klawohn, Messerotti Benvenuti and Hajcak2023), while another in adults found no significant links (Muir et al., Reference Muir, Hedges-Muncy, Clawson, Carbine and Larson2020). Additionally, neither study reported significant differences between depressed individuals and controls in the amplitude of their delta oscillations. Despite the relevance of delta and theta oscillations in the study of depression, their role in the familial transmission of risk for depression has not been studied.

Our first objective was to identify how neural correlates of error monitoring are associated with a lifetime history of recurrent major depressive disorder (MDD) and current depression symptoms in mothers. Given the blunted ERN seen in adults with remitted depression (Weinberg, Liu, et al., 2016), we expected that mothers with a history of recurrent MDD would show a blunted ERN relative to mothers without a history of depression. We also expected that a blunted ERN would be associated with more severe current symptoms of depression. Our second objective was to assess how mothers’ and daughters’ neural responses to errors were associated. We hypothesized that mothers’ and daughters’ ERN would be positively associated (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Fisher, Hicks, Zucker and Durbin2018; Suor et al., Reference Suor, Calentino, Granros and Burkhouse2022). Our third objective was to assess how neural measures of error monitoring in daughters were associated with maternal history of depression and current depression symptoms. Consistent with previous work (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Bress, Hajcak and Gibb2018), we expected that a maternal history of recurrent MDD and higher current maternal depression symptoms would be associated with a blunted ERN in daughters. For all three objectives, we did not formulate hypotheses for delta and theta power as these analyses were exploratory.

Methods

Participants

Mothers and their biological daughters were recruited from the Greater Montreal area using online and print advertisements (Ethridge et al., Reference Ethridge, Freeman, Sandre, Banica, Dirks and Weinberg2022; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Ethridge, Banica, Sandre, Dirks, Kujawa and Weinberg2022). Our recruitment strategy targeted two groups: mothers reporting a history of recurrent MDD and mothers with no history of MDD. Eligibility was verified through a phone screening, using an adapted version of the Mini International Neuropsychiatry Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller and Dunbar1998). To be included in the study, mothers needed to have a daughter aged 10–19 years old, live with them at least 50% of the time, and both had to be able to read and write in English. For all mothers, exclusion criteria consisted of (i) a history of head injury resulting in loss of consciousness for more than 10 minutes and (ii) a history of psychosis or mania, or current substance abuse. Additionally, for mothers without a history of recurrent MDD, a lifetime history of any psychiatric disorder was exclusionary (except for specific phobia (n = 3 in this group), given that it is less heritable, impairing, and predictive of children’s risk for depression compared to other disorders (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath and Eaves1992)). Daughters with a history of head injury resulting in loss of consciousness for more than 10 minutes were also excluded. Following screening, 109 mother-daughter dyads were invited to the lab for testing. Maternal eligibility was updated after enrollment by interviews with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–5 (SCID-5; First et al., Reference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer2016). Further exclusion criteria from the analyses of the current study were (i) no member of the dyad completed the Flanker task (n = 7), and (ii) no member of the dyad had valid EEG data with sufficient error trials after data preprocessing (n = 5). Thus, 97 mother-daughter dyads were included in the current study (44 mothers had a lifetime history of recurrent MDD). Demographics of the sample are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of study participants

Note: Income is reported in Canadian dollars (CAD). IDAS-II: Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms-II, MDD: major depressive disorder, MFQ: Mood and Feelings Questionnaire, SD: standard deviation.

** : p < .01, ***: p < .001

Mothers provided written informed consent for themselves and written parental consent for their daughter (if aged less than 18 years old). Daughters aged less than 18 years old provided assent and those aged 18 and above provided consent. All procedures were approved by McGill University ethics board and the study was conducted according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Procedures

Clinical interviews

Semi-structured interviews were conducted in both mothers (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders (SCID-5); First et al., Reference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer2016) and daughters (Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID); Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Sheehan, Shytle, Janavs, Bannon, Rogers and Wilkinson2010) to identify diagnoses of current and lifetime anxiety, mood, obsessive-compulsive, psychotic, substance use, trauma and stressor-related disorders. Interviews were conducted by graduate students and supervised by AW. Interrater reliability (Landis & Koch, Reference Landis and Koch1977) for unipolar mood disorders, which are of primary interest in the current study, was assessed by 4 interviewers from a subset (n = 20) of interviews and was found to range from κ = .70 in daughters to κ = .88 in mothers.

Measures

Participants completed questionnaires assessing their own current depression symptoms. In adolescent daughters, depression symptoms were assessed with the 32-item Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ; Angold et al., Reference Angold, Costello, Messer and Pickles1995), which showed excellent internal consistency in our sample (α = .95). Mothers’ current depression symptoms were assessed with the 20-item General Depression scale from the expanded version of the Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms (IDAS-II; Watson et al., Reference Watson, O'Hara, Naragon-Gainey, Koffel, Chmielewski, Kotov and Ruggero2012). This scale demonstrated high internal consistency in our sample (α = .92).

Flanker task

Participants performed an arrow version of the Flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, Reference Eriksen and Eriksen1974). Stimuli were presented and responses were recorded with the Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.; Albany, CA), running on an Intel Core i7 computer. On each trial of the task, 5 horizontally aligned arrowheads were presented at the center of a 19-inch computer monitor. The central arrowhead was the target stimulus and the two arrowheads on each side were the flankers. Half of the trials were congruent (i.e. the target and flanker stimuli point in the same direction) and the other half were incongruent (i.e. the target and flanker stimuli point in opposite directions). Trial order was randomized throughout the task. Participants were instructed to press the left or right button on a mouse according to the direction of the target stimulus. On each trial, stimuli were presented for 200 ms. A white fixation cross was then presented on a black screen for 1,800 ms or until a response was given. The intertrial interval then followed, which also consisted of a white fixation cross on a black screen for a duration ranging randomly from 1,000 to 2,000 ms.

Before beginning the task, participants performed a 6-trial practice session. They were told to be as fast and accurate as possible. Participants then performed the actual task, which was separated in five blocks of 30 trials, for a total of 150 trials. Performance-based feedback was displayed on-screen at the end of each block. If accuracy was 80% or above, participants were instructed to “Please try to respond faster,” in order to increase the likelihood of committing errors. If accuracy was between 76 and 79%, the feedback was “You are doing a great job.” If accuracy was 75% or below, participants were instructed to “Please try to be more accurate.” Analyses pertaining to behavioral performance are presented in the Supplement.

EEG recordings

EEG was recorded at 1000Hz sampling rate from 32 electrodes mounted in the Easycap and placed according to the 10/20 system. The ground electrode was placed at FPz. Signals were recorded with BrainVision actiCHamp amplifier and Pycorder recording software (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). Electrical conducting gel was used to maintain electrode impedance below 10 kΩ throughout the recordings.

EEG signal treatment and feature extraction

EEG preprocessing

Continuous EEG signals were preprocessed with the Maryland Analysis of Developmental EEG (MADE) pipeline (Debnath et al., Reference Debnath, Buzzell, Morales, Bowers, Leach and Fox2020), running on Matlab 2020a. The MADE pipeline relies on EEGLAB’s (Delorme & Makeig, Reference Delorme and Makeig2004) functions and data structure. Data were preprocessed separately for event-related potentials (ERP) and time-frequency analyses. The first step consists of offline filtering using EEGLAB’s firfilt plugin. Continuous data were filtered with a Hamming window finite impulse response filter, with a 0.1 Hz high-pass filter (0.1 Hz transition width). A 30 Hz low-pass filter was used for ERP analyses and a 50 Hz low-pass filter was used for time-frequency analyses (10 Hz transition width in both cases). The stopband attenuation was 53 dB. The channel_properties function from EEGLAB’s FASTER plugin (Nolan et al., Reference Nolan, Whelan and Reilly2010) was used to identify and remove flat or noisy channels according to three-values: Hurst exponent, correlation with other channels, and channel variance. After this step, residual flat channels were removed from the data if their standard deviation across the whole recording was smaller than 1. Independent component analysis (ICA) was used to remove non-neural artifacts such as blinks, saccades, and muscle artifacts. Given that ICA performs better on data filtered with a 1 Hz high-pass filter, the ICA was performed on a copy of the recordings. This copied dataset was first epoched into 1-second segments. Those 1-second epochs with very low/high amplitude (± 1000 μV) or excessive EMG activity (below −30 and above 100 dB between 20–40 Hz) were marked as bad epochs. Channels containing more than 20% of these epochs, suggesting bad channels missed by FASTER, were removed from both the copied and the original datasets. These bad epochs were then removed from the copied dataset. ICA was then conducted on the copied dataset with EEGLAB’s runica function. ICA weights were transferred to the original dataset. The adjusted-ADJUST toolbox (Leach et al., Reference Leach, Morales, Bowers, Buzzell, Debnath, Beall and Fox2020; Mognon et al., Reference Mognon, Jovicich, Bruzzone and Buiatti2011) was used to automatically identify artifactual independent components in the original dataset, which were then rejected. Continuous data were epoched from −500 ms to 1,000 ms relative to response onset for ERP analyses, and from −1,000 ms to 2,000 ms for time-frequency analyses. Longer epochs are needed for time-frequency analyses, which however comes with the cost of losing participants due to artifacts in the longer epochs (see results). A threshold-based rejection method was used to deal with residual artifacts. For frontal electrodes near the eyes (Fp1, Fp2), any epoch with voltage exceeding ±100 μV was removed. For other electrodes, epochs for which voltage exceeded ±100 μV in more than 10% of electrodes were removed. For remaining epochs, individual channels were interpolated with a spherical spline procedure at the epoch level if voltage exceeded the ±100 μV threshold. Channels that were removed in the first steps of the pipeline were also interpolated with a spherical spline procedure. Finally, electrodes were re-referenced to the average of both mastoids (TP9 and TP10).

Feature extraction

ERP analyses were performed with ERPLAB (Lopez-Calderon & Luck, Reference Lopez-Calderon and Luck2014). Error and correct trials were averaged separately and baseline-corrected (−500 to −300 ms). Only participants with at least 6 error trials were included (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Riesel and Proudfit2013; Olvet & Hajcak, Reference Olvet and Hajcak2009; Sandre et al., Reference Sandre, Banica, Riesel, Flake, Klawohn and Weinberg2020). In the current study, mothers committed fewer errors than daughters. We therefore excluded more mothers than daughters from EEG analyses due to insufficient error trials. Among the 97 dyads included in this study, 90 daughters and 55 mothers had at least 6 error trials. Mothers with insufficient trials did not differ from those with at least 6 trials regarding depression symptoms or history of recurrent MDD (all p-values > .31). Given these sample sizes, our study was powered to detect medium group differences (d ≈ .6) and correlations (r ≈ .3) in daughters, and large groups differences (d ≈ .8) and medium correlations (r ≈ .35) in mothers.

A difference wave was computed by subtracting the ERPs to correct responses from that of errors. The ΔERN was assessed as the difference wave’s mean amplitude in the 0–100 post-response interval at electrode Cz. Time-frequency transforms were performed with the time-frequency principal components analysis (TF-PCA) approach (Bernat et al., Reference Bernat, Williams and Gehring2005; Buzzell et al., Reference Buzzell, Niu, Aviyente and Bernat2022). First, and similarly to the procedure used in Buzzell et al. (Reference Buzzell, Barker, Troller-Renfree, Bernat, Bowers, Morales and Fox2019), we used a subsampling procedure to match the number of trials across conditions. Six trials were randomly selected without replacement across all valid trials in a condition and averaged. This procedure was repeated 25 times. The resulting 25 estimates were bootstrapped 100 times; the mean of the bootstrapped samples was used in further analyses. Second, delta and theta frequencies were filtered separately, given that lower frequencies (such as delta) have larger magnitudes than higher frequencies. Thus, for delta TF-PCA analyses, data were filtered with a 4 Hz Butterworth low-pass filter. For theta TF-PCA analyses, data were filtered with a 2 Hz Butterworth high-pass filter. Theta was averaged from 4 medial frontal electrodes (Fz, Cz, FC1, FC2), whereas delta was averaged from 4 medial parietal electrodes (Cz, Pz, CP1, CP2). Given that the highest frequency of interest in the current study was theta, data were downsampled to 32 Hz to reduce computing time.

Following this, Cohen’s class reduced interference distributions was used to obtain a time-frequency decomposition of response-locked average power across trials of the two different conditions (i.e., error and correct trials). For both delta and theta frequency bands, the first PCA factor was selected. Like the approach taken by Buzzell et al. (Reference Buzzell, Barker, Troller-Renfree, Bernat, Bowers, Morales and Fox2019), we used a hybrid approach where we derived PCA factors from averaged power and applied those factors to total power. Total power, unlike average power, is computed at the trial-level before being averaged across trials and thus includes both phase-locked and non-phased locked brain activity (Cohen, Reference Cohen2014).

Similar to the ΔERN, brain oscillations to correct responses were subtracted from that of errors to obtain the Δdelta and Δtheta. Split-half reliability coefficients for the EEG measures of interest fell in the moderate range in daughters (ΔERN: r = .55; Δdelta: r = .51; Δtheta: r = .59) and in the low to moderate range in mothers (ΔERN: r = .46; Δdelta: r = .59; Δtheta: r = .49).

Statistical analyses

For our first objective, t-tests were performed to compare EEG markers of error monitoring (ΔERN, Δdelta, and Δtheta) between mothers with and without a lifetime history of depression. Pearson’s correlations were also performed between mothers’ EEG markers of error monitoring and their current symptoms of depression. For our second objective, Pearson’s correlations were performed between daughters’ and mothers’ EEG markers of error monitoring. For our third objective, t-tests were performed to compare daughters’ EEG markers of error monitoring according to their mother’s lifetime history of recurrent MDD. Pearson’s correlations were also performed between daughters’ EEG markers of error monitoring and their mothers’ current symptoms of depression. Since these latter analyses aimed to identify whether error monitoring mechanisms in mothers may constitute a risk marker for future depression in their daughters, independent of daughters’ mood states, daughters with a lifetime history of depression (n = 25) were excluded from this set of analyses, leaving 67 daughters for these analyses. Finally, all analyses were repeated while taking into account several other potential confounding factors, such maternal symptoms of anxiety and history of anxiety disorders, daughters’ symptoms of anxiety and history of anxiety disorders, maternal and daughters’ age, as well as family income (Online supplement).

Results

Measures of depression symptoms

All bivariate associations between study variables are presented in Table 2. From this table, we can see that there was no concordance in depression symptoms between mothers and daughters. According to established cutoffs of ≥29 for the MFQ (Daviss et al., Reference Daviss, Birmaher, Melhem, Axelson, Michaels and Brent2006) and ≥47 for the IDAS General Depression scale (De la Rosa-Cáceres et al., Reference De la Rosa-Cáceres, Lozano, Sanchez-Garcia, Fernandez-Calderon, Rossi and Diaz-Batanero2023; Stasik-O’Brien et al., Reference Stasik-O’Brien, Brock, Chmielewski, Naragon-Gainey, Koffel, McDade-Montez and Watson2018), 16 daughters and 26 mothers reported clinically significantly levels of depression.

Table 2. Bivariate associations between study variables

Note: IDAS-II: Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms-II, M: mean, MFQ: Mood and Feelings Questionnaire, SD: standard deviation. *: p < .05, **: p < .01.

Impact of lifetime depression history and current depression on EEG correlates of error monitoring in mothers

Fifty-five mothers had valid ERP data and 53 had valid time-frequency data. Among those, 23 had a lifetime history of recurrent MDD. We found that mothers with a lifetime history of recurrent MDD had reduced Δdelta [t(50) = 2.09, p = .042, d = .59] and increased Δtheta [t(50) = −2.18, p = .034, d = .58] power relative to those without a history of recurrent MDD. There was no significant between-group difference in ΔERN amplitude [t(52) = 1.14, p = .26, d = .31]. Similarly, we found that current depression symptoms, as assessed by the IDAS-II General Depression scale, were significantly associated with decreased Δdelta [r(50) = −.28, p = .045] and increased Δtheta [r(50 = .28, p = .049] power across the whole sample of mothers with valid EEG data. There was no significant association between depression symptoms and ΔERN amplitude [r(52) = −.20, p = .15] (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Mothers’ electroencephalogram (EEG) correlates of error-monitoring according to lifetime history/current symptoms of depression. ERP waveforms and time-frequency plots for Δdelta and Δtheta depicts EEG correlates of error-monitoring in mothers with and without a lifetime history of recurrent major depressive disorder. Scalp maps shows the topographical distribution of error-minus correct difference in the 0–100 ms post-response interval (ERN panel) and of the first component from TF-PCA (delta and theta panels). The scatterplots on the right show the associations between current depression symptoms and EEG correlates of error-monitoring.

Familiality of electrophysiological correlates of error-monitoring

Across our sample, there were 48 dyads with valid ERP data for both the daughter and the mother, and 46 with valid time-frequency data. Correlation analyses revealed that mothers’ and daughters’ ΔERN were significantly correlated [r(46) = .41, p = .004] (Figure 2). However, Δdelta [r(44) = .14, p = .34] and Δtheta [r(44) = .25, p = .095] were not significantly correlated in mother-daughter dyads.

Figure 2. Associations between mother’s and daughters’ neural response to errors. The left part of the figure shows ERP waveforms and Δdelta and Δtheta time-frequency plots for mother-daughter dyads, as well as topographical distribution of the error minus correct difference for ERPs/electroencephalogram (EEG) power. The scatterplots on the right show associations between mothers’ and daughters’ EEG correlates of error-monitoring.

Association between mothers’ lifetime history of depression and current depression symptoms and daughters’ neural responses to errors

After excluding daughters with a lifetime history of depression, there were 67 daughters remaining with valid EEG data. Among those, 31 (46.3%) had a mother with a history of recurrent MDD. In daughters, none of the three neural measures were significantly associated with a lifetime history of recurrent MDD in mothers [ΔERN: t(65) = 1.28, p = .21, d = .31; Δdelta: t(65) = 1.75, p = .086, d = .71; Δtheta: t(65) = 0.45, p = .66, d = .11]. Additionally, in daughters, neither ΔERN [r(62) = −.17, p = .19] nor Δtheta [r(62) = .11, p = .41] was associated with maternal depression symptoms. However, daughters’ Δdelta response was significantly correlated with maternal symptoms of depression [r(62) = −.37, p = .002], such that daughters of mothers with more current symptoms of depression showed decreased delta power following errors (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Link between mothers’ lifetime history of depression/current depression symptoms on daughters’ electroencephalogram (EEG) markers of error monitoring. ERP waveforms and time-frequency plots for Δdelta and Δtheta depicts EEG correlates of error-monitoring in daughters according to their mothers’ lifetime history of recurrent major depressive disorder. Scalp maps shows the topographical distribution of the error minus correct difference for ERPs/EEG power. The scatterplots on the right shows the associations between mothers’ current depression symptoms and daughters’ EEG correlates of error-monitoring.

We also conducted several sensitivity analyses to assess the effect of other variables on our primary analyses. When accounting for maternal anxiety symptoms, most results remained significant. When accounting for maternal history of anxiety disorder, only the association between maternal depression symptoms and Δdelta in daughters, as well as the analysis pertaining to the familiality of the ERN, remained significant (Table S1). Nevertheless, all effects remained of similar magnitude and in the same direction. Those differences when including maternal history of anxiety as a covariate may be due to low power given that few (n = 16) mothers had a history of anxiety disorder. All analyses remained significant when controlling for daughters’ anxiety symptoms and history of anxiety disorder (Table S2). Finally, most results remained significant when controlling for mothers’ and daughters’ age, family income, and daughters’ current symptoms of depression (Table S3 and Table S4).”

Discussion

In the current study, our aim was to better understand familial transmission of depression, and whether neural measures of error monitoring might be implicated in this transmission. To that end, we first looked at whether these neural measures differed in mothers with a history of recurrent depression. Next, we examined the extent to which these neural responses were correlated between mothers and daughters. Finally, we examined the extent to which maternal history of depression and current symptoms of depression were associated with neural measures of error-monitoring in daughters.

First, we found that mothers with a history of recurrent depression had reduced delta and increased theta power relative to mothers without a history of depression, although these groups did not differ significantly in terms of ERN amplitude. Mothers’ current symptoms of depression also correlated significantly with EEG power in these two frequency bands, but not with the ERN. Although error-related delta and theta have only rarely been assessed in relation with depression, these results contrast somewhat with two previous studies in adults and adolescents. One of these reported no significant associations between theta and delta power and depression symptoms in an adult sample enriched for MDD and generalized anxiety disorder (Muir et al., Reference Muir, Hedges-Muncy, Clawson, Carbine and Larson2020). The other, in adolescents, reported decreased theta associated with depression, but, contrary to hypotheses, no significant reduction in delta power (Dell'Acqua et al., Reference Dell’Acqua, Brush, Burani, Santopetro, Klawohn, Messerotti Benvenuti and Hajcak2023). Further research is needed, but one possible explanation for these results is that the increases in theta and reductions in delta power observed in the current study appear later in life and are associated with the duration and recurrence of depressive episodes. Future studies in larger clinical samples will be necessary to explore this question.

The results in mothers are also consistent with previous evidence that error-related oscillations in delta and theta bands reflect at least partially distinct processes (Yordanova et al., Reference Yordanova, Falkenstein, Hohnsbein and Kolev2004). On the one hand, the increase in error-related theta may suggest that signals indicating the need for cognitive control (Cavanagh & Frank, Reference Cavanagh and Frank2014) are preserved or even enhanced in mothers with a history of depression. On the other hand, delta oscillations have been suggested to reflect strategic adjustments in behavior after the need for cognitive control has been signaled (Sandre & Weinberg, Reference Sandre and Weinberg2019). Given that delta oscillations are known to be involved in motivational processes (Knyazev, Reference Knyazev2007, Reference Knyazev2012) and that error-related delta may reflect the motivational salience of errors (Umemoto et al., Reference Umemoto, Cole, Allison, Dolan, Lazarov, Auerbach and Schneier2021), decreased error-related delta in mothers with a history of depression may reflect decreased motivation to implement cognitive control. Also, while blunted error-related delta has not yet been reported in depression, blunted delta power has been observed in other contexts, such as following monetary reward (Foti et al., Reference Foti, Weinberg, Bernat and Proudfit2015; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Infantolino, Klein, Perlman, Kotov and Hajcak2018), social feedback (Jin et al., Reference Jin, Sabharwal, Infantolino, Jarcho and Nelson2019), or when viewing pleasant pictures (Dell’Acqua, Brush, et al., 2022; Dell’Acqua, Dal Bò, et al., 2022). Decreased response to and engagement with both positive and negative stimuli and events is consistent with the emotion context insensitivity theory of depression, which suggests that depressed individuals have decreased emotional reactivity (Bylsma et al., Reference Bylsma, Morris and Rottenberg2008; Bylsma, Reference Bylsma2021; Rottenberg et al., Reference Rottenberg, Gross and Gotlib2005). Thus, given that blunted delta is observed not only in the context of error monitoring but for other psychological processes as well, it may represent a more general effect of reduced motivation associated with depression.

We also found significant concordance of neural responses to errors in mother-daughter dyads, although only for the ERN. While there was no such significant familial concordance for power in the delta and theta bands, the associations were in the expected direction. It should be noted that our familiality analyses were limited by a small number of dyads in which both members had sufficient error trials. Nevertheless, concordance of the ERN amplitude is consistent with prior studies showing similar associations in mother-child dyads (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Fisher, Hicks, Zucker and Durbin2018; Suor et al., Reference Suor, Calentino, Granros and Burkhouse2022). The similarity in the neural response to errors in mothers and daughters could partially be explained by similarity in brain structures. For instance, intergenerational concordance of gray matter structure in several regions brain regions, including the anterior cingulate cortex – an important generator of the ERN – has been reported in depressed mothers and their never-depressed daughters (Minami et al., Reference Minami, Hirano, Ueda, Takamiya, Yamagishi, Kamiya and Yamagata2022). However, parenting behaviors associated with depression have also been associated with variation in the ERN in offspring (Banica et al., Reference Banica, Sandre and Weinberg2019; Brooker & Buss, Reference Brooker and Buss2014; Chong et al., Reference Chong, Mirzadegan and Meyer2020; Meyer & Wissemann, Reference Meyer and Wissemann2020), suggesting another pathway for familial transmission of error monitoring processes; in other words, rather than direct transmission of biological risk for atypical brain function, it may be that parents with a blunted ERN parent in a way that results in a blunted ERN in their offspring. Finally, certain psychological characteristics that have been associated with an enhanced ERN, such as perfectionism (Meyer & Wissemann, Reference Meyer and Wissemann2020) or the tendency to worry (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Moran, Schroder, Donnellan and Yeung2013), can be transmitted from parents to children through observational learning (Carmo et al., Reference Carmo, Oliveira, Brás and Faísca2021; Pavlova et al., Reference Pavlova, Bagnell, Cumby, Vallis, Abidi, Lovas and Uher2022). Thus, the mother-daughter concordance in ERN amplitude might also reflect vicarious transmission of specific psychological traits that in turn enhance the ERN. Future longitudinal studies will be helpful in identifying specific mechanisms of intergenerational transmission of neural responses to errors.

Finally, we also assessed how the brain correlates of error monitoring in daughters were associated with their mothers’ history of depression or current depression symptoms. For this set of analyses, we excluded daughters with a history or current diagnosis of MDD, given that it can be challenging when someone has already met criteria for MDD to know whether observed alterations to neural markers precede symptoms, are themselves a symptom of the disorder, or are a consequence of the disorder. In contrast to previous work (e.g., Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Bress, Hajcak and Gibb2018), we did not find an association between adolescent’s ΔERN and their mother’s symptoms of depression. However, mothers’ depression symptoms were significantly associated with daughters’ error-related delta power. Also, while error-related delta power did not significantly differ in daughters according to maternal history of recurrent MDD, we found a medium-to-large effect size (d = .71), suggesting that we may have lacked the necessary power to detect a difference. This also highlights the importance of examining continuous relationships between neural response to errors and depression symptoms, as it allows assessing the whole spectrum of depression severity and the dimensional nature of depression (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Tiemeier and Krueger2022). Only focusing on diagnoses overlooks the functional impairment associated with subthreshold symptoms of depression (Balázs et al., Reference Balázs, Miklósi, Keresztény, Hoven, Carli, Wasserman and Wasserman2013; Cuijpers & Smit, Reference Cuijpers and Smit2004). Nevertheless, diagnostic measures allow to capture information about depression across the lifespan. We thus advocate for a balanced use of both diagnoses and dimensional measures. As previously noted, there seems to be a consistent association between depression symptoms and blunted delta across tasks tapping different motivational and affective processes. For instance, in addition to error processing, blunted delta in depression has been associated with other performance monitoring processes, such as reward processing (Foti et al., Reference Foti, Weinberg, Bernat and Proudfit2015; Jin et al., Reference Jin, Sabharwal, Infantolino, Jarcho and Nelson2019; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Infantolino, Klein, Perlman, Kotov and Hajcak2018). We may thus see similar effects across metrics of different performance monitoring processes. However, such a phenomenon may not be specific to performance monitoring, as blunted delta during affective picture viewing has also been observed (Dell’Acqua, Brush, et al., 2022; Dell’Acqua, Dal Bò, et al., 2022), suggesting broad motivational disengagement. Blunted error-related delta in adolescent girls whose mothers have increased depression symptoms may thus constitute a risk marker for future depression, especially since blunted delta was associated with depression in their mothers, and given that blunted reward-related delta prospectively predicts depression onset in adolescents girls (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Infantolino, Klein, Perlman, Kotov and Hajcak2018).

The current results also raise some important questions. For example, although daughters’ and mothers’ error-related delta power were both associated with maternal depression symptoms, significant familial concordance of error-related neural response was only found for the ERN. While those differences may partly be attributable to a small sample size, it also raises questions about the differences in the functional significance of the ERN, delta, and theta, and how those differences affect familial transmission of the risk for depression. Previous work has shown that time-frequency decomposition of ERPs may reveal unique associations between neural measures and individual differences (Cavanagh et al., Reference Cavanagh, Meyer and Hajcak2017). Thus, it is possible that some processes that are specifically indexed by error-related delta, such as evaluation of the motivational salience of errors (Umemoto et al., Reference Umemoto, Cole, Allison, Dolan, Lazarov, Auerbach and Schneier2021), are particularly involved in the risk for developing depression. Additionally, though we only observed significant familial concordance for the ERN and not for error-related delta/theta, associations for spectral measures were in the expected direction. It is possible that the ERN receives additional contributions from neural oscillations outside the delta/theta range (Carp & Compton, Reference Carp and Compton2009; Koelewijn et al., Reference Koelewijn, van Schie, Bekkering, Oostenveld and Jensen2008; Völker et al., Reference Völker, Fiederer, Berberich, Hammer, Behncke, Kršek and Ball2018), and that power in these frequency bands may be more concordant in mother-daughter dyads.

We must also note that although error-related delta and theta were associated with depression symptoms in mothers, we did not find such associations in daughters for any of our neural measures of response to errors. This may be related to the fact that, for these analyses, we excluded daughters who had already had a depressive episode. Though this decision was made to better explore the association between neural markers of performance monitoring and risk for depression, it may also have obscured associations between these markers and daughters’ current symptoms. In this sample of adolescent girls, blunted error-related delta may also better reflect risk for future increases in depression. For instance, prior research has shown an association between a blunted ERN and increases in depression symptoms over time, without a significant cross-sectional correlation at baseline (Sandre et al., Reference Sandre, Banica and Weinberg2023). Since this study was cross-sectional, we cannot assess whether the intergenerational transmission of the neural response to errors is associated with the development of depression in adolescence. Future studies should use a longitudinal design among mother-daughter dyads to test this hypothesis. These results must also be interpreted in the light of further limitations. First, our sample only included mothers and their adolescent daughters. While internalizing disorders are more common in women and girls and thus the mother-daughter dyad is the most at risk for intergenerational transmission of depression (Connell & Goodman, Reference Connell and Goodman2002), our experimental design precludes assessment of the mother-son and father-child relationships. Recent research has found links between the father-child relationship and offspring internalizing symptoms (Bilodeau-Houle et al., Reference Bilodeau-Houle, Bouchard, Morand-Beaulieu, Herringa, Milad and Marin2020; Gregory et al., Reference Gregory, Kivisto, Perdue and Estell2019), as well as paternal transmission of depression (Dachew et al., Reference Dachew, Ayano, Duko, Lawrence, Betts and Alati2023), highlighting the importance of looking at dyadic relationships with both parents. Additionally, mothers committed fewer errors than daughters, resulting in more data loss in mothers and a smaller sample size in analyses that included EEG data from mothers. Future studies might explore ways to encourage participants to better balance accuracy and speed to result in sufficient error trials. However, in such tasks, participants from different experimental groups may use different strategies (e.g., Wylie et al., Reference Wylie, van den Wildenberg, Ridderinkhof, Bashore, Powell, Manning and Wooten2009), therefore introducing additional experimental challenges.

Despite these limitations, our results may have relevant clinical implications. Prior research has shown that targeted interventions can affect the amplitude of the ERN (Klawohn et al., Reference Klawohn, Hajcak, Amir, Kathmann and Riesel2020; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Gibby, Wissemann, Klawohn, Hajcak and Schmidt2020; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Jackson, Amir and Hajcak2017). If a blunted neural response to errors constitutes a vulnerability for depression that is transmitted from mother to daughter, early assessment of that response and intervention to change it may have beneficial impact in youth. Future research should assess whether increasing a previously blunted neural response to errors can protect from developing depression later in life. In sum, results from this study suggest that neural response to errors is impacted by history of recurrent depression and current depression symptoms in adult women, is transmitted in mother-daughter dyads, and, in adolescent girls, is associated with their mothers’ current symptoms of depression. Future longitudinal studies will be necessary to assess whether neural responses to errors, especially delta power, can prospectively predict the onset of depression in adolescents.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001780.

Acknowledgements

We wish to express our gratitude to mother-daughter dyads who participated in this study and to research assistants who helped with data collection.

Funding statement

This project was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) through the Canada Research Chair in Clinical Neuroscience (awarded to AW). SMB was supported by a CIHR fellowship (MFE164627). IB was supported by a doctoral training award from the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Société et culture. CF and AS were supported by doctoral training awards from the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Santé. PE was supported by a CIHR doctoral training award.

Competing interests

The authors report no conflict of interest.

References

Aarts, K., Vanderhasselt, M. A., Otte, G., Baeken, C., & Pourtois, G. (2013). Electrical brain imaging reveals the expression and timing of altered error monitoring functions in major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(4), 939950. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034616 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., Messer, S. C., & Pickles, A. (1995). Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5(4), 237249 Google Scholar
Anokhin, A. P., Golosheykin, S., & Heath, A. C. (2008). Heritability of frontal brain function related to action monitoring. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 524534. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00664.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balázs, J., Miklósi, M., Keresztény, A., Hoven, C. W., Carli, V., Wasserman, C., & Wasserman, D. (2013). Adolescent subthreshold-depression and anxiety: Psychopathology, functional impairment and increased suicide risk. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines, 54(6), 670677. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banica, I., Sandre, A., & Weinberg, A. (2019). Overprotective/authoritarian maternal parenting is associated with an enhanced error-related negativity (ERN) in emerging adult females. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 137, 1220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.12.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernat, E. M., Williams, W. J., & Gehring, W. J. (2005). Decomposing ERP time-frequency energy using PCA. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116(6), 13141334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2005.01.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bilodeau-Houle, A., Bouchard, V., Morand-Beaulieu, S., Herringa, R. J., Milad, M. R., & Marin, M. F. (2020). Anxiety sensitivity moderates the association between father-child relationship security and fear transmission. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 579514. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.579514 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brázdil, M., Roman, R., Daniel, P., & Rektor, I. (2005). Intracerebral error-related negativity in a simple go/NoGo task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19(4), 244255. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.19.4.244 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooker, R. J., & Buss, K. A. (2014). Harsh parenting and fearfulness in toddlerhood interact to predict amplitudes of preschool error-related negativity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 148159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burwell, S. J., Malone, S. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2016). One-year developmental stability and covariance among oddball, novelty, go/no-go, and flanker event-related potentials in adolescence: A monozygotic twin study. Psychophysiology, 53(7), 9911007. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buzzell, G. A., Barker, T. V., Troller-Renfree, S. V., Bernat, E. M., Bowers, M. E., Morales, S., & Fox, N. A. (2019). Adolescent cognitive control, theta oscillations, and social observation. NeuroImage, 198, 1330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.077 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buzzell, G. A., Niu, Y., Aviyente, S., & Bernat, E. (2022). A practical introduction to EEG time-frequency principal components analysis (TF-PCA). Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 55, 101114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bylsma, L. M. (2021). Emotion context insensitivity in depression: Toward an integrated and contextualized approach. Psychophysiology, 58(2), e13715. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13715 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bylsma, L. M., Morris, B. H., & Rottenberg, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4), 676691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carmo, C., Oliveira, D., Brás, M., & Faísca, L. (2021). The influence of parental perfectionism and parenting styles on child perfectionism. Children (Basel), 8(9), 777. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8090777 Google ScholarPubMed
Carp, J., & Compton, R. J. (2009). Alpha power is influenced by performance errors. Psychophysiology, 46(2), 336343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00773.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrasco, M., Harbin, S. M., Nienhuis, J. K., Fitzgerald, K. D., Gehring, W. J., & Hanna, G. L. (2013). Increased error-related brain activity in youth with obsessive-compulsive disorder and unaffected siblings. Depression and Anxiety, 30(1), 3946. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22035 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, J. F., & Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 414421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, J. F., Meyer, A., & Hajcak, G. (2017). Error-specific cognitive control alterations in generalized anxiety disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging, 2(5), 413420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.01.004 Google ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, J. F., & Shackman, A. J. (2015). Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: Meta-analytic evidence. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 109(1-3), 315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiu, P. H., & Deldin, P. J. (2007). Neural evidence for enhanced error detection in major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(4), 608616. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.608 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chong, L. J., Mirzadegan, I. A., & Meyer, A. (2020). The association between parenting and the error-related negativity across childhood and adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45, 100852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100852 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayson, P. E. (2020). Moderators of the internal consistency of error-related negativity scores: A meta-analysis of internal consistency estimates. Psychophysiology, 57(8), e13583. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13583 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayson, P. E., Rocha, H. A., McDonald, J. B., Baldwin, S. A., & Larson, M. J. (2024). A registered report of a two-site study of variations of the flanker task: ERN experimental effects and data quality. Psychophysiology, 61(9), e14607. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.14607 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. X. (2014). Analyzing neural time series data: Theory and practice. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9609.001.0001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connell, A. M., & Goodman, S. H. (2002). The association between psychopathology in fathers versus mothers and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior problems: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 746773. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.746 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connelly, J. P., & O'Connell, M. (2022). Gender differences in vulnerability to maternal depression during early adolescence: Girls appear more susceptible than boys. Psychology in the Schools, 59(2), 297315. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuijpers, P., & Smit, F. (2004). Subthreshold depression as a risk indicator for major depressive disorder: A systematic review of prospective studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 109(5), 325331. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00301.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cyranowski, J. M., Frank, E., Young, E., & Shear, M. K. (2000). Adolescent onset of the gender difference in lifetime rates of major depression: A theoretical model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(1), 2127. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.21 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dachew, B., Ayano, G., Duko, B., Lawrence, B., Betts, K., & Alati, R. (2023). Paternal depression and risk of depression among offspring: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Network Open, 6(8), e2329159. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2023.29159 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daviss, W. B., Birmaher, B., Melhem, N. A., Axelson, D. A., Michaels, S. M., & Brent, D. A. (2006). Criterion validity of the mood and feelings questionnaire for depressive episodes in clinic and non-clinic subjects. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 47(9), 927934. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01646.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De la Rosa-Cáceres, A., Lozano, O. M., Sanchez-Garcia, M., Fernandez-Calderon, F., Rossi, G., & Diaz-Batanero, C. (2023). Assessing internalizing symptoms and their relation with levels of impairment: Evidence-based cutoffs for interpreting inventory of depression and anxiety symptoms (IDAS-II) scores. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 45(1), 170180. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-022-10008-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Debnath, R., Buzzell, G. A., Morales, S., Bowers, M. E., Leach, S. C., & Fox, N. A. (2020). The Maryland analysis of developmental EEG (MADE) pipeline. Psychophysiology, 57(6), e13580. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13580 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell’Acqua, C., Brush, C. J., Burani, K., Santopetro, N. J., Klawohn, J., Messerotti Benvenuti, S., & Hajcak, G. (2022). Reduced electrocortical responses to pleasant pictures in depression: A brief report on time-domain and time-frequency delta analyses. Biological Psychology, 170, 108302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108302 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell’Acqua, C., Dal Bò, E., Moretta, T., Palomba, D., & Messerotti Benvenuti, S. (2022). EEG time-frequency analysis reveals blunted tendency to approach and increased processing of unpleasant stimuli in dysphoria. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 8161. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-12263-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell'Acqua, C., Hajcak, G., Amir, N., Santopetro, N. J., Brush, C. J., & Meyer, A. (2023). Error-related brain activity in pediatric major depressive disorder: An ERP and time-frequency investigation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 184, 100109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2023.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 921. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ethridge, P., Freeman, C., Sandre, A., Banica, I., Dirks, M. A., & Weinberg, A. (2022). Intergenerational transmission of depression risk: Mothers' neural response to reward and history of depression are associated with daughters' neural response to reward across adolescence. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 131(6), 598610. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000662 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components: II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology, 78(6), 447455. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(91)90062-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Williams, J. B. W., Karg, R. S., & Spitzer, R. L. (2016). User’s guide for the SCID-5-CV structured clinical interview for DSM-5® disorders: Clinical version. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc Google Scholar
Fissler, M., Winnebeck, E., Schroeter, T. A., Gummbersbach, M., Huntenburg, J. M., Gärtner, M., & Barnhofer, T. (2017). Brief training in mindfulness may normalize a blunted error-related negativity in chronically depressed patients. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(6), 11641175. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-017-0540-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foti, D., Weinberg, A., Bernat, E. M., & Proudfit, G. H. (2015). Anterior cingulate activity to monetary loss and basal ganglia activity to monetary gain uniquely contribute to the feedback negativity. Clinical Neurophysiology, 126(7), 13381347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2014.08.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, C., Ethridge, P., Banica, I., Sandre, A., Dirks, M. A., Kujawa, A., & Weinberg, A. (2022). Neural response to rewarding social feedback in never-depressed adolescent girls and their mothers with remitted depression: Associations with multiple risk indices. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 131(2), 141151. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000728 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehring, W. J., Liu, Y., Orr, J. M., & Carp, J. (2012). The error-related negativity (ERN/Ne). In The oxford handbook of event-related potential components (pp. 231291). Oxford University Press Google Scholar
Goodman, S. H. (2020). Intergenerational transmission of depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16, 213238. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071519-113915 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H., Buthmann, J. L., & Miller, J. G. (2023). The functioning of offspring of depressed parents: Current status, unresolved issues, and future directions. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 5, 375397. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-120621-043144 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gotlib, I. H., Goodman, S. H., & Humphreys, K. L. (2020). Studying the intergenerational transmission of risk for depression: Current status and future directions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(2), 174179. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420901590 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, J., Kivisto, K. L., Perdue, N. H., & Estell, D. B. (2019). Father-child play, child emotional dysregulation, and adolescent internalizing symptoms: A longitudinal multiple mediation analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 31(4), 13251338. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579418000767 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith, J. M., Young, J. F., & Hankin, B. L. (2021). Longitudinal coupling of depression in parent-adolescent dyads: Within- and between-dyad effects over time. Clinical Psychological Science, 9(6), 10591079. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702621998313 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanna, G. L., Liu, Y., Isaacs, Y. E., Ayoub, A. M., Brosius, A., Salander, Z., & Gehring, W. J. (2018). Error-related brain activity in adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder and major depressive disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 35(8), 752760. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22767 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, A. J., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2008). Spatiotemporal dynamics of error processing dysfunctions in major depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(2), 179188. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.19 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, A. J., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2010). Effects of task-relevant incentives on the electrophysiological correlates of error processing in major depressive disorder. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 119128. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.10.1.119 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679709. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.109.4.679 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jin, J., Sabharwal, A., Infantolino, Z. P., Jarcho, J. M., & Nelson, B. D. (2019). Time-frequency delta activity to social feedback demonstrates differential associations with depression and social anxiety symptoms. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 189. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00189 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keil, J., Weisz, N., Paul-Jordanov, I., & Wienbruch, C. (2010). Localization of the magnetic equivalent of the ERN and induced oscillatory brain activity. NeuroImage, 51(1), 404411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1992). The genetic epidemiology of phobias in women. The interrelationship of agoraphobia, social phobia, situational phobia, and simple phobia. Archives of general psychiatry, 49-018200400(4), 273281. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820040025003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Sampson, N. A., Berglund, P., Gruber, M. J., Al-Hamzawi, A., Andrade, L., & Wilcox, M. A. (2015). Anxious and non-anxious major depressive disorder in the world health organization world mental health surveys. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 24(3), 210226. https://doi.org/10.1017/s2045796015000189 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Wang, P. S., & Wittchen, H. U. (2010). The societal costs of anxiety and mood disorders: An epidemiological perspective. In Preedy, V. R., & Watson, R. R. (Ed.), Handbook of disease burdens and quality of life measures (pp. 15091525). Springer New York, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-78665-0_88 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kieffaber, P. D., Osborne, J., Norton, E., & Hilimire, M. (2023). Deconstructing the functional significance of the error-related negativity (ERN) and midline frontal theta oscillations using stepwise time-locking and single-trial response dynamics. NeuroImage, 274, 120113-120113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klawohn, J., Hajcak, G., Amir, N., Kathmann, N., & Riesel, A. (2020). Application of attentional bias modification training to modulate hyperactive error-monitoring in OCD. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 156, 7986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.07.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knyazev, G. G. (2007). Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 31(3), 377395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knyazev, G. G. (2012). EEG delta oscillations as a correlate of basic homeostatic and motivational processes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(1), 677695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.10.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ko, J. H., Ptito, A., Monchi, O., Cho, S. S., Van Eimeren, T., Pellecchia, G., & Strafella, A. P. (2009). Increased dopamine release in the right anterior cingulate cortex during the performance of a sorting task: A [11C]FLB 457 PET study. Neuroimage, 46(2), 516521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koelewijn, T., van Schie, H. T., Bekkering, H., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. (2008). Motor-cortical beta oscillations are modulated by correctness of observed action. Neuroimage, 40(2), 767775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
König, H., König, H. H., & Konnopka, A. (2019). The excess costs of depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 29, e30. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796019000180 Google ScholarPubMed
Ladouceur, C. D., Slifka, J. S., Dahl, R. E., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D. A., & Ryan, N. D. (2012). Altered error-related brain activity in youth with major depression. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(3), 351362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Tiemeier, H., & Krueger, R. F. (2022). Seven reasons why binary diagnostic categories should be replaced with empirically sounder and less stigmatizing dimensions. JCPP advances, 2(4), e12108. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcv2.12108 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawler, J. M., Hruschak, J., Aho, K., Liu, Y., Ip, K. I., Lajiness-O'Neill, R., & Fitzgerald, K. D. (2021). The error-related negativity as a neuromarker of risk or resilience in young children. Brain and Behavior, 11(3), e02008. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.2008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leach, S. C., Morales, S., Bowers, M. E., Buzzell, G. A., Debnath, R., Beall, D., & Fox, N. A. (2020). Adjusting ADJUST: Optimizing the ADJUST algorithm for pediatric data using geodesic nets. Psychophysiology, 57(8), e13566, https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13566 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, G., Rice, F., Harold, G. T., Collishaw, S., & Thapar, A. (2011). Investigating environmental links between parent depression and child depressive/anxiety symptoms using an assisted conception design. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(5), 451459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Y., Zhao, J., & Guo, W. (2018). Emotional roles of mono-aminergic neurotransmitters in major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders [Mini review]. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loechner, J., Sfärlea, A., Starman, K., Oort, F., Thomsen, L. A., Schulte-Körne, G., & Platt, B. (2020). Risk of Depression in the Offspring of Parents with Depression: The Role of Emotion Regulation, Cognitive Style, Parenting and Life Events. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 51(2), 294309. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-019-00930-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez-Calderon, J., & Luck, S. J. (2014). ERPLAB: An open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 213. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A., Bress, J. N., Hajcak, G., & Gibb, B. E. (2018). Maternal depression is related to reduced error-related brain activity in child and adolescent offspring. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 47(2), 324335. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A., Danielson, C. K., Danzig, A. P., Bhatia, V., Black, S. R., Bromet, E., & Klein, D. N. (2017). Neural biomarker and early temperament predict increased internalizing symptoms after a natural disaster. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(5), 410416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.02.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A., Gibby, B., Wissemann, K., Klawohn, J., Hajcak, G., & Schmidt, N. B. (2020). A brief, computerized intervention targeting error sensitivity reduces the error-related negativity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20(1), 172180. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00760-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A., Mehra, L., & Hajcak, G. (2021). Error-related negativity predicts increases in anxiety in a sample of clinically anxious female children and adolescents over 2 years. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 46(4), E472E479. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.200128 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, A., Riesel, A., & Proudfit, G. H. (2013). Reliability of the ERN across multiple tasks as a function of increasing errors. Psychophysiology, 50(12), 12201225. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12132 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A., & Wissemann, K. (2020). Controlling parenting and perfectionism is associated with an increased error-related negativity (ERN) in young adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(1), 8795. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minami, F., Hirano, J., Ueda, R., Takamiya, A., Yamagishi, M., Kamiya, K., & Yamagata, B. (2022). Intergenerational concordance of brain structure between depressed mothers and their never-depressed daughters. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 76(11), 579586. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.13461 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mognon, A., Jovicich, J., Bruzzone, L., & Buiatti, M. (2011). ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. Psychophysiology, 48(2), 229240. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01061.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morken, I. S., Viddal, K. R., von Soest, T., & Wichstrøm, L. (2023). Explaining the female preponderance in adolescent depression—A four-wave cohort study. Research On Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 51(6), 859869. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-023-01031-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, J. S., Fisher, M., Hicks, B. M., Zucker, R. A., & Durbin, C. E. (2018). Feedback-related neurophysiology in children and their parents: Developmental differences, familial transmission, and relationship to error-monitoring. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 132, 338352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.08.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, J. S., Moran, T. P., Schroder, H. S., Donnellan, M. B., & Yeung, N. (2013). On the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: A meta-analysis and conceptual framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 466. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00466 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muir, A. M., Hedges-Muncy, A., Clawson, A., Carbine, K. A., & Larson, M. J. (2020). Dimensions of anxiety and depression and neurophysiological indicators of error-monitoring: Relationship with delta and theta oscillatory power and error-related negativity amplitude. Psychophysiology, 57(9), e13595. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13595 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, B. D., Infantolino, Z. P., Klein, D. N., Perlman, G., Kotov, R., & Hajcak, G. (2018). Time-frequency reward-related delta prospectively predicts the development of adolescent-onset depression. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(1), 4149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.07.005 Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, B. D., Jackson, F., Amir, N., & Hajcak, G. (2017). Attention bias modification reduces neural correlates of response monitoring. Biological Psychology, 129, 103110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.08.059 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolan, H., Whelan, R., & Reilly, R. B. (2010). FASTER: Fully automated statistical thresholding for EEG artifact rejection. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 192(1), 152162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.07.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nutt, D. J. (2008). Relationship of neurotransmitters to the symptoms of major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(Suppl E1), 47.Google Scholar
O'Connell, R. G., Dockree, P. M., Bellgrove, M. A., Kelly, S. P., Hester, R., Garavan, H., & Foxe, J. J. (2007). The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: A high-density electrical mapping study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25(8), 25712579. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05477.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2008). The error-related negativity (ERN) and psychopathology: Toward an endophenotype. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(8), 13431354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2009). The stability of error-related brain activity with increasing trials [https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00848.x]. Psychophysiology, 46(5), 957961. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00848.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olvet, D. M., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2010). Depression symptom severity and error-related brain activity. Psychiatry Research, 179(1), 3037. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.06.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlova, B., Bagnell, A., Cumby, J., Vallis, E. H., Abidi, S., Lovas, D., & Uher, R. (2022). Sex-specific transmission of anxiety disorders from parents to offspring. JAMA Network Open, 5(7), e2220919e2220919. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.20919 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzetta, R., Ozkan, D. G., Era, V., Tieri, G., Zabberoni, S., Taglieri, S., & Aglioti, S. M. (2023). Combined EEG and immersive virtual reality unveil dopaminergic modulation of error monitoring in parkinson’s disease. Npj Parkinson’s Disease, 9(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41531-022-00441-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzetta, R., Wokke, M. E., Aglioti, S. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2022). Doing it wrong: A systematic review on electrocortical and behavioral correlates of error monitoring in patients with neurological disorders. Neuroscience, 486, 103125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.01.027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riesel, A. (2019). The erring brain: Error-related negativity as an endophenotype for OCD-A review and meta-analysis. Psychophysiology, 56(4), e13348. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13348 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riesel, A., Endrass, T., Kaufmann, C., & Kathmann, N. (2011). Overactive error-related brain activity as a candidate endophenotype for obsessive-compulsive disorder: Evidence from unaffected first-degree relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(3), 317324. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030416 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riesel, A., Härpfer, K., Thoma, L., Kathmann, N., & Klawohn, J. (2023). Associations of neural error-processing with symptoms and traits in a dimensional sample recruited across the obsessive-compulsive spectrum. Psychophysiology, 60(2), e14164, https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.14164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riesel, A., Klawohn, J., Grützmann, R., Kaufmann, C., Heinzel, S., Bey, K., & Kathmann, N. (2019). Error-related brain activity as a transdiagnostic endophenotype for obsessive-compulsive disorder, anxiety and substance use disorder. Psychological Medicine, 49(7), 12071217. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291719000199 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riesel, A., Weinberg, A., Endrass, T., Meyer, A., & Hajcak, G. (2013). The ERN is the ERN is the ERN? Convergent validity of error-related brain activity across different tasks. Biological Psychology, 93(3), 377385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.04.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rottenberg, J., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 627639. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.114.4.627 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salk, R. H., Hyde, J. S., & Abramson, L. Y. (2017). Gender differences in depression in representative national samples: Meta-analyses of diagnoses and symptoms. Psychological Bulletin, 143(8), 783822. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandre, A., Banica, I., Riesel, A., Flake, J., Klawohn, J., & Weinberg, A. (2020). Comparing the effects of different methodological decisions on the error-related negativity and its association with behaviour and gender. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 156, 1839. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.06.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandre, A., Banica, I., & Weinberg, A. (2023). Blunted neural response to errors prospectively predicts increased symptoms of depression during the COVID-19 pandemic. Emotion, 23(7), 19291944. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandre, A., & Weinberg, A. (2019). Neither wrong nor right: Theta and delta power increase during performance monitoring under conditions of uncertainty. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 146, 225239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.09.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoenberg, P. L. A. (2014). The error processing system in major depressive disorder: Cortical phenotypal marker hypothesis. Biological Psychology, 99, 100114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.03.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schrijvers, D., de Bruijn, E. R. A., Maas, Y., De Grave, C., Sabbe, B. G. C., & Hulstijn, W. (2008). Action monitoring in major depressive disorder with psychomotor retardation. Cortex, 44(5), 569579. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schrijvers, D., De Bruijn, E. R. A., Maas, Y. J., Vancoillie, P., Hulstijn, W., & Sabbe, B. G. C. (2009). Action monitoring and depressive symptom reduction in major depressive disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71(3), 218224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.09.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl. 20), 2233; quiz 34-57,Google ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D. V., Sheehan, K. H., Shytle, R. D., Janavs, J., Bannon, Y., Rogers, J. E., & Wilkinson, B. (2010). Reliability and validity of the mini international neuropsychiatric interview for children and adolescents (MINI-KID). The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 71(3), 313326. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.09m05305whi CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stasik-O’Brien, S. M., Brock, R. L., Chmielewski, M., Naragon-Gainey, K., Koffel, E., McDade-Montez, E., & Watson, D. (2018). Clinical utility of the inventory of depression and anxiety symptoms (IDAS). Assessment, 26(5), 944960. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191118790036 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suor, J. H., Calentino, A. E., Granros, M., & Burkhouse, K. L. (2022). Maternal error-related negativity relationship with offspring error-related negativity and negative parenting styles: A novel model of internalizing psychopathology risk. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 7(4), 435442. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.10.007 Google Scholar
Ullsperger, M., Danielmeier, C., & Jocham, G. (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. Physiological Reviews, 94(1), 3579. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00041.2012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umemoto, A., Cole, S. L., Allison, G. O., Dolan, S., Lazarov, A., Auerbach, R. P., & Schneier, F. (2021). Neurophysiological predictors of gaze-contingent music reward therapy among adults with social anxiety disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 143, 155162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.09.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Völker, M., Fiederer, L. D. J., Berberich, S., Hammer, J., Behncke, J., Kršek, P., & Ball, T. (2018). The dynamics of error processing in the human brain as reflected by high-gamma activity in noninvasive and intracranial EEG. Neuroimage, 173, 564579. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.059 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., O'Hara, M. W., Naragon-Gainey, K., Koffel, E., Chmielewski, M., Kotov, R., & Ruggero, C. J. (2012). Development and validation of new anxiety and bipolar symptom scales for an expanded version of the IDAS (the IDAS-II). Assessment, 19(4), 399420. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191112449857 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., Dieterich, R., & Riesel, A. (2015). Error-related brain activity in the age of RDoC: A review of the literature. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98(2 Pt 2), 276299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.02.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., Ethridge, P., Oumeziane, B. A., & Foti, D. (2022). Time frequency analyses in event-related potential methodologies. In Gable, P. A., Miller, M. W., & Bernat, E. M. (Ed.), The oxford handbook of EEG frequency. Oxford University Press, https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780192898340.013.5 Google Scholar
Weinberg, A., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Increased error-related brain activity distinguishes generalized anxiety disorder with and without comorbid major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4), 885896. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028270 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., Kotov, R., & Proudfit, G. H. (2015). Neural indicators of error processing in generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(1), 172185. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., Liu, H., & Shankman, S. A. (2016). Blunted neural response to errors as a trait marker of melancholic depression. Biological Psychology, 113, 100107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.11.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, A., Meyer, A., Hale-Rude, E., Perlman, G., Kotov, R., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2016). Error-related negativity (ERN) and sustained threat: Conceptual framework and empirical evaluation in an adolescent sample. Psychophysiology, 53(3), 372385. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, F.-M., & Coveñas, R. (2010). Classical neurotransmitters and neuropeptides involved in major depression: A review. International Journal of Neuroscience, 120(7), 455470. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207454.2010.483651 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wylie, S. A., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Bashore, T. R., Powell, V. D., Manning, C. A., & Wooten, G. F. (2009). The effect of speed-accuracy strategy on response interference control in parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 47(8−9), 18441853. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Y., Wu, Z., Xin, S., Gao, Y., Han, Y., Zhao, J., & Li, B. (2024). Temporal trends and age-period-cohort analysis of depression in U.S. adults from 2013 to 2022. Journal of Affective Disorders, 362, 237243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2024.06.090 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeung, M. K., Lee, T. L., & Chan, A. S. (2020). Neurocognitive development of flanker and Stroop interference control: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain and Cognition, 143, 105585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105585 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yordanova, J., Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., & Kolev, V. (2004). Parallel systems of error processing in the brain. Neuroimage, 22(2), 590602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zbozinek, T. D., Rose, R. D., Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Sherbourne, C., Sullivan, G., Stein, M. B., & Craske, M. G. (2012). Diagnostic overlap of generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder in a primary care sample. Depression and Anxiety, 29(12), 10651071. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.2026 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of study participants

Figure 1

Table 2. Bivariate associations between study variables

Figure 2

Figure 1. Mothers’ electroencephalogram (EEG) correlates of error-monitoring according to lifetime history/current symptoms of depression. ERP waveforms and time-frequency plots for Δdelta and Δtheta depicts EEG correlates of error-monitoring in mothers with and without a lifetime history of recurrent major depressive disorder. Scalp maps shows the topographical distribution of error-minus correct difference in the 0–100 ms post-response interval (ERN panel) and of the first component from TF-PCA (delta and theta panels). The scatterplots on the right show the associations between current depression symptoms and EEG correlates of error-monitoring.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Associations between mother’s and daughters’ neural response to errors. The left part of the figure shows ERP waveforms and Δdelta and Δtheta time-frequency plots for mother-daughter dyads, as well as topographical distribution of the error minus correct difference for ERPs/electroencephalogram (EEG) power. The scatterplots on the right show associations between mothers’ and daughters’ EEG correlates of error-monitoring.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Link between mothers’ lifetime history of depression/current depression symptoms on daughters’ electroencephalogram (EEG) markers of error monitoring. ERP waveforms and time-frequency plots for Δdelta and Δtheta depicts EEG correlates of error-monitoring in daughters according to their mothers’ lifetime history of recurrent major depressive disorder. Scalp maps shows the topographical distribution of the error minus correct difference for ERPs/EEG power. The scatterplots on the right shows the associations between mothers’ current depression symptoms and daughters’ EEG correlates of error-monitoring.

Supplementary material: File

Morand-Beaulieu et al. supplementary material

Morand-Beaulieu et al. supplementary material
Download Morand-Beaulieu et al. supplementary material(File)
File 30.9 KB