Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T15:55:55.825Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Microbial exposure at birth and the development of behavioral temperament during the first three years of childhood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 October 2024

Arthur Chortatos*
Affiliation:
R&D Department, Division of Mental Health Services, Akershus University Hospital, Lørenskog, Norway
Maiju Pesonen
Affiliation:
Oslo Centre for Biostatistics and Epidemiology, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Owen Thomas
Affiliation:
R&D Department, Division of Mental Health Services, Akershus University Hospital, Lørenskog, Norway
Elena Toffol
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, University of Helsinki Faculty of Medicine, Helsinki, Finland
Ville Airaksinen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Emma Musakka
Affiliation:
Institute of Public Health and Clinical Nutrition, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland Department of Health Security, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare, Kuopio, Finland
Martin Täubel
Affiliation:
Department of Health Security, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare, Kuopio, Finland
Pirkka Kirjavainen
Affiliation:
Institute of Public Health and Clinical Nutrition, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland
Katri Backman
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Kuopio University Hospital, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland Institute of Clinical Medicine, Obstetrics and Gynecology, Pediatrics and Psychiatry, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland
Juha Pekkanen
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, University of Helsinki Faculty of Medicine, Helsinki, Finland Department of Health Security, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare, Kuopio, Finland
Leea Keski-Nisula
Affiliation:
Institute of Clinical Medicine, Obstetrics and Gynecology, Pediatrics and Psychiatry, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Kuopio University Hospital, Kuopio, Finland
Soili M. Lehto
Affiliation:
R&D Department, Division of Mental Health Services, Akershus University Hospital, Lørenskog, Norway Department of Psychiatry, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland Institute of Clinical Medicine, Obstetrics and Gynecology, Pediatrics and Psychiatry, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
*
Corresponding author: Arthur Chortatos; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We investigated cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between neonate microbial exposure and emerging behavioral temperament measures at the ages of 1, 2, and 3 years. Infants and mothers (n = 335) were extracted from the Kuopio Birth Cohort Study. Temperament was assessed using the Infant Behavioral and Early Childhood Behavioral Questionnaires. Microbial samples were collected from oral cavity at birth and the bacterial profiles were assessed using 16S rRNA gene sequencing. Microbial diversity was characterized using alpha and beta diversity metrics. Analyses were performed for the most abundant genera. The sample was analyzed as a whole, as well as divided into subgroups representing no antibiotic use during birth (n = 198) and those with antibiotic use during birth (n = 137). No significant associations were observed between microbial profiles and behavioral measures after Bonferroni corrections. Nevertheless, our pre-correction results indicated an association between increased behavioral temperament surgency in the first year and beta diversity (high abundance of Bacteroides, Faecalibacterium and Blautia, low abundance of Lactobacillus) in the antibiotic use group. Additionally, pre-corrections, a high relative abundance of Staphylococcus was associated with increased surgency through years 1, 2, and 3 in the no antibiotics group, prompting consideration into a possible link between antibiotic use and emerging behavioral temperament.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Childbirth is an event often characterized by the appearance of an offspring exposed to fluids of various origin (amniotic, vaginal etc.), and it is with these fluids the neonate first encounters an array of microbial organisms that will likely influence his/her development (Heijtz et al., Reference Heijtz, Wang, Anuar, Qian, Björkholm, Samuelsson, Hibberd, Forssberg and Pettersson2011). Generally, a neonate is considered to exit the womb in a sterile state, thereafter quickly encountering millions of microorganisms from the immediate surroundings, although some findings have questioned whether the initial exposure to microbes may already have begun within the amniotic sac (Aagaard et al., Reference Aagaard, Ma, Antony, Ganu, Petrosino and Versalovic2014; Bearfield et al., Reference Bearfield, Davenport, Sivapathasundaram and Allaker2002; Dzidic et al., Reference Dzidic, Collado, Abrahamsson, Artacho, Stensson, Jenmalm and Mira2018; Rosenblatt et al., Reference Rosenblatt, Steinberg, Mankuta and Zini2015).

From the moment of birth, humans develop a symbiotic relationship with a variety of bacteria, bacteriophage, fungi, protozoa and viruses, collectively called the microbiome (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Jordan, Baker and Carlson2017). These are primarily located in the gastrointestinal tract, on the skin conjunctiva, and within the oral cavity (Sender et al., Reference Sender, Fuchs and Milo2016). Following the initial microbial exposure at birth, a proportion of the prevalent microorganisms encountered proceed further toward colonization of the newborn host (Sampaio-Maia & Monteiro-Silva, Reference Sampaio-Maia and Monteiro-Silva2014). The oral cavity serves as an initial entry point to colonization of both the oral and gut microbiota in infants, and likely serves to influence the composition of the initial gut microbiome as well (Costello et al., Reference Costello, Carlisle, Bik, Morowitz and Relman2013; D’Agostino et al., Reference D’Agostino, Ferrara, Valentini, Stoica and Dolci2022; Dal Bello & Hertel, Reference Dal Bello and Hertel2006; Dominguez-Bello et al., Reference Dominguez-Bello, Costello, Contreras, Magris, Hidalgo, Fierer and Knight2010; Xiao et al., Reference Xiao, Fiscella and Gill2020).

Samples taken from neonate skin and oral mucosa directly after birth show various bacteria capable of initiating the colonization process are already present (Dominguez-Bello et al., Reference Dominguez-Bello, Costello, Contreras, Magris, Hidalgo, Fierer and Knight2010). Moreover, it has previously been demonstrated that the mode of delivery creates an effect upon the types of bacteria to seed the microbiome (Biasucci et al., Reference Biasucci, Benenati, Morelli, Bessi and Boehm2008; Dominguez-Bello et al., Reference Dominguez-Bello, Costello, Contreras, Magris, Hidalgo, Fierer and Knight2010; Nelun Barfod et al., Reference Nelun Barfod, Magnusson, Lexner, Blomqvist, Dahlen and Twetman2011; Penders et al., Reference Penders, Thijs, Vink, Stelma, Snijders, Kummeling, van den Brandt and Stobberingh2006).

Evidence suggests that children delivered vaginally exhibit bacterial strains similar to their mother’s vaginal microbiota, whilst cesarean section (CS) deliveries manifest strains similar to their mother’s skin microbiota or those in the hospital environment (Chu et al., Reference Chu, Ma, Prince, Antony, Seferovic and Aagaard2017; Dominguez-Bello et al., Reference Dominguez-Bello, Costello, Contreras, Magris, Hidalgo, Fierer and Knight2010; Holgerson et al., Reference Holgerson, Harnevik, Hernell, Tanner and Johansson2011; Madan et al., Reference Madan, Hoen, Lundgren, Farzan, Cottingham, Morrison, Sogin, Li, Moore and Karagas2016). The routine use of prophylactic antibiotics during CS deliveries are thought to contribute to the presence of maternal skin and nosocomially acquired strains upon the neonate (Kaan et al., Reference Kaan, Kahharova and Zaura2021). The early colonizing strains are considered to have a significant impact on future health, helping to prime the mucosal immune system and further reducing vulnerability to a variety of diseases later in life, including those that affect psychological development and eventual mental health (Guarner & Malagelada, Reference Guarner and Malagelada2003; Heijtz et al., Reference Heijtz, Wang, Anuar, Qian, Björkholm, Samuelsson, Hibberd, Forssberg and Pettersson2011; Salminen & Isolauri, Reference Salminen and Isolauri2006; Turroni et al., Reference Turroni, Ribbera, Foroni, van Sinderen and Ventura2008).

There currently exist a number of proposed mechanisms thought to explain how the developing microbiome affects the emerging behavioral temperament of an infant. These range from a neural perspective, involving neurotransmitters sourced from gut bacteria, to the production of pro-inflammatory cytokines that influence brain function, as well as the production of neuroactive metabolites such as short-chain fatty acids (Kim & Shim, Reference Kim and Shim2023). Metabolites are products created when bacteria metabolize nutrients in areas of the body, and can affect the central nervous system as they are able to penetrate the blood brain barrier and directly impact central nervous system development and function (Rutsch et al., Reference Rutsch, Kantsjö and Ronchi2020). These pathways cohesively create a complex system inducing a bi-directional communication network between the central nervous system and the gut microbiome ecosystem - forming the proposed pathway whereby the microbiome influences the development of behavioral temperament - known as the gut-brain axis (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Harvey, Martin, van der Beek, Knol, Cryan and Renes2018). The gut-brain axis is susceptible to modification from these pathways, thereby disrupting communication, further impacting both physical and mental health of an individual (Alving-Jessep et al., Reference Alving-Jessep, Botchway, Wood, Hilton and Blissett2022).

A cross-sectional study by Rothenberg and associates using a sample of 46 three-year-old children found a number of varying correlations between gut sourced bacterial genera and infant mental and psychomotor neurodevelopmental indices (Rothenberg et al., Reference Rothenberg, Chen, Shen, Nong, Nong, Trinh, Biasini, Liu, Zeng, Zou, Ouyang and Korrick2021). Another cross-sectional study reported an association between surgency and a greater phylogenetic diversity for 77 infants between 18 and 27 months of age (Christian et al., Reference Christian, Galley, Hade, Schoppe-Sullivan, Dush and Bailey2015). By contrast, a recent longitudinal pilot study (n = 34) assessing a variety of brain imaging and fear behavior outcomes in infants reported no association between fear behavior as measured by the infant behavior questionnaire fear subscale and the gut microbiome in infants, sampled at one month and one year of age (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Xia, Azcarate-Peril, Rosin, Fine, Mu, Zopp, Kimmel, Styner, Thompson, Propper and Knickmeyer2021). As the majority of infant temperament studies have explored the developing stages of the microbiome using gut samples usually retrieved from ≥1 month of age (as in the aforementioned studies), research using a time-point in the earliest part of neonate microbial exposure is currently scarce, with one study reporting upon gut microbiome data from weeks 1-3 (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Lee, Wiley, Lagishetty, Sandman, Jacobs and Glynn2021). As bacteria present in the first moments of birth partly proceed to colonize the oral and subsequent gut microbiota, the present study sample seemed ideal for exploring the presently underexplored first postnatal moments. The aim of our study was therefore to explore whether neonate bacterial exposure at birth had comparable effects upon behavioral outcomes as results from studies using later microbiome samples - primarily from gut microbiota - have reported.

Methods

Study population

The present study utilizes data collected as part of the Finnish Kuopio Birth Cohort (KuBiCo) Study (www.kubico.fi). KuBiCo investigates the effects of genetics and pregnancy-related environmental, lifestyle, and psychological risk factors on the health of the mother and their offspring. KuBiCo is a co-operation project between the University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio University Hospital, and the Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare; it is an epidemiological study initiated in 2012 with an aim to collect data from approximately 10,000 mother-child pairs from the eastern and central Finnish population of approximately 1 million inhabitants. A detailed description of KuBiCo has been given elsewhere (Huuskonen et al., Reference Huuskonen, Keski-Nisula, Heinonen, Voutilainen, Tuomainen, Pekkanen, Lampi, Lehto, Haaparanta, Elomaa, Voutilainen, Backman, Kokki, Kumpulainen, Paananen, Vahakangas and Pasanen2018). The participation rate was approximately 37% of all pregnant women in the region during 2012–2017. The cohort has been analyzed and established as representing the Finnish pregnant population (Huuskonen et al., Reference Huuskonen, Keski-Nisula, Heinonen, Voutilainen, Tuomainen, Pekkanen, Lampi, Lehto, Haaparanta, Elomaa, Voutilainen, Backman, Kokki, Kumpulainen, Paananen, Vahakangas and Pasanen2018). Participants gave their written informed consent after receiving a full explanation of the study. The protocol of KuBiCo has been approved by the research ethics committee of the Central Finland Health Care District (8.12.2011, K-S SHP Dnro 18U/2011).

Selection of the study population

All children from KuBiCo with an oral cavity microbial sample collected immediately after birth and who were equal to or more than four years by the end of the year 2017 were invited to attend a five-year clinical follow-up (n = 2041). Microbial samples were sequenced and analyzed from those who attended the five-year follow-up (n = 464). Of these 464 children, 372 had at least one of the three behavioral questionnaires completed during the first three years. As mode of delivery has been shown to alter the density and counts of oral microbial species in neonates (Blum et al., Reference Blum, Silva, Byrne, Butler, Adams, Reynolds and Dashper2022), we chose to select only vaginal delivery births from this sample resulting in a sample size of n = 335. Furthermore, as the administration of intrapartum antibiotics, regardless of delivery mode, leads to alterations to the emergent microbiome (Dierikx et al., Reference Dierikx, Visser, Benninga, van Kaam, de Boer, de Vries, van Limbergen and de Meij2020), we chose to create subgroups reflecting antibiotic use (AB use; n = 137) and no antibiotic use (No AB use; n = 198) for the analyses.

Child temperament measures

The Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ) and the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ) are widely used measures of infant temperament (Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2006; Rothbart, Reference Rothbart1981). The IBQ (revised, very short form, IBQ-VFS) was used to measure the temperament of the infants at the age of 12 months, while the ECBQ (very short form, ECBQ-VSF) was used for children at 2 and 3 years of age. The IBQ-VFS consists of 37 questions employing a 1–7 Likert scale with a ‘not applicable’ option in addition. The ECBQ-VSF has 36 questions and was scored similarly to the IBQ. These measures provide three broad dimensions of behavior temperament scores: surgency, effortful control, and negative affect (NEG), and were completed by the mother.

Surgency/Positive Emotionality (SUR) is defined as the tendency to experience and express positive emotions, seek out stimulation and novelty, and approach new people and situations. Effortful control (EFF) is defined as the ability to regulate one’s attention and emotions and inhibit dominant responses in favor of subdominant responses. NEG is defined as the tendency to experience and express negative emotions such as fear, sadness, and anger (De Pauw, Reference De Pauw and Widiger2016), with low levels of EFF reported to predispose toward a later range of psychopathology, such as anxiety and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Nigg, Reference Nigg2006). The scales result in a total score whereby higher scores indicate more surgency/NEG behaviors, or else better effortful control.

Covariates

Causal directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) were used to identify the minimal sufficient adjustment set when estimating the effect of the newborn oral microbiome on behavior temperament (Figure 1). Potential covariates and associations between them, and the exposure and outcome, were defined based on subject-matter knowledge and literature search. The biasing paths were blocked by adjusting the subsequent models for maternal stress during pregnancy and maternal age. Additionally, models were chosen to be adjusted for sex of the child to yield a more precise estimate. Maternal depression scores were obtained from third trimester Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Score (EPDS) assessment, with participants responding online between 28th and 44th gestational weeks and eight weeks after delivery. The EPDS is a 10-item 4-level Likert-scale questionnaire used for screening postpartum depression in clinics (range 0–30) (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987). Maternal age and child sex data were obtained from the hospital birth register.

Figure 1. The causal directed acyclic graph (DAG) used to identify the minimal sufficient adjustment set when estimating the effect of the neonatal oral microbiome on behavior temperament (green arrow). The variables and associations included in the DAG were based on subject-matter knowledge and literature search. The biasing paths (red arrows) were blocked by adjusting the subsequent models for maternal stress during pregnancy and maternal age. Additionally, models were chosen to be adjusted for the sex of the child to yield a more precise estimate.

Microbial sampling, DNA extraction, and purification

Oral swab samples were taken in the delivery room immediately after birth, prior to palpation of the oral palate. Samples were collected by a midwife swirling a flocked swab (ESwab™, Copan, Brescia, Italy) from both inner cheeks of the newborn.

The microbial DNA was extracted using the protocol introduced as ‘method 1’ in a previous study (Yuan et al., Reference Yuan, Cohen, Ravel, Abdo and Forney2012) with minor modifications. Here we used 300 µl of the amies buffer solution as the sample volume for DNA extraction and added 30 µl of mutanolysin (5KU/ml, Sigma-Aldrich Co., USA) prior the first incubation. Further modifications from the original protocol included use of 30 µl of Proteinase K (20 mg/ml, Qiagen, Valencia, CA), 300 µl of AL buffer (Qiagen, Valencia, CA) and addition of 300 µl of ethanol prior to DNA purification with the QIAmp DNA mini kit (Qiagen, Valencia, CA) following manufacturer instructions. Per every 100 processed samples, two Eswab blanks (from two clean Eswabs using 300 µl of amies liquid solution), two positive controls (known bacterial and fungal mock communities), and two negative reagents controls were processed along with the actual samples.

Bacterial 16S rRNA gene amplification and amplicon sequencing

The DNA extracted from oral swab and control samples was shipped frozen to the sequencing service partner LGC Genomics (Germany), who did the library preparation and sequencing, similar to the protocol described elsewhere (Jayaprakash et al., Reference Jayaprakash, Adams, Kirjavainen, Karvonen, Vepsalainen, Valkonen, Jarvi, Sulyok, Pekkanen, Hyvarinen and Taubel2017). The V4-V5 region of the bacterial 16S rRNA gene was amplified in a nested approach to reduce input of eukaryotic DNA using pre-amplification with 341F-1061R (Andersson et al., Reference Andersson, Lindberg, Jakobsson, Backhed, Nyren and Engstrand2008; Herlemann et al., Reference Herlemann, Labrenz, Jurgens, Bertilsson, Waniek and Andersson2011) prior to the final amplification with 515FY-926R. DNA amplicons were sequenced on Illumina MiSeq platform with V3 chemistry resulting in paired-end reads with a length of 300 bp each. The sequence libraries were demultiplexed using Illumina’s bcl2fastq v1.8.4 software and the barcode and primer sequence removal and sorting performed with custom Python v2.7.6 script. Adapter sequences were removed from the 3′ end of reads with a proprietary script discarding reads shorter than 100 bp.

Bioinformatics

The 16S amplicon data was analyzed by standard dada2 pipeline version 1.8 (Callahan et al., Reference Callahan, McMurdie, Rosen, Han, Johnson and Holmes2016). Taxonomy was assigned using SILVA database version 138 (Quast et al., Reference Quast, Pruesse, Yilmaz, Gerken, Schweer, Yarza, Peplies and Glockner2013). Phylogenetic tree was constructed in QIIME version 1.9.1 (Caporaso et al., Reference Caporaso, Kuczynski, Stombaugh, Bittinger, Bushman, Costello, Fierer, Peña, Goodrich, Gordon, Huttley, Kelley, Knights, Koenig, Ley, Lozupone, McDonald, Muegge, Pirrung and Knight2010). The downstream post processing included removal of non-bacterial (for 16S), singletons, mitochondria, eukaryote and chloroplast amplicon sequence variants (ASVs). We included sequencing of oral swab blank samples, as well as negative and reagent controls from DNA extraction (alongside bacterial and fungal mock communities). The bacterial mock community is a cell suspension containing seven bacterial strains (including Staphylococcus aureus, Streptomyces californicus, Bacillus cereus, Escherichia coli, Pseudomonas aeruginosa, Sphingomonas faeni, and Mycobacterium mucogenicum). Negative and reagent controls were utilized primarily in the quantitative PCR analyses, to control the process of DNA extraction, alongside the positive controls (mock communities). Oral swab sample blanks were used to flag and remove contaminant ASVs from the bacterial sequencing data. We applied function isContaminant of the Decontam package version 1.2 (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Proctor, Holmes, Relman and Callahan2018), using prevalence method and a rather stringent probability threshold 0.5. This method compares prevalence of detection of an ASV in controls versus real samples and flags ASVs that occur at higher prevalence in the controls, as to be expected from reagent and other contaminants. In this decontamination step we used 12 swab blank samples as controls to compare ASV prevalence in controls versus actual samples. Those blanks most closely resemble the reagents constituents and processing steps of the actual samples, since they were processed, extracted, amplified, and sequenced alongside the oral swab samples. Using this approach, a total of n = 35 bacterial ASVs with <1000 sequence reads were removed prior to downstream analyses (Supplement 1).

Alpha diversity measures microbiome diversity within a single sample and reflects community heterogeneity, typically described by relative abundance and total number of species (D’Agostino et al., Reference D’Agostino, Ferrara, Valentini, Stoica and Dolci2022). The alpha diversity measures Chao1, observed species, and Shannon index were calculated using estimate_richness -function in R-package phyloseq version 1.38.0 (McMurdie & Holmes, Reference McMurdie and Holmes2013), applying rarefaction values of 1002 sequences (rarefy_even_depth -function in phyloseq). The fourth alpha diversity metric Faith’s phylogenetic diversity was computed using pd-function in R-package picante version 1.8.2 (Kembel et al., Reference Kembel, Cowan, Helmus, Cornwell, Morlon, Ackerly, Blomberg and Webb2010).

Beta diversity (β-diversity) measures similarity or dissimilarity between two communities and is used to study associations between environmental variables and microbial composition, providing a statistical comparison between two samples (D’Agostino et al., Reference D’Agostino, Ferrara, Valentini, Stoica and Dolci2022). Phylogenetically informed variation between pairs of samples (beta diversity) in the bacterial community was evaluated with Generalized UniFrac distances calculated using R-package GUniFrac version 1.7 (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Bittinger, Charlson, Hoffmann, Lewis, Wu, Collman, Bushman and Li2012) with midpoint rooted tree and α = 1 for abundance weighted beta diversity.

Statistical analysis

The behavioral questionnaires generating three behavioral temperament subscales Effortful control (EFF)/NEG/Surgency/Positive Emotionality (SUR) for three time-points (years 1, 2, 3) of the infants’ development were tested for associations with the neonates’ bacterial exposure measures of alpha and beta diversity at birth. Principal component (PC) analysis was used to reduce the dimensionality of the four alpha diversity metrics (describing the diversity of the bacterial exposure within individual) into two PCs, alpha PC1 and alpha PC2, while still preserving maximum amount of information. Principal coordinate (PCo) analysis of generalized unifrac distances was used to generate principal coordinates beta PCo1 and beta PCo2. Scree plots were used for both PCA and PCo to determine the number of components or coordinates chosen. Pearson correlation coefficients between the relative abundances of each bacterial genus and PCo1 and 2 were calculated to better understand how bacterial community composition impacts the beta diversity principal coordinates.

Linear regression models were used to test for cross-sectional effects of alpha and beta diversity metrics (alpha PC1 and 2, beta PCo 1 and 2) on behavior temperament EFF, NEG, and SUR at year 1. Models were adjusted for sex of the child, maternal age, and 3rd trimester EPDS score, and fitted for the whole sample as well as in subgroups of those who received antibiotics during birth (n = 137) and those who did not (n = 198). Any missing data in maternal age and EPDS score were mean imputed while multiple imputations by chained equations with m = 5 imputations were used to account for any missing year 1 EFF/NEG/SUR values. Sex of the child, maternal age, EPDS score, and EFF/NEG/SUR values at year 2 or 3 (when available) were included in the imputation models. As a sensitivity analysis, complete case data (n = 281) with non-missing year 1 EFF/NEG/SUR values available were analyzed.

Linear mixed models with random intercepts for each newborn (id) were used to test for longitudinal effects of alpha and beta diversity metrics on repeatedly measured behavior temperament EFF/NEG/SUR at years 1, 2, and 3, and fitted with R-package lme4 version 1.1.27.1 and lmerTest version 3.1.3. Inclusion of random intercepts allows each newborn to have their own intercept and a slope parallel to others. This can be interpreted as estimating individual levels of the average behavioral subscale score while assuming the relationship with the predictors as being the same for each newborn. Alpha and beta diversity measures as well as continuous time were included as fixed effects. Models were additionally adjusted for fixed effects of the sex of the child, maternal age, and 3rd trimester EPDS score, and fitted for the whole sample as well as in subgroups of antibiotic use during birth. Any missing outcome values were handled within the mixed model framework.

As a secondary analysis, the linear regression models and linear mixed models were used to test effects of the most abundant individual genera on behavior temperament at year 1, or repeatedly measured behavior temperament at years 1, 2, and 3. Bacterial genera with median relative abundance >0 were included to target only dominant genera in the swab samples. Covariate adjustments were identical to those of primary analyses.

In all analyses, Bonferroni corrections taking into account the number of predictors and the number of models run were used to counteract the multiple comparison problem. Models with alpha and beta diversity metrics were tested at the significance level of 0.0014 (4 bacterial features, 3 outcomes EFF/NEG/SUR, 3 models for full sample and 2 antibiotic use subgroups), whereas genera level analyses were tested at the significance level of 0.0007 (8 microbiome features, 3 outcomes EFF/NEG/SUR, 3 model for full sample and two antibiotic use subgroups).

The structure and approach to all methodology in this study was performed in adherence with the STORMS (Strengthening The Organizing and Reporting of Microbiome Studies) checklist guidelines for microbiome analyses in human microbiome research (Mirzayi et al., Reference Mirzayi, Renson, Sansone, Zohra, Elsafoury, Geistlinger, Kasselman, Eckenrode, van de Wijgert, Loughman, Marques, MacIntyre, Arumugam, Azhar, Beghini, Bergstrom, Bhatt, Bisanz, Braun and Waldron2021); the completed STORMS checklist is available for inspection in its entirety in Supplement 2.

The code used to perform the statistical analyses was written in R and is deposited in GitHub: https://github.com/maijupesonen/Microbial_exposure_at_birth_and_temperament.

Results

Population characteristics

The mean age of mothers involved in the study was 31.2 years with a mean gestational length at birth of 279 days (39 weeks + 6 days). Maternal third trimester EPDS score had a mean value of 4.1, and 151 (51%) of the infants were female. Missing values for maternal age (3% missing) and third trimester EPDS score (21% missing) were mean imputed when included as covariates in the regression models. Behavioral subscale scores from years 1 to 3 ranged from 5.0 to 5.4, and the distribution of the behavioral subscale scores across years 1–3 grouped by sex of the child were comparable (Figure 2) Girls had on average slightly higher EFF scores in comparison to boys across ages of 1–3 years, whereas boys had on average slightly higher SUR scores in comparison to girls at all ages. NEG scores showed largest change over time in both sexes being highest at age 1 and decreasing after that. The proportion of missing values for behavioral subscale scores increased over time with 16% being missing at year 1, 24% at year 2, and 29% at year 3. Cronbach’s alpha for reliability was 0.75, 0.78, and 0.83 for the SUR, EFF, and NEG subscale, respectively, suggesting an acceptable to good internal consistency of the items measuring the same characteristic.

Figure 2. Distributions of the observed behavior temperament scores effortful control, negative affect, and surgency/positive emotionality over 1, 2, and 3 years of age grouped by sex of the child. The numbers of non-missing subscale score values per year are given at the bottom of effortful control panel and are the same across subscales.

Microbial composition

In total, we observed 22 unique bacterial phyla and 370 unique bacterial genera (unassigned taxa excluded). Most of the observed genera were rare and present only in 1–5 newborns. Four phyla (Firmicutes, Actinobecteriota, Bacteroidota, Proteobacteria), and eight genera (Lactobacillus, Staphylococcus, Corynebacterium, Prevotella, Anaerococcus, Bacteroides, Stenotrophomonas, Streptococcus) were present in most samples (defined as at least 50% of the samples having the taxa present at some non-zero relative abundance). Typically, a sample was dominated by 1-3 genus. The most prevalent genera alone, Lactobacillus, constituted at least 54% of the entire microbiome in 50% of the samples, whereas all eight of the most prevalent genera constituted at least 78% of the entire microbiome in 50% of the samples (Supplement 3, Figure S1).

Microbial exposure effects on behavioral temperament

Based on scree plots (Supplement 3, Figure S2), two first PCs of alpha diversity metrics (alpha PC1 and 2) and two first principal coordinates of the generalized unifrac distance matrix (beta PCo1 and 2) were chosen, resulting in a total of 98.3% of the variation in alpha diversity and 62.6% of the variation in beta diversity explained. Behavioral temperament outcomes were tested for associations with these four bacterial exposure measures of alpha and beta diversity. As shown in Figure 3, alpha PC1 captured richness (total number of species in the community) while alpha PC2 captured evenness.

Figure 3. Loadings of alpha diversity metrics on two first principal components. Positive (orange) and negative (dark blue) loadings of Chao1, observed species, Faith’s phylogenetic diversity, and Shannon index on alpha diversity principal component 1 (panel a) and principal component 2 (panel b).

Of the 330 genera detected in swab samples (after rarefaction, unassigned taxa excluded), the relative abundances of 12 genera correlated (>|0.4|) with at least one of the beta diversity principal axes. The full correlation results for relative abundances of each genus and beta PCo1 and 2 are presented in Supplement 4. Beta PCo1 was positively associated with Lactobacillus, and negatively associated with Bacteroides, Faecalibacterium and Blautia, while beta PCo2 was positively associated with Gardnerella and Corynebacterium, and negatively associated with Faecalibacterium and Blautia.

Beta PCo1 could thus be interpreted as reflecting exposure to Lactobacilli dominant (“healthy”) vaginal microbiota vs. other sources (faecal, mixed vaginal microbiota), whereas beta PCo2 is reflecting exposure to vaginal microbiota dominated by genera other than Lactobacilli, suggesting bacterial imbalance potentially caused by bacterial vaginosis (Supplement 3, Figure S3).

When each behavioral subscale was tested for associations with alpha PC1 and 2, together with beta PCo1 and 2 output at year one, we found an uncorrected significant negative association between beta PCo1 and SUR (β = −0.99, p = 0.02) for the antibiotic use (AB use) group only, suggesting that increased surgency is associated with a higher abundance of Bacteroides, Faecalibacterium and Blautia, and a lower abundance of Lactobacillus (Figure 4); however, no significant associations were observed after Bonferroni corrections (Table 1).

Figure 4. Strongest correlations between the two first generalized unifrac principal coordinates and bacterial genera. Positive (orange) and negative (blue) correlations between bacterial genera (unidentified genera filtered out) and principal coordinate 1 (panel a) and 2 (panel b) computed from the generalized unifrac distance matrix. For both principal coordinates, only the most strongly correlated genera (>|0.40|) were considered and plotted in an ascending order.

Table 1. Linear regression models with behavior temperament effortful control (EFF)/negative affect (NEG)/surgency/positive emotionality (SUR) at year 1 as a dependent variable and alpha and beta diversity measures as independent variables

Models were adjusted for sex of the child, maternal age, and 3rd trimester EPDS score, and fitted for the whole sample as well as in subgroups of antibiotic use during birth (AB). The results are reported as estimate (standard error). **denotes a significance after Bonferroni correction (threshold for significance 0.0014), *denotes a p-value <0.05.

In the group of mothers not receiving AB during birth, the association between beta PCo1 and SUR was not significant (β = 0.18, p = 0.70). As a sensitivity analysis, we refitted the full sample model (both AB groups combined) adding an interaction term between AB use and beta PCo1. The interaction between AB use and beta PCo1 was significant at the uncorrected 0.05-level (β = −1.36, p = 0.031), supporting the finding that the effect of beta PCo1 on surgency is different between the AB use groups (results not shown).

No significant associations were found when repeatedly measured behavioral subscales at 1, 2, and 3 years of age were tested for associations with alpha PC1 and PC2, and beta PCo1 and 2 (Table 2).

Table 2. Linear mixed models with random intercepts for repeatedly measured behavior temperament effortful control (EFF)/negative affect (NEG)/surgency/positive emotionality (SUR) at years 1, 2, and 3. alpha- and beta-diversity measures as well as continuous time were included as fixed effects

Models were additionally adjusted for fixed effects of the sex of the child, maternal age, and 3rd trimester EPDS score, and fitted for the whole sample as well as in subgroups of antibiotic use during birth (AB). The results are reported as fixed effect estimate (standard error). **denotes a significance after Bonferroni correction (threshold for significance 0.0014), *denotes a p-value <0.05.

Bacterial genera analyses

Secondary analyses were performed to study associations between individual bacterial genera, the behavior temperament, and its development during the first three years with the most prevalent bacterial genera. Rare genera were removed to retain the dominant genera reflecting overall oral microbiome composition. The genera considered included Lactobacillus, Streptococcus, Staphylococcus, Corynebacterium, Stenotrophomonas, Bacteroides, Prevotella, and Anaerococcus. No significant associations were found between the tested bacterial genera and the three behavior temperament subscales at year 1. (Table 3). A positive association between repeatedly measured surgency at ages of 1, 2, and 3 and relative abundance of Staphylococcus was observed in the no antibiotic use during birth group (β = 1.19, p = 0.02), however, becoming non-significant after Bonferroni correction (Table 4).

Table 3. Linear regression models with behavior temperament effortful control (EFF)/negative affect (NEG)/surgency/positive emotionality (SUR) at year 1 as a dependent variable and most abundant individual bacterial genera as independent variables

Models were adjusted for sex of the child, maternal age, and 3rd trimester EPDS score, and fitted for the whole sample as well as in subgroups of antibiotic use during birth (AB). The results are reported as estimate (standard error). **denotes a significance after Bonferroni correction (threshold for significance 0.0007), *denotes a p-value <0.05.

Table 4. Linear mixed models with random intercepts for repeatedly measured behavior temperament effortful control (EFF)/negative affect (NEG)/surgency/positive emotionality (SUR) at years 1, 2, and 3. Most abundant individual bacterial genera as well as continuous time as fixed effects

Models were additionally adjusted for fixed effects of the sex of the child, maternal age, and 3rd trimester EPDS score, and fitted for the whole sample as well as in subgroups of antibiotic use during birth (AB). The results are reported as fixed effect estimate (standard error). **denotes a significance after Bonferroni correction (threshold for significance 0.0007), *denotes a p-value <0.05.

The sensitivity analyses for behavior temperament at year 1 models which were performed on the complete case data (n = 281) produced identical results, the one exception to this being an uncorrected negative association found between Anaerococcus genus and NEG at year 1 (β = −3.93, p = 0.03, data not shown).

Discussion

The present study explored the associations between bacterial exposure profiles at birth and behavioral temperament assessed during infancy and early childhood. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study performed addressing a newborn’s initial bacterial exposure and infant temperament during the first three years of childhood. We tested associations between bacterial alpha and beta diversity measures against three behavioral temperament subscales (EFF, NEG, and SUR) for infants in year one alone and longitudinally across years 1, 2, and 3. No significant associations were found after Bonferroni corrections.

The use of neonatal bacterial exposure profiles for predicting behavioral outcomes is still considered an emergent field of research. In this study, prior to corrections for multiple comparison testing, the behavioral subscale surgency at the age of one was negatively associated with beta PCo1 (high abundance of Bacteroides, Faecalibacterium and Blautia, low abundance of Lactobacillus) for the group using antibiotics during childbirth. Surgency is a behavioral temperament dimension associated with expressions of positive emotions, reward seeking, and a high activity level, often linked with sociability and lack of shyness (Holmboe, Reference Holmboe, Zeigler-Hill and Shackelford2016). Our pre-correction observations are comparable to a recent study investigating the gut microbiome of infants at four different time-points during the first year of an infant’s life, whereby an association between surgency and beta diversity at the earliest time-point (1–3 weeks) for 23 infants was reported (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Lee, Wiley, Lagishetty, Sandman, Jacobs and Glynn2021). Moreover, Fox et al., identified specific genera from the gut microbiome (Bifidobacterium, Lachnospiraceae, and Collinsella) as being positively associated with the surgency scale at age 12 months (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Lee, Wiley, Lagishetty, Sandman, Jacobs and Glynn2021). By contrast, Aatsinki et al., found a higher abundance of Bifidobacterium and Streptococcus, and a lower abundance of Atopobium at age 2.5 months, were associated with greater surgency scores, also measured using the Infant Behavior Questionnaire in 6-month-old infants (Aatsinki et al., Reference Aatsinki, Lahti, Uusitupa, Munukka, Keskitalo, Nolvi, O’Mahony, Pietila, Elo, Eerola, Karlsson and Karlsson2019). Another study found that surgency scores were associated with a unique microbiota community profile in gut microbiota composition (beta diversity) for boys only, using the stool-sourced gut microbiome samples of 77 children between 18 and 27 months of age (Christian et al., Reference Christian, Galley, Hade, Schoppe-Sullivan, Dush and Bailey2015). However, neither of these studies appear to have adjusted for intrapartum antibiotic use.

Although non-significant post-correction, the observed association in the antibiotic use group accentuates the potential formative role of intrapartum antibiotic exposure upon the developing microbiome, with the significance of the association further emphasized by our neonatal sample being exclusively composed of vaginal delivery births. Roesch et al. (Reference Roesch, Silveira, Corso, Dobbler, Mai, Rojas, Laureano and Procianoy2017) reported that intrapartum antibiotic administration modified vaginal microbiota composition by balancing the dominance of Lactobacillus and thus increasing the microbial diversity (Roesch et al., Reference Roesch, Silveira, Corso, Dobbler, Mai, Rojas, Laureano and Procianoy2017). In line with this, we observed that the group with antibiotic use - thought to be a consequence of antibiotic prophylaxis for group B streptococcal colonization in pregnant women (Lyytikainen et al., Reference Lyytikainen, Nuorti, Halmesmaki, Carlson, Uotila, Vuento, Ranta, Sarkkinen, Ammala, Kostiala and Jarvenpaa2003) - had lower beta PCo1 values, on average, in comparison to the group without intrapartum antibiotics. Additionally, these low beta PCo1 values were associated with a higher relative abundance of Bacteroides and lower relative abundance of Lactobacillus. We cannot exclude the possibility, however, that the vaginal microbiota in women treated or untreated with antibiotics may have differed to begin with. Alternately, the results may imply that higher relative exposure to fecal microbes over vaginal microbes is associated with surgency, due to fecal microbes being proposed as more likely to contribute to the infant gut microbiota development in vaginally delivered infants (Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Mazzoni, Hogstrom, Bryant, Bergerat, Cher, Pochan, Herman, Carrigan, Sharp, Huttenhower, Lander, Vlamakis, Xavier and Yassour2020). One reason why the association is seen only in the antibiotic exposed group may be owing to differences in microbial exposure reflected by beta PCoA1.

Our group with no intrapartum antibiotic use displayed a pre-correction association between the Staphylococcus genus abundance at birth and surgency across years 1–3. Staphylococcus, together with Lactobacillus, Propionibacterium, and Streptococcus, are often the most abundant genera in neonates across all body sites (Chu et al., Reference Chu, Ma, Prince, Antony, Seferovic and Aagaard2017). It is feasible to consider the lack of exposure to intrapartum antibiotics sustained the abundance of these genera (whereas exposure to antibiotics could have resulted in their decrease), yet the association with surgency shown here is novel. Loughman et al., reported a lower abundance of Prevotella in gut microbiota at 12 months was associated with an increase of behavioral problems at 2 years of age, surmising that recent exposure to antibiotics was the best predictor of decreased Prevotella (Loughman et al., Reference Loughman, Ponsonby, O’Hely, Symeonides, Collier, Tang, Carlin, Ranganathan, Allen, Pezic, Saffery, Jacka, Harrison, Sly, Vuillermin and Group2020). These findings elicit further attention into potential mechanisms linking intrapartum antibiotic use and the emerging infant behavioral temperament.

The majority of previous microbiota research on infants and children have utilized gut microbiome profiles, usually obtained from stool samples (Aatsinki et al., Reference Aatsinki, Lahti, Uusitupa, Munukka, Keskitalo, Nolvi, O’Mahony, Pietila, Elo, Eerola, Karlsson and Karlsson2019; Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Xia, Azcarate-Peril, Rosin, Fine, Mu, Zopp, Kimmel, Styner, Thompson, Propper and Knickmeyer2021; Loughman et al., Reference Loughman, Ponsonby, O’Hely, Symeonides, Collier, Tang, Carlin, Ranganathan, Allen, Pezic, Saffery, Jacka, Harrison, Sly, Vuillermin and Group2020), whereas a microbial profile sourced from the first oral cavity exposure is infrequently used (Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Adler, du Plessis, Landau, Dashper, Reynolds, Schwartz and Simmons2020). Even though microbiota sourced from another location may cause uncertainties regarding the reliability of comparisons with previous literature, colonization of the gut has been charted back from initial oral colonization (D’Agostino et al., Reference D’Agostino, Ferrara, Valentini, Stoica and Dolci2022; Dominguez-Bello et al., Reference Dominguez-Bello, Costello, Contreras, Magris, Hidalgo, Fierer and Knight2010; Olsen & Yamazaki, Reference Olsen and Yamazaki2019; Xiao et al., Reference Xiao, Fiscella and Gill2020), hence the emerging microbiome trajectory and its effect upon behavioral characteristics in early life retains plausibility.

Strengths of this study include the exclusive focus on infants born via vaginal delivery and the use of bacterial samples reflecting actual exposure at birth, the inclusion of longitudinal behavior measures up to three years of age, as well as the conservative statistical approach applied. Furthermore, a fundamental element in approaching observational studies is the incorporation of confounder assessment. In this study, DAGs were used to identify the minimal sufficient adjustment set when estimating the effect of the newborn microbial exposure on behavior temperament. Our use of DAGs for the determination of confounding adjustment is a method encouraged and endorsed within the STORMS checklist guidelines for microbiome analyses in human microbiome research (Mirzayi et al., Reference Mirzayi, Renson, Sansone, Zohra, Elsafoury, Geistlinger, Kasselman, Eckenrode, van de Wijgert, Loughman, Marques, MacIntyre, Arumugam, Azhar, Beghini, Bergstrom, Bhatt, Bisanz, Braun and Waldron2021). Limitations in this study, as with the majority of studies in this area (Aatsinki et al., Reference Aatsinki, Lahti, Uusitupa, Munukka, Keskitalo, Nolvi, O’Mahony, Pietila, Elo, Eerola, Karlsson and Karlsson2019; Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Xia, Azcarate-Peril, Rosin, Fine, Mu, Zopp, Kimmel, Styner, Thompson, Propper and Knickmeyer2021; Christian et al., Reference Christian, Galley, Hade, Schoppe-Sullivan, Dush and Bailey2015; Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Prescott, McCulloch, Trinchieri, Valladares, Dreisbach, Alhusen and Grossmann2021), is the use of parent rated IBQ and ECBQ questionnaires to assess infant behavioral temperament; the parent reported data may be prone to bias. Furthermore, our analysis is constricted by the singular microbiota sample obtained moments after birth. As the newly introduced bacterial species rapidly change whilst the microbiome develops throughout the gastrointestinal tract (Dashper et al., Reference Dashper, Mitchell, Cao, Carpenter, Gussy, Calache, Gladman, Bulach, Hoffmann, Catmull, Pruilh, Johnson, Gibbs, Amezdroz, Bhatnagar, Seemann, Mnatzaganian, Manton and Reynolds2019; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Ajami, O’Brien, Hutchinson, Smith, Wong, Ross, Lloyd, Doddapaneni, Metcalf, Muzny, Gibbs, Vatanen, Huttenhower, Xavier, Rewers, Hagopian, Toppari, Ziegler and Petrosino2018) caution is required when comparing our results with studies performed at later stages of infant development. Additionally, emerging evidence now suggests prenatal inflammation may contribute to offspring temperament traits, which would necessitate maternal infections processed as potential confounders rather than as an element affecting the neonate microbiome only, as analyzed in the present study (Serrano et al., Reference Serrano, Womack, Yount, Chowdhury, Arnold, Brunner, Duberstein, Barrett, Scheible, Miller and O’Connor2024).

Although the various gut-brain axis pathways are proposed as the prime mechanisms for the developing microbiome’s effect upon behavioral temperament (Alving-Jessep et al., Reference Alving-Jessep, Botchway, Wood, Hilton and Blissett2022), Kelsey et al. (Reference Kelsey, Prescott, McCulloch, Trinchieri, Valladares, Dreisbach, Alhusen and Grossmann2021) also discuss the likelihood that the link between taxa diversity and behavioral temperament is mediated by functional brain network connectivity (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Prescott, McCulloch, Trinchieri, Valladares, Dreisbach, Alhusen and Grossmann2021). It is therefore likely that additional factors beyond the developing neonate microbiome, such as environmental influences and genetic predispositions, contribute to behavioral outcomes during early childhood. For example, in contrast to other temperament traits, the family environment also appears to play an important role in shaping individuals’ surgent behavioral traits (Holmboe, Reference Holmboe, Zeigler-Hill and Shackelford2016).

In conclusion, this study investigated the association between initial bacterial exposure profiles at birth and behavioral outcomes in early childhood. Although no significant associations were observed after Bonferroni corrections, the pre-correction findings indicate a possible link between intrapartum antibiotic use and the emerging infant behavioral temperament. Further studies are needed addressing the impact of the earliest periods of bacterial exposure at birth to oral colonization and subsequent trajectory towards gut microbiome development. Furthermore, our findings underscore the need for methodologically rigorous research in this emerging field necessary to explore the role of the neonate microbiome in behavioral development.

Understanding the complex interplay between the neonatal microbiome and behavioral outcomes could provide valuable insights and potentially inform interventions for promoting positive behavioral health in children.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001676.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Author contributions

MP performed the statistical analyses with contributions from AC and SML. KB, JP, LK-N, and SML designed the original cohort study this study is a part of. AC wrote the first draft of the manuscript with contributions and comments from MP and SML. All the authors reviewed and commented on subsequent drafts of the manuscript for important intellectual content.

Funding statement

None.

Competing interests

None.

References

Aagaard, K., Ma, J., Antony, K. M., Ganu, R., Petrosino, J., & Versalovic, J. (2014). The placenta harbors a unique microbiome. Science Translational Medicine, 6(237), 237265. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.3008599 Google ScholarPubMed
Aatsinki, A. K., Lahti, L., Uusitupa, H. M., Munukka, E., Keskitalo, A., Nolvi, S., O’Mahony, S., Pietila, S., Elo, L. L., Eerola, E., Karlsson, H., & Karlsson, L. (2019). Gut microbiota composition is associated with temperament traits in infants. Brain Behavior and Immunity, 80, 849858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2019.05.035 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alving-Jessep, E., Botchway, E., Wood, A. G., Hilton, A. C., & Blissett, J. M. (2022). The development of the gut microbiome and temperament during infancy and early childhood: A systematic review. Developmental Psychobiology, 64(7), e22306. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22306 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersson, A. F., Lindberg, M., Jakobsson, H., Backhed, F., Nyren, P., & Engstrand, L. (2008). Comparative analysis of human gut microbiota by barcoded pyrosequencing. Plos One, 3(7), e2836. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0002836 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bearfield, C., Davenport, E. S., Sivapathasundaram, V., & Allaker, R. P. (2002). Possible association between amniotic fluid micro-organism infection and microflora in the mouth. BJOG-An International Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 109(5), 527533. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1470-0328(02)01349-6 Google ScholarPubMed
Biasucci, G., Benenati, B., Morelli, L., Bessi, E., & Boehm, G. (2008). Cesarean delivery may affect the early biodiversity of intestinal bacteria. Journal of Nutrition, 138(9), 1796S1800S. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/138.9.1796S CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blum, J., Silva, M., Byrne, S. J., Butler, C. A., Adams, G. G., Reynolds, E. C., & Dashper, S. G. (2022). Temporal development of the infant oral microbiome. Critical Reviews in Microbiology, 48(6), 730742. https://doi.org/10.1080/1040841X.2021.2025042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callahan, B. J., McMurdie, P. J., Rosen, M. J., Han, A. W., Johnson, A. J., & Holmes, S. P. (2016). DADA2: High-resolution sample inference from illumina amplicon data. Nature Methods, 13(7), 581583. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3869 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caporaso, J. G., Kuczynski, J., Stombaugh, J., Bittinger, K., Bushman, F. D., Costello, E. K., Fierer, N., Peña, A. G., Goodrich, J. K., Gordon, J. I., Huttley, G. A., Kelley, S. T., Knights, D., Koenig, J. E., Ley, R. E., Lozupone, C. A., McDonald, D., Muegge, B. D., Pirrung, M., …, & Knight, R. (2010). QIIME allows analysis of high-throughput community sequencing data. Nature Methods, 7(5), 335336. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.f.303 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, A. L., Xia, K., Azcarate-Peril, M. A., Rosin, S. P., Fine, J. P., Mu, W., Zopp, J. B., Kimmel, M. C., Styner, M. A., Thompson, A. L., Propper, C. B., & Knickmeyer, R. C. (2021). Infant gut microbiome composition is associated with non-social fear behavior in a pilot study. Nature Communications, 12(1), 3294. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-23281-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J., Bittinger, K., Charlson, E. S., Hoffmann, C., Lewis, J., Wu, G. D., Collman, R. G., Bushman, F. D., & Li, H. (2012). Associating microbiome composition with environmental covariates using generalized UniFrac distances. Bioinformatics, 28(16), 21062113. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/bts342 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christian, L. M., Galley, J. D., Hade, E. M., Schoppe-Sullivan, S., Dush, C. K., & Bailey, M. T. (2015). Gut microbiome composition is associated with temperament during early childhood. Brain Behavior and Immunity, 45, 118127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2014.10.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, D. M., Ma, J., Prince, A. L., Antony, K. M., Seferovic, M. D., & Aagaard, K. M. (2017). Maturation of the infant microbiome community structure and function across multiple body sites and in relation to mode of delivery. Nature Medicine, 23(3), 314326. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm.4272 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, E. K., Carlisle, E. M., Bik, E. M., Morowitz, M. J., & Relman, D. A. (2013). Microbiome assembly across multiple body sites in low-birthweight infants. mBio, 4(6), e0078213. https://doi.org/10.1128/mBio.00782-13 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item edinburgh postnatal depression scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150(6), 782786. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Agostino, S., Ferrara, E., Valentini, G., Stoica, S. A., & Dolci, M. (2022). Exploring oral microbiome in healthy infants and children: A systematic review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(18), 11403. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811403 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dal Bello, F., & Hertel, C. (2006). Oral cavity as natural reservoir for intestinal lactobacilli. Systematic and Applied Microbiology, 29(1), 6976. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.syapm.2005.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dashper, S. G., Mitchell, H. L., Cao, K. A. L., Carpenter, L., Gussy, M. G., Calache, H., Gladman, S. L., Bulach, D. M., Hoffmann, B., Catmull, D. V., Pruilh, S., Johnson, S., Gibbs, L., Amezdroz, E., Bhatnagar, U., Seemann, T., Mnatzaganian, G., Manton, D. J., & Reynolds, E. C. (2019). Temporal development of the oral microbiome and prediction of early childhood caries. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 19732. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-56233-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, N. M., Proctor, D. M., Holmes, S. P., Relman, D. A., & Callahan, B. J. (2018). Simple statistical identification and removal of contaminant sequences in marker-gene and metagenomics data. Microbiome, 6(1), 226. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40168-018-0605-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Pauw, S. (2016). Childhood personality and temperament. In Widiger, T. A. (Ed.), The oxford handbook of the five factor model. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199352487.013.21 Google Scholar
Dierikx, T. H., Visser, D. H., Benninga, M. A., van Kaam, A., de Boer, N. K. H., de Vries, R., van Limbergen, J., & de Meij, T. G. J. (2020). The influence of prenatal and intrapartum antibiotics on intestinal microbiota colonisation in infants: A systematic review. The Journal of Infection, 81(2), 190204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jinf.2020.05.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dominguez-Bello, M. G., Costello, E. K., Contreras, M., Magris, M., Hidalgo, G., Fierer, N., & Knight, R. (2010). Delivery mode shapes the acquisition and structure of the initial microbiota across multiple body habitats in newborns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(26), 1197111975. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1002601107 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, A. B., Jordan, S., Baker, B. J., & Carlson, N. S. (2017). The maternal infant microbiome: Considerations for labor and birth. MCN-the American Journal of Maternal-Child Nursing, 42(6), 318325. https://doi.org/10.1097/Nmc.0000000000000373 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dzidic, M., Collado, M. C., Abrahamsson, T., Artacho, A., Stensson, M., Jenmalm, M. C., & Mira, A. (2018). Oral microbiome development during childhood: An ecological succession influenced by postnatal factors and associated with tooth decay. The ISME Journal, 12(9), 22922306. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41396-018-0204-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, M., Lee, S. M., Wiley, K. S., Lagishetty, V., Sandman, C. A., Jacobs, J. P., & Glynn, L. M. (2021). Development of the infant gut microbiome predicts temperament across the first year of life. Development and Psychopathology, 34(5), 19141925. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000456 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guarner, F., & Malagelada, J.-R. (2003). Gut flora in health and disease. The Lancet, 361(9356), 512519. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12489-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heijtz, R. D., Wang, S., Anuar, F., Qian, Y., Björkholm, B., Samuelsson, A., Hibberd, M. L., Forssberg, H., & Pettersson, S. (2011). Normal gut microbiota modulates brain development and behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(7), 30473052. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010529108 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herlemann, D. P., Labrenz, M., Jurgens, K., Bertilsson, S., Waniek, J. J., & Andersson, A. F. (2011). Transitions in bacterial communities along the 2000 km salinity gradient of the Baltic Sea. The ISME Journal, 5(10), 15711579. https://doi.org/10.1038/ismej.2011.41 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holgerson, P. L., Harnevik, L., Hernell, O., Tanner, A. C. R., & Johansson, I. (2011). Mode of birth delivery affects oral microbiota in infants. Journal of Dental Research, 90(10), 11831188. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022034511418973 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmboe, K. (2016). The construct of surgency. In Zeigler-Hill, V., & Shackelford, T. K. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24612-3_2123 Google Scholar
Huuskonen, P., Keski-Nisula, L., Heinonen, S., Voutilainen, S., Tuomainen, T. P., Pekkanen, J., Lampi, J., Lehto, S. M., Haaparanta, H., Elomaa, A. P., Voutilainen, R., Backman, K., Kokki, H., Kumpulainen, K., Paananen, J., Vahakangas, K., & Pasanen, M. (2018). Kuopio birth cohort - design of a Finnish joint research effort for identification of environmental and lifestyle risk factors for the wellbeing of the mother and the newborn child. BMC Pregnancy & Childbirth, 18(1), 381. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12884-018-2013-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jayaprakash, B., Adams, R. I., Kirjavainen, P., Karvonen, A., Vepsalainen, A., Valkonen, M., Jarvi, K., Sulyok, M., Pekkanen, J., Hyvarinen, A., & Taubel, M. (2017). Indoor microbiota in severely moisture damaged homes and the impact of interventions. Microbiome, 5(1), 138. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40168-017-0356-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaan, A., Kahharova, D., & Zaura, E. (2021). Acquisition and establishment of the oral microbiota. Periodontology 2000, 86(1), 123141. https://doi.org/10.1111/prd.12366 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelsey, C. M., Prescott, S., McCulloch, J. A., Trinchieri, G., Valladares, T. L., Dreisbach, C., Alhusen, J., & Grossmann, T. (2021). Gut microbiota composition is associated with newborn functional brain connectivity and behavioral temperament. Brain Behavior and Immunity, 91, 472486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.11.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kembel, S. W., Cowan, P. D., Helmus, M. R., Cornwell, W. K., Morlon, H., Ackerly, D. D., Blomberg, S. P., & Webb, C. O. (2010). Picante: R tools for integrating phylogenies and ecology. Bioinformatics, 26(11), 14631464. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btq166 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, G. H., & Shim, J. O. (2023). Gut microbiota affects brain development and behavior. Clinical and Experimental Pediatrics, 66(7), 274280. https://doi.org/10.3345/cep.2021.01550 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loughman, A., Ponsonby, A. L., O’Hely, M., Symeonides, C., Collier, F., Tang, M. L. K., Carlin, J., Ranganathan, S., Allen, K., Pezic, A., Saffery, R., Jacka, F., Harrison, L. C., Sly, P. D., Vuillermin, P., & Group, B. I. S. (2020). Gut microbiota composition during infancy and subsequent behavioural outcomes. EBioMedicine, 52, 102640. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ebiom.2020.102640 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyytikainen, O., Nuorti, J. P., Halmesmaki, E., Carlson, P., Uotila, J., Vuento, R., Ranta, T., Sarkkinen, H., Ammala, M., Kostiala, A., & Jarvenpaa, A. L. (2003). Invasive group B streptococcal infections in Finland: A population-based study. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 9(4), 469473. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid0904.020481 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madan, J. C., Hoen, A. G., Lundgren, S. N., Farzan, S. F., Cottingham, K. L., Morrison, H. G., Sogin, M. L., Li, H. Z., Moore, J. H., & Karagas, M. R. (2016). Association of cesarean delivery and formula supplementation with the intestinal microbiome of 6-week-old infants. JAMA Pediatrics, 170(3), 212219. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.3732 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMurdie, P. J., & Holmes, S. (2013). Phyloseq: An R package for reproducible interactive analysis and graphics of microbiome census data. Plos One, 8(4), e61217. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061217 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirzayi, C., Renson, A., Sansone, S.-A., Zohra, F., Elsafoury, S., Geistlinger, L., Kasselman, L. J., Eckenrode, K., van de Wijgert, J., Loughman, A., Marques, F. Z., MacIntyre, D. A., Arumugam, M., Azhar, R., Beghini, F., Bergstrom, K., Bhatt, A., Bisanz, J. E., Braun, J., …, & Waldron, L. (2021). Reporting guidelines for human microbiome research: The STORMS checklist. Nature Medicine, 27(11), 18851892. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-021-01552-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, C. M., Mazzoni, C., Hogstrom, L., Bryant, A., Bergerat, A., Cher, A., Pochan, S., Herman, P., Carrigan, M., Sharp, K., Huttenhower, C., Lander, E. S., Vlamakis, H., Xavier, R. J., & Yassour, M. (2020). Delivery mode affects stability of early infant gut microbiota. Cell Reports Medicine, 1(9), 100156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.xcrm.2020.100156 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelun Barfod, M., Magnusson, K., Lexner, M. O., Blomqvist, S., Dahlen, G., & Twetman, S. (2011). Oral microflora in infants delivered vaginally and by caesarean section. International Journal of Paediatric Dentistry, 21(6), 401406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-263X.2011.01136.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3-4), 395422. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsen, I., & Yamazaki, K. (2019). Can oral bacteria affect the microbiome of the gut? Journal of Oral Microbiology, 11(1), 1586422. https://doi.org/10.1080/20002297.2019.1586422 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penders, J., Thijs, C., Vink, C., Stelma, F. F., Snijders, B., Kummeling, I., van den Brandt, P. A., & Stobberingh, E. E. (2006). Factors influencing the composition of the intestinal microbiota in early infancy. Pediatrics, 118(2), 511521. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2824 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior & Development, 29(3), 386401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quast, C., Pruesse, E., Yilmaz, P., Gerken, J., Schweer, T., Yarza, P., Peplies, J., & Glockner, F. O. (2013). The SILVA ribosomal RNA gene database project: Improved data processing and web-based tools. Nucleic Acids Research, 41(D1), D590596. https://doi.org/10.1093/nar/gks1219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roesch, L. F., Silveira, R. C., Corso, A. L., Dobbler, P. T., Mai, V., Rojas, B. S., Laureano, A. M., & Procianoy, R. S. (2017). Diversity and composition of vaginal microbiota of pregnant women at risk for transmitting group B streptococcus treated with intrapartum penicillin. Plos One, 12(2), e0169916. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169916 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblatt, R., Steinberg, D., Mankuta, D., & Zini, A. (2015). Acquired oral microflora of newborns during the first 48 hours of life. Journal of Clinical Pediatric Dentistry, 39(5), 442446. https://doi.org/10.17796/1053-4628-39.5.442 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52(2), 569578. https://doi.org/https://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1129176 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothenberg, S. E., Chen, Q., Shen, J., Nong, Y., Nong, H., Trinh, E. P., Biasini, F. J., Liu, J., Zeng, X., Zou, Y., Ouyang, F., & Korrick, S. A. (2021). Neurodevelopment correlates with gut microbiota in a cross-sectional analysis of children at 3 years of age in rural China. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 7384. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-86761-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutsch, A., Kantsjö, J. B., & Ronchi, F. (2020). The gut-brain axis: How microbiota and host inflammasome influence brain physiology and pathology. Frontiers in Immunology, 11, 604179. https://doi.org/10.3389/fimmu.2020.604179 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salminen, S., & Isolauri, E. (2006). Intestinal colonization, microbiota, and probiotics. The Journal of Pediatrics, 149(5), S115S120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2006.06.062 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sampaio-Maia, B., & Monteiro-Silva, F. (2014). Acquisition and maturation of oral microbiome throughout childhood: An update. Dental Research Journal, 11(3), 291301.Google ScholarPubMed
Sender, R., Fuchs, S., & Milo, R. (2016). Are we really vastly outnumbered? Revisiting the ratio of bacterial to host cells in humans. Cell, 164(3), 337340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2016.01.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serrano, J., Womack, S., Yount, C., Chowdhury, S. F., Arnold, M., Brunner, J., Duberstein, Z., Barrett, E. S., Scheible, K., Miller, R. K., & O’Connor, T. G. (2024). Prenatal maternal immune activation predicts observed fearfulness in infancy. Developmental Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001718 (Advance online publication).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, C. A., Adler, C., du Plessis, M. R., Landau, E. R., Dashper, S. G., Reynolds, E. C., Schwartz, O. S., & Simmons, J. G. (2020). Oral microbiome composition, but not diversity, is associated with adolescent anxiety and depression symptoms. Physiology & Behavior, 226, 113126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113126 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, C. J., Ajami, N. J., O’Brien, J. L., Hutchinson, D. S., Smith, D. P., Wong, M. C., Ross, M. C., Lloyd, R. E., Doddapaneni, H. V., Metcalf, G. A., Muzny, D., Gibbs, R. A., Vatanen, T., Huttenhower, C., Xavier, R. J., Rewers, M., Hagopian, W., Toppari, J., Ziegler, A.-G., …, & Petrosino, J. F. (2018). Temporal development of the gut microbiome in early childhood from the TEDDY study. Nature, 562(7728), 583588. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0617-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turroni, F., Ribbera, A., Foroni, E., van Sinderen, D., & Ventura, M. (2008). Human gut microbiota and bifidobacteria: From composition to functionality. Antonie Van Leeuwenhoek, 94(1), 3550. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10482-008-9232-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, S., Harvey, L., Martin, R., van der Beek, E. M., Knol, J., Cryan, J. F., & Renes, I. B. (2018). Targeting the gut microbiota to influence brain development and function in early life. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 95, 191201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiao, J., Fiscella, K. A., & Gill, S. R. (2020). Oral microbiome: Possible harbinger for children’s health. International Journal of Oral Science, 12(1), 12. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41368-020-0082-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, S., Cohen, D. B., Ravel, J., Abdo, Z., & Forney, L. J. (2012). Evaluation of methods for the extraction and purification of DNA from the human microbiome. Plos One, 7(3), e33865. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033865 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. The causal directed acyclic graph (DAG) used to identify the minimal sufficient adjustment set when estimating the effect of the neonatal oral microbiome on behavior temperament (green arrow). The variables and associations included in the DAG were based on subject-matter knowledge and literature search. The biasing paths (red arrows) were blocked by adjusting the subsequent models for maternal stress during pregnancy and maternal age. Additionally, models were chosen to be adjusted for the sex of the child to yield a more precise estimate.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Distributions of the observed behavior temperament scores effortful control, negative affect, and surgency/positive emotionality over 1, 2, and 3 years of age grouped by sex of the child. The numbers of non-missing subscale score values per year are given at the bottom of effortful control panel and are the same across subscales.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Loadings of alpha diversity metrics on two first principal components. Positive (orange) and negative (dark blue) loadings of Chao1, observed species, Faith’s phylogenetic diversity, and Shannon index on alpha diversity principal component 1 (panel a) and principal component 2 (panel b).

Figure 3

Figure 4. Strongest correlations between the two first generalized unifrac principal coordinates and bacterial genera. Positive (orange) and negative (blue) correlations between bacterial genera (unidentified genera filtered out) and principal coordinate 1 (panel a) and 2 (panel b) computed from the generalized unifrac distance matrix. For both principal coordinates, only the most strongly correlated genera (>|0.40|) were considered and plotted in an ascending order.

Figure 4

Table 1. Linear regression models with behavior temperament effortful control (EFF)/negative affect (NEG)/surgency/positive emotionality (SUR) at year 1 as a dependent variable and alpha and beta diversity measures as independent variables

Figure 5

Table 2. Linear mixed models with random intercepts for repeatedly measured behavior temperament effortful control (EFF)/negative affect (NEG)/surgency/positive emotionality (SUR) at years 1, 2, and 3. alpha- and beta-diversity measures as well as continuous time were included as fixed effects

Figure 6

Table 3. Linear regression models with behavior temperament effortful control (EFF)/negative affect (NEG)/surgency/positive emotionality (SUR) at year 1 as a dependent variable and most abundant individual bacterial genera as independent variables

Figure 7

Table 4. Linear mixed models with random intercepts for repeatedly measured behavior temperament effortful control (EFF)/negative affect (NEG)/surgency/positive emotionality (SUR) at years 1, 2, and 3. Most abundant individual bacterial genera as well as continuous time as fixed effects

Supplementary material: File

Chortatos et al. supplementary material 1

Chortatos et al. supplementary material
Download Chortatos et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 15.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Chortatos et al. supplementary material 2

Chortatos et al. supplementary material
Download Chortatos et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 39.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Chortatos et al. supplementary material 3

Chortatos et al. supplementary material
Download Chortatos et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 914.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Chortatos et al. supplementary material 4

Chortatos et al. supplementary material
Download Chortatos et al. supplementary material 4(File)
File 34.3 KB