The developmental psychopathology perspective provides a valuable framework for understanding mental health disorders and has particularly contributed to improved diagnosis and treatment for children and adolescents (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2013). Given that a central tenant of this approach is focus on characterizing normative development to inform understanding of atypical or aberrant psychology and behavior and that this approach is especially amenable to the investigation of transitional points in development across the life span (Toth & Cicchetti, Reference Toth and Cicchetti2010), it is surprising that one significant developmental epoch in the female life span, that associated with the transition to motherhood, has yet to receive full benefit from this perspective (Bos, Reference Bos2017; Goodman & Dimidjian, Reference Goodman and Dimidjian2012). The dynamic processes of pregnancy and lactation represent a developmental inflection point, one that is arguably the most fundamental and profound in the postnatal female life span. Pregnancy initiates broad and dramatic alterations in maternal anatomy, physiology, and metabolism (Strauss, Barbieri, & Macy Ladd, Reference Strauss, Barbieri and Macy Ladd2014; Torgersen & Curran, Reference Torgersen and Curran2006; Williams, Reference Williams2003). Among these changes is the growth and development of a new organ, the placenta, that has immune, endocrine, and vascular properties (Petraglia, Florio, Nappi, & Genazzani, Reference Petraglia, Florio, Nappi and Genazzani1996). The pregnant woman exhibits endocrine alterations (the scope of which are unrivaled by any other in the postnatal life span), increases in blood volumes and cardiac output, hypercoagulation, insulin resistance, and a shift toward a Th-2 cytokine profile (Greer, Reference Greer1999; Kuhl, Reference Kuhl1991; Poole & Claman, Reference Poole and Claman2004; Torgersen & Curran, Reference Torgersen and Curran2006). These changes are among those comprising the extensive transformation of maternal physiology necessary to maintain the pregnancy and to prepare the maternal brain for the challenges of motherhood. However, to date, characterization of typical changes in human brain and behavior during pregnancy and the postpartum period are lacking, limiting the ability understand risk for psychopathology that may arise during this sensitive period of development. Nonetheless, here, we begin to address this issue by synthesizing what is known thus far about normative and pathological neuropsychological changes that occur during the peripartum period and how they relate to biological processes during this time.
Maternal Programming: Typical Developmental Trajectories Associated With Motherhood
It has long been recognized that the fetal period represents a sensitive phase in the human life span. However, what is less frequently acknowledged is that pregnancy and the postpartum period represent sensitive periods of neurological development for the mother as well (Glynn & Sandman, Reference Glynn and Sandman2011). There exists a very well-articulated literature describing maternal changes in brain and behavior in nonhuman animal species, particularly rodents. Leaders in this area of research have documented that as a result of pregnancy, delivery, lactation, and interaction with offspring, females show changes in a range of behavioral domains, such as fear response, aggression, receptiveness to pups, and also domains that more broadly support parental performance including memory and planning (Bridges, Reference Bridges1984; Fahrbach, Morrell, & Pfaff, Reference Fahrbach, Morrell and Pfaff1985; Kinsley et al., Reference Kinsley, Madonia, Gifford, Tureski, Griffin, Lowry and Lambert1999; Li & Fleming, Reference Li and Fleming2003; Numan & Insel, Reference Numan and Insel2003; Numan, Rosenblatt, & Komisaruk, Reference Numan, Rosenblatt and Komisaruk1977). The neurological systems that control the development and onset of maternal behavior also are well described (cf. Barrett & Fleming, Reference Barrett and Fleming2011; Bridges, Reference Bridges2015), and it is established that reproductive history is correlated with neuronal structure, signaling, and neurogenesis (Brummelte, Pawluski, & Galea, Reference Brummelte, Pawluski and Galea2006; Byrnes, Casey, & Bridges, Reference Byrnes, Casey and Bridges2012; Byrnes, Casey, Carini, & Bridges, Reference Byrnes, Casey, Carini and Bridges2013; Keyser-Marcus et al., Reference Keyser-Marcus, Stafisso-Sandoz, Gerecke, Jasnow, Nightingale, Lambert and Kinsley2001; Kinsley et al., Reference Kinsley, Trainer, Stafisso-Sandoz, Quadros, Marcus, Hearon and Lambert2006; Pfaff, Waters, Khan, Zhang, & Numan, Reference Pfaff, Waters, Khan, Zhang and Numan2011; Shingo et al., Reference Shingo, Gregg, Enwere, Fujikawa, Hassam, Geary and Weiss2003).
Despite evidence that most of these neurological changes in rodent mothers persist throughout the life span and may be cumulative (i.e., with additional each litter, the effects are magnified; Gatewood et al., Reference Gatewood, Morgan, Eaton, McNamara, Stevens, Macbeth and Kinsely2005; Lemaire et al., Reference Lemaire, Billard, Dutar, George, Piazza, Epelbaum and Mayo2006; Love et al., Reference Love, Torrey, McNamara, Morgan, Banks, Hester and Lambert2005), little is known about typical changes in the human female during this developmental period, although like other species, several physiological alterations implicated in brain function are apparent. For example, in humans and nonhuman primates, such as rodents, the prenatal endocrine milieu appears to set the stage for the onset and development of sensitive and effective maternal behavior (Bardi, French, Ramirez, & Brent, Reference Bardi, French, Ramirez and Brent2004; Feldman, Weller, Zagoory-Sharon, & Levine, Reference Feldman, Weller, Zagoory-Sharon and Levine2007; Fleming, Ruble, Krieger, & Wong, Reference Fleming, Ruble, Krieger and Wong1997; Glynn, Davis, Sandman, & Goldberg, Reference Glynn, Davis, Sandman and Goldberg2016; Maestripieri & Zehr, Reference Maestripieri and Zehr1998; Saltzman & Abbott, Reference Saltzman and Abbott2009). Further, the hormone exposures of pregnancy predict attachment and persisting alterations in maternal behaviors that are detectable at least as late as the end of the first postpartum year (Glynn et al., Reference Glynn, Davis, Sandman and Goldberg2016).
In addition to overtly maternal behaviors, another widely observed biobehavioral alteration coincident with motherhood is downregulation of stress responding, both physiological and behavioral, in humans (de Weerth & Buitelaar, Reference de Weerth and Buitelaar2005; Glynn, Wadhwa, Dunkel Schetter, Chicz-Demet, & Sandman, Reference Glynn, Wadhwa, Dunkel Schetter, Chicz-Demet and Sandman2001) and other animals (Slattery & Neumann, Reference Slattery and Neumann2008; Vierin & Bouissou, Reference Vierin and Bouissou2001; Wartella et al., Reference Wartella, Amory, Lomas, Macbeth, McNamara, Stevens and Kinsley2003). During pregnancy, in humans, responsiveness of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal and sympathetic–adrenal–medullary systems are progressively dampened (Matthews & Rodin, Reference Matthews and Rodin1992; Nisell, Hjemdahl, Linde, & Lunell, Reference Nisell, Hjemdahl, Linde and Lunell1985a, Reference Nisell, Hjemdahl, Linde and Lunell1985b; Schulte, Weisner, & Allolio, Reference Schulte, Weisner and Allolio1990), and these changes are similarly observed in psychological responses to stressful challenges (Glynn, Dunker Schetter, Wadhwa, & Sandman, Reference Glynn, Dunkel Schetter, Wadhwa and Sandman2004; Glynn et al., Reference Glynn, Wadhwa, Dunkel Schetter, Chicz-Demet and Sandman2001). There is reason to believe that this downregulation of stress responding may serve to protect the mother and fetus from the adverse effects of acute stressors as pregnancy advances; women who do not show this typical change are at increased risk for preterm delivery (Buss et al., Reference Buss, Entringer, Reyes, Chicz-DeMet, Sandman, Waffarn and Wadhwa2009; Glynn, Dunkel Schetter, Hobel, & Sandman, Reference Glynn, Dunkel Schetter, Hobel and Sandman2008). The downregulated stress response then is sustained in the postpartum period by the endocrine profile of lactation (Heinrichs, Neumann, & Ehlert, Reference Heinrichs, Neumann and Ehlert2002) and also is coupled with enhanced aggression in response to threat, which may allow the mother to more successfully protect and provide for her offspring (Hahn-Holbrook, Holt-Lunstad, Holbrook, Coyne, & Lawson, Reference Hahn-Holbrook, Holt-Lunstad, Holbrook, Coyne and Lawson2011). Additional capabilities to protect and tend to young successfully that have been documented in humans during pregnancy include deeper nonconscious processing of facial cues (Raz, Reference Raz2014) and enhanced emotion recognition (Pearson, Lightman, & Evans, Reference Pearson, Lightman and Evans2009).
Unlike the concordance across species in the domain of stress responding and aggression, the patterns in the domain of cognitive function are somewhat less homologous. Among rodent mothers, pregnancy, or manipulation of hormones that simulate the prenatal endocrine milieu, result in enhanced spatial, working, and recognition memory, increased planning abilities, and a reduced latency to prey capture (Kinsley et al., Reference Kinsley, Madonia, Gifford, Tureski, Griffin, Lowry and Lambert1999, Reference Kinsley, Blair, Karp, Hester, McNamara, Orthmeyer and Lambert2014; Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Berry, Griffins, Amory-Meyers, Madonia-Lomas, Love and Kinsley2005; Macbeth, Gautreaux, & Luine, Reference Macbeth, Gautreaux and Luine2008; Pawluski, Vanderbyl, Ragan, & Galea, Reference Pawluski, Vanderbyl, Ragan and Galea2006). However, in humans, there is little evidence of enhanced function in these domains. Many memory and attentional functions appear to be unaffected by pregnancy in humans (Henry & Rendell, Reference Henry and Rendell2007), and furthermore, it has been repeatedly documented that episodic and verbal recall memory are diminished (Glynn, Reference Glynn2010a, Reference Glynn2012; Henry & Rendell, Reference Henry and Rendell2007). Much remains to be determined about what precise aspects of cognitive function are altered as a result of the transition to motherhood and also whether or not these alterations persist as they do throughout the life span in other nonhuman animals.
Most recently, researchers have begun to assess the neurological transformations supporting the transition to motherhood in humans (Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Feldman, et al., Reference Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Feldman, Wang and Swain2010; Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Newman, et al., Reference Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Newman, Feldman and Swain2010; Swain, Lorberbaum, Kose, & Strathearn, Reference Swain, Lorberbaum, Kose and Strathearn2007; Swain et al., Reference Swain, Tasgin, Mayes, Feldman, Constable and Leckman2008). Motherhood is associated with increased functional connectivity in regions that may subserve sensitive and responsive maternal behavior (Atzil, Hendler, & Feldman, Reference Atzil, Hendler and Feldman2011; Atzil et al., Reference Atzil, Touroutoglou, Rudy, Salcedo, Feldman, Hooker and Barrett2017; Swain et al., Reference Swain, Ho, Rosenblum, Morelen, Dayton and Muzik2017). Further, stronger intrinsic connectivity (functional connectivity between brain regions assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging during a resting or task-free state) in the medial-amygdala network is associated with increased dopamine levels within this network, providing some of the first evidence among humans of dopaminergic mechanisms underlying human maternal behavior (Atzil et al., Reference Atzil, Touroutoglou, Rudy, Salcedo, Feldman, Hooker and Barrett2017). Most recent are findings documenting change in gray matter volumes from pre- to postpregnancy (Hoekzema et al., Reference Hoekzema, Barba-Muller, Pozzobon, Picado, Lucco, Garcia-Garcia and Vilarroya2017). Specifically, changes in gray matter volume are related to maternal attachment and are observable at least 2 years after delivery. These emerging findings highlighting the neurological transformation of the human female, make a compelling case for additional investigation of the neural mechanisms that may underlie both risk and resilience for maternal psychopathology.
Fetal Contributions to Maternal Developmental Trajectories
In Reference Bell1968, Bell published a seminal paper challenging existing models of the parent–child relationship as unidirectional, underscoring the reciprocal nature of the processes occurring between parent and child and permanently altering the way in which we characterize this unit. In contrast, this bidirectional conceptualization is largely ignored in the consideration of this unique relationship in the context of the prenatal period. While it is widely recognized that maternal signals shape the development of the fetus, it is not as widely acknowledged that this is only one side of the relationship and that fetal and placental signals also may shape the development of the maternal brain and behavior. In order for the fetus to effect change in maternal phenotypes, signals need to be transmitted from the intrauterine compartment into the maternal compartment. Several modes of signaling are available to the fetus and may transmit information across the fetal–maternal threshold. Abundant previous research has explored signal transmission in the opposite direction, but here we uniquely highlight three “languages” in which signals may be communicated from fetus to mother: (a) hormones; (b) motor activity; and (c) gene transfer.
The maternal–fetal endocrine milieu
The placenta is made up of mostly fetal tissue (villous chorion), and these fetal-identity cells (trophoblasts) produce hormones that are secreted into maternal circulation (Cuffe, Holland, Salomon, Rice, & Perkins, Reference Cuffe, Holland, Salomon, Rice and Perkins2017; Kliman, Reference Kliman, Knobil, Skinner and Neill1999; Mastorakos & Ilias, Reference Mastorakos and Ilias2003). Although there is understanding of how some fetal endocrine signals may influence the development of maternal behaviors and risk for psychopathology (discussed below), it remains largely unknown whether many hormones that are synthesized from the placenta and released into the maternal compartment are involved in shaping maternal phenotypes. This is the case for chorionic gonadotropin (Braunstein, Rasor, Adler, Danzer, & Wade, Reference Braunstein, Rasor, Adler, Danzer and Wade1976), activin (Muttukrishna, Child, Groome, & Ledger, Reference Muttukrishna, Child, Groome and Ledger1997; Petraglia et al., Reference Petraglia, Gallinelli, de Vita, Lewis, Mathews and Vale1994), inhibin (Muttukrishna et al., Reference Muttukrishna, Child, Groome and Ledger1997), relaxin (Eddie et al., Reference Eddie, Lester, Bennett, Bell, Geier, Johnston and Niall1986; Sakbun, Koay, & Bryant-Greenwood, Reference Sakbun, Koay and Bryant-Greenwood1987; Tkachenko, Shchekochikhin, & Schrier, Reference Tkachenko, Shchekochikhin and Schrier2014), leptin (Masuzaki et al., Reference Masuzaki, Ogawa, Sagawa, Hosoda, Matsumoto, Mise and Nakao1997), gonadotropin-releasing hormone (Siler-Khodr, Khodr, & Valenzuela, Reference Siler-Khodr, Khodr and Valenzuela1984), and human placental lactogen (hPL; Kliman, Reference Kliman1994).
Other hormones that traverse the fetomaternal interface have more complicated derivations. Across many species during gestation, predictable and large increases in estrogens and progesterone are observed. Human serum estradiol and progesterone levels during pregnancy reflect placental and maternal (ovarian and adrenal) secretion; however, those observed in the maternal circulation are overwhelmingly of placental origin (Diczfalusy & Troen, Reference Diczfalusy and Troen1962). Placental estradiol and progesterone are interdependent with the maternal system (Mesiano, Reference Mesiano, Strauss and Barbieri2014); the precursor for placental steroid hormones is cholesterol extracted from both maternal and fetal circulation (Tuckey, Reference Tuckey2005). Rodent models demonstrate the important role of sex steroid hormones in the onset and maintenance of maternal behaviors (Brunton & Russell, Reference Brunton and Russell2008; Numan & Insel, Reference Numan and Insel2003; Saltzman & Maestripieri, Reference Saltzman and Maestripieri2011). These connections between sex steroid exposures and quality of maternal behaviors also have been repeatedly observed among nonhuman primates, including tamarins, marmosets, titi monkeys, macaques, and baboons, as well as humans (Bardi, Shimizu, Barrett, Borgognini-Tarli, & Huffman, Reference Bardi, Shimizu, Barrett, Borgognini-Tarli and Huffman2003; Fleming, Ruble, et al., Reference Fleming, Ruble, Krieger and Wong1997; Glynn et al., Reference Glynn, Davis, Sandman and Goldberg2016; Jarcho, Mendoza, & Bales, Reference Jarcho, Mendoza and Bales2012; Pryce, Abbott, Hodges, & Martin, Reference Pryce, Abbott, Hodges and Martin1988).
Oxytocin (OT) is produced in the fetal compartment and secreted into the maternal bloodstream (Dawood, Wang, Gupta, & Fuchs, Reference Dawood, Wang, Gupta and Fuchs1978; Malek, Blann, & Mattison, Reference Malek, Blann and Mattison1996), where it represents approximately 83% of circulating OT (Liu, Reference Liu, Lockwood, Iams and Greene2013; Nakazawa, Makino, Iizuka, Kohsaka, & Tsukada, Reference Nakazawa, Makino, Iizuka, Kohsaka and Tsukada1984). Few longitudinal studies have assessed OT levels during pregnancy, but most of those have documented some evidence of increases across gestation (Dawood, Ylikorkala, Trivedi, & Fuchs, Reference Dawood, Ylikorkala, Trivedi and Fuchs1979; De Geest, Thiery, Piron-Possoyt, & Vanden Driessche, Reference De Geest, Thiery, Piron-Possoyt and Vanden Driessche1985; Levine, Zagoory-Sharon, Feldman, & Weller, Reference Levine, Zagoory-Sharon, Feldman and Weller2007; MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Gold, Feeley, Hayton, Carter and Zelkowitz2014; Silber, Larsson, & Uvnas-Moberg, Reference Silber, Larsson and Uvnas-Moberg1991). OT is crucially involved in gestational and parturitional processes, but also implicated in a wide range of social and attachment behaviors (Bartz, Zaki, Bolger, & Ochsner, Reference Bartz, Zaki, Bolger and Ochsner2011; Campbell, Reference Campbell2010; Heinrichs, von Dawans, & Domes, Reference Heinrichs, von Dawans and Domes2009; Insel, Reference Insel2010; Kirsch et al., Reference Kirsch, Esslinger, Chen, Mier, Lis, Siddhanti and Meyer-Lindenberg2005). The role of OT in controlling maternal behavior across species is experimentally evinced by studies in which intraventricular treatment with OT induces maternal behavior in virgin rats (Pedersen & Prange, Reference Pedersen and Prange1979), intracerebral administration of OT increases approach and touching of infants in macaques (Holman & Goy, Reference Holman, Goy, Pryce, Martin and Skuse1995), and peripheral administration of an OT antagonist reduces maternal interest in marmosets (Seltzer & Ziegler, Reference Seltzer and Ziegler2007). Similarly in humans, women who exhibit higher levels of OT during gestation and the early postpartum period report feeling closer and more attached to their fetuses, display enhanced theory of mind, and exhibit more infant directed gaze, affectionate touch, positive affect, and “motherese” vocalizations (Feldman et al., Reference Feldman, Weller, Zagoory-Sharon and Levine2007; MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Gold, Feeley, Hayton, Carter and Zelkowitz2014).
Corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) is a peptide hormone that is synthesized primarily in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus and plays a central role in regulating pituitary–adrenal function and the physiological response to stress (Vale, Spiess, Rivier, & Rivier, Reference Vale, Spiess, Rivier and Rivier1981). The placenta, fetal membranes, and decidua all synthesize CRH (McLean & Smith, Reference McLean and Smith1999), but it is placental CRH that is released into maternal circulation, resulting in maternal plasma CRH levels that are 1000 times higher during pregnancy than in the nonpregnant state (Mesiano, Reference Mesiano, Strauss and Barbieri2014). In contrast to the inhibitory influence of maternal stress signals (e.g., cortisol) on expression of the CRH gene in the hypothalamus, maternal cortisol activates the promoter region of the gene in the placenta and stimulates CRH synthesis (King, Smith, & Nicholson, Reference King, Smith and Nicholson2001; Scatena & Adler, Reference Scatena and Adler1998). Further, placental CRH stimulates the release of maternal ACTH, which is manufactured from its precursor proopiomelanocortin (POMC) that is partially derived from the placenta (Chen, Chang, Krieger, & Bardin, Reference Chen, Chang, Krieger and Bardin1986). Maternal ACTH stimulates cortisol production, which then stimulates placental CRH (pCRH) synthesis and secretion (Mastorakos & Ilias, Reference Mastorakos and Ilias2003), resulting in a progressive increase in all three hormones across pregnancy. Although influences of pCRH on the regulation of maternal behaviors have yet to be documented, it is worth noting that through this placental signal the fetus could, hypothetically, manipulate maternal phenotypes indirectly through altering cortisol levels that have been implicated in maternal behavior and attachment in humans and other species (Bardi et al., Reference Bardi, French, Ramirez and Brent2004; Fleming, Steiner, & Corter, Reference Fleming, Steiner and Corter1997; Hennessy, Harney, Smotherman, Coyle, & Levine, Reference Hennessy, Harney, Smotherman, Coyle and Levine1977; Rees, Panesar, Steinger, & Fleming, Reference Rees, Panesar, Steinger and Fleming2004).
In addition to the roles of fetoplacental hormones in gestational physiology and onset of maternal psychology and behavior, variations in these hormone levels and trajectories have been implicated in risk for psychopathology. For example, it has been hypothesized that the rapidly declining levels of gonadal steroids may be implicated in postpartum depression (PPD) and postpartum psychosis among vulnerable women (Ahokas, Aito, & Rimon, Reference Ahokas, Aito and Rimon2000; Bloch, Daly, & Rubinow, Reference Bloch, Daly and Rubinow2003; Bloch et al., Reference Bloch, Schmidt, Danaceau, Murphy, Nieman and Rubinow2000; Wieck et al., Reference Wieck, Kumar, Hirst, Marks, Campbell and Checkley1991), midgestation maternal plasma OT concentrations have been associated with PPD (Skrundz, Bolten, Nast, Hellhammer, & Meinlschmidt, Reference Skrundz, Bolten, Nast, Hellhammer and Meinlschmidt2011), and midgestation pCRH has been associated with depressive symptoms both during pregnancy and the postpartum period (Glynn & Sandman, Reference Glynn and Sandman2014; Rich-Edwards et al., Reference Rich-Edwards, Mohllajee, Kleinman, Hacker, Majzoub, Wright and Gillman2008; Yim et al., Reference Yim, Glynn, Dunkel Schetter, Hobel, Chicz-DeMet and Sandman2009). These studies exemplify the possibility for fetal tissue-derived signals to be directly implicated in maternal psychopathology.
Fetal behavior
Although rarely considered, one additional pathway of potential fetal signaling is through fetal motor activity. Spontaneous fetal movement transiently stimulates maternal sympathetic arousal (DiPietro et al., Reference DiPietro, Caulfield, Irizarry, Chen, Merialdi and Zavaleta2006; DiPietro, Irizarry, Costigan, & Gurewitsch, Reference DiPietro, Irizarry, Costigan and Gurewitsch2004). Further, experimentally evoking mild startle response in the fetus, which is characterized by increases motor activity, generates a transient maternal heart rate suppression and an increase in sympathetic activation (DiPietro et al., Reference DiPietro, Voegtline, Costigan, Aguirre, Kivlighan and Chen2013). The mother does not appear to habituate to the fetal movement stimulus, which continues to invoke a maternal physiological response for the duration of gestation (DiPietro, Costigan, & Voegtline, Reference DiPietro, Costigan and Voegtline2015). The precise biological pathway through which fetal movements might affect maternal arousal is currently unknown. However, it is unlikely that this occurs through conscious perception of these movements. At term, women detect as few as 16% of fetal movements (Johnson, Jordan, & Paine, Reference Johnson, Jordan and Paine1990), which is consistent with the fact that although they are relatively skilled at detecting large or prolonged fetal movements, pregnant women are limited in their ability to detect smaller spontaneous or evoked fetal movements (Kisilevsky, Killen, Muir, & Low, Reference Kisilevsky, Killen, Muir and Low1991). Given that the pathway likely does not operate through conscious channels, DiPietro et al. (Reference DiPietro, Irizarry, Costigan and Gurewitsch2004) propose that the most plausible mechanism is through perturbations of the uterine wall. They further suggest that these interactions may have broader implications for the role of the fetus in shaping maternal behavior, specifically suggesting that the sympathetic activation in response to the fetal movement signal may begin to prepare the female for the impending demands of motherhood by redirecting maternal resources away from competing but less relevant environmental demands. These findings and assertions raise the provocative question of whether the degree of prenatal synchrony between mother and fetus might set the stage for postnatal mother–infant interaction.
Fetal microchimerism
In Reference Herzenberg, Bianchi, Schroder, Cann and Iverson1979, Herzenberg, Bianchi, Schroder, Cann, and Iverson demonstrated the presence of cells containing a Y chromosome in the plasma of women who were pregnant with male fetuses. Subsequently, cells containing male DNA were demonstrated in the plasma of healthy women decades after giving birth to a son (Bianchi, Zickwolf, Weil, Sylvester, & DeMaria, Reference Bianchi, Zickwolf, Weil, Sylvester and DeMaria1996), a phenomenon described as fetal microchimerism. During pregnancy, there is an asymmetric bidirectional exchange of maternal and fetal cells across the placental barrier (more fetal cells transferred to mother than vice versa; Lo, Lau, Chan, Leung, & Chang, Reference Lo, Lau, Chan, Leung and Chang2000). Fetal cells have been detected in a range of human maternal tissues, including thyroid, heart, liver, lungs, adrenals, kidneys, and bone marrow (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Nelson, Furst, McSweeney, Roberts, Zhen and Bianchi2001; Khosrotehrani, Johnson, Cha, Salomon, & Bianchi, Reference Khosrotehrani, Johnson, Cha, Salomon and Bianchi2004). A debate exists regarding the purpose of these fetal cells, and whether they exert salutary or detrimental influences on the mother's health and development (Boddy, Forunato, Sayres, & Aktipis, Reference Boddy, Forunato, Sayres and Aktipis2015). For example, there is accumulating evidence that they may play a role in maternal wound healing (Mahmood & O'Donoghue, Reference Mahmood and O'Donoghue2014; Nassar et al., Reference Nassar, Droitcourt, Mathieu-d'Argent, Kim, Khosrotehrani and Aractingi2012), but these cells also have been identified at tumor sites (Kallenbach, Johnson, & Bianchi, Reference Kallenbach, Johnson and Bianchi2011) and have been associated with pregnancy complications (Gammill, Aydelotte, Guthrie, Nkwopara, & Nelson, Reference Gammill, Aydelotte, Guthrie, Nkwopara and Nelson2013; Gammill, Stephenson, Aydelotte, & Nelson, Reference Gammill, Stephenson, Aydelotte and Nelson2014). Potentially relevant to the issue of maternal programming is the finding that, although the blood–brain barrier usually prevents the passage of cells, during pregnancy it appears that fetal cells may migrate to the maternal brain (Chan et al., Reference Chan, Gurnot, Montine, Sonnen, Guthrie and Nelson2012). These fetal cells are capable of taking on a range of attributes including neuron-, astrocyte- and oligodendrocyte-like cell types, conceivably allowing participation in neural circuitry and molecular communication (Tan et al., Reference Tan, Liao, Sun, Okabe, Xiao and Dawe2005; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Tan, Yeo, Sasajala, Tan, Xiao and Udolph2010). Whether fetal cells in the maternal brain have any functional significance has yet to be demonstrated. Nonetheless, fetal cells are preferentially found in brain regions known to subserve maternal behavior, such as the olfactory bulb (Tan et al., Reference Tan, Liao, Sun, Okabe, Xiao and Dawe2005), raising the possibility that the attraction of fetal cells to specific brain areas could represent a pathway through which the fetus affects development and onset of maternal behavior and may have implications for risk for maternal psychopathology (Boddy et al., Reference Boddy, Forunato, Sayres and Aktipis2015; Glynn, Reference Glynn, Zimmermann and Connors2010b).
Evolutionary Perspectives on Maternal Programming
A hallmark of the developmental psychopathology perspective is the emphasis on an interdisciplinary approach. Viewing the maternal–fetal relationship in an evolutionary context can help explain why (and with what ultimate consequence) maternal biology, psychology, and behavior are responsive to fetal signals. Until the 1970s, maternal–fetal biology was generally presumed to reflect a synergistic, cooperative relationship, but Trivers (Reference Trivers1974) reframed the parent–child relationship in terms of an evolutionary system of “parent–offspring conflict,” which overturned the peaceful paradigm of maternal–fetal harmony.
Natural selection has shaped the process by which the fetus extracts resources from the mother, and how the mother provides for the fetus, but the evolutionary incentives of a mother and her fetus are slightly misaligned (Parker, Royle, & Hartley, Reference Parker, Royle and Hartley2002; Schrader & Travis, Reference Schrader and Travis2009). This evolutionary tug-of-war is played out, at least mostly, unconsciously, so when we describe mother and offspring “interests” or assign agency to their “strategies,” this is biological shorthand for describing the machinations of natural selection that favor perpetuation of certain allelic variants over others. From the mother's perspective, the adaptive value of investment (energy, time, and risk) in her pregnancy must be weighed against the costs (energy, attention, nutrition, and risk) to her existing children plus depletion of resources available for her future pregnancies. Meanwhile, selection on fetal traits prioritizes benefits to the fetus itself over the cost of impairing the mother from providing for its siblings and diminishing the likelihood of future siblings being born (Godfray, Reference Godfray1995). In this way, pregnancy becomes a battleground with mother and fetus selected to prefer different amounts of maternal investment, with selection promoting mutual manipulation.
How do the fetus and mother send, interpret, and respond to communication between each other if those messages have been selected to be, sometimes, misleading, threatening, or coercive (Haig, Reference Haig1996)? Many hormones produced by the fetoplacental unit flow into maternal circulation. A large body of research in evolutionary ecology (particularly ornithology) has investigated how selection shapes the “honesty” of offspring-to-mother signals (Godfray, Reference Godfray1995; Kilner & Johnstone, Reference Kilner and Johnstone1997). In competitive circumstances with limited resources, an offspring may compensate for her mother's and siblings’ lower, compared to her own, optimal investment in her by exaggerating her needs (Royle, Hartley, & Parker, Reference Royle, Hartley and Parker2002). For example, a nestling bird may beg in a way that indicates its need for food is far greater than it really is (Kilner, Reference Kilner1995; Kilner, Noble, & Davies, Reference Kilner, Noble and Davies1999), because incomplete indulgence of an exaggerated request will (approximately) result in fulfillment of the needed amount. However, from the mother's point of view, if the fetus's honest needs cannot be distinguished from dishonest signals, the mother's best strategy is to ignore all signals (Haig, Reference Haig1996). The ensuing selective forces can be framed as a game-theory cascade (Bergstrom, Reference Bergstrom1995) in which fetus preempts anticipated maternal strategy by mimicking signals the mother uses to communicate between different parts of her body that govern the transfer of resources (Haig, Reference Haig1993), producing hormones biochemically identical (e.g., CRH) or closely resembling (e.g., hPL/luteinizing hormone) nonplacental hormones, which the mother preempts by downregulating maternal production of those hormones (e.g., LH; progesterone) to isolate fetal signals, although the advantage of gaining conveyance between mother and fetus is balanced against the cost of losing conveyance between parts of the mother's body (Haig, Reference Haig1996). Ultimately, the fetus is advantaged because placental hormones can “corrupt a mother's internal lines of communication” (Haig, Reference Haig1993). Otherwise inexplicable enormous levels of placental hormones in maternal circulation may be due to this arms-race escalation from selection for fetal exploitation of maternal hormone receptors alongside selection for maternal resistance by downregulating receptor expression (Haig, Reference Haig1993).
The mother's evolutionarily optimal investment in the offspring is, theoretically, calibrated to optimize her number of surviving descendants by balancing investment in her own health versus reproduction, and her current versus future pregnancies. This adaptive appraisal takes into account the mother's somatic resources (physical health and stored energy), number, ages, and quality (health and competence) of her current offspring, and the mother's own age and likelihood to have future offspring (Daly & Wilson, Reference Daly, Wilson, Parmigiani and vom Saal1995; Williams, Reference Williams1966). The appraisal is a constantly updating assessment across a woman's reproductive life phase, as well as across the trajectory of any given reproductive event. For example, low-cost (to the mother in terms of time and energy investment) decision points, such delaying ovulation, are more sensitive to resource scarcity than high-cost decision points, such as spontaneous abortion, which poses a greater health risk to the mother and wastes the time and energy that was already devoted to that pregnancy. Mechanisms of intrinsic monitoring of somatic condition and age may include signals of cellular senescence such as telomere length and/or p53 and Rb protein levels (Ben-Porath & Weinberg, Reference Ben-Porath and Weinberg2005). Mechanisms of extrinsic monitoring of offspring condition and quantity are more difficult to identify but hypothetically could be linked to maternal hormones involved in gestation, lactation, social bonding, or stress.
The maternal–fetal conflict paradigm has broad implications for mental health during (and after) pregnancy. The endocrinology of pregnancy has been implicated in maternal psychological health (Beddoe, Paul Yang, Kennedy, Weiss, & Lee, Reference Beddoe, Paul Yang, Kennedy, Weiss and Lee2009; Glynn, Davis, & Sandman, Reference Glynn, Davis and Sandman2013), physical health (Magness, Reference Magness and Bazer1998; Schlomer, Del Giudice, & Ellis, Reference Schlomer, Del Giudice and Ellis2011), cognitive performance (Glynn, Reference Glynn, Zimmermann and Connors2010b; Henry & Sherwin, Reference Henry and Sherwin2012), and behavior (Glynn et al., Reference Glynn, Davis, Sandman and Goldberg2016), so fetal signaling, both honest and manipulative, may influence function in these domains. Moreover, the degree to which a mother valuates investing in her fetus versus current and future offspring varies between mothers and within a mother across her life span. Maternal–fetal conflict decreases as the mother's age increases, because of the inherently diminishing likelihood of future pregnancies (Trivers, Reference Trivers1974). This may result in more harmonious maternal attitudes and health outcomes for later-age mothers.
A woman's energetic investment or even emotional attachment to her fetus may be adaptively calibrated to respond to fetal “vigor,” signals of survival likelihood such as health, strength, or size. The evolutionary cost of diverting time and resources away from current or future offspring and toward a fetus unlikely to survive may result in selection favoring maternal physiologic divestment strategies such as spontaneous abortion (Forbes, Reference Forbes1997; Nepomnaschy et al., Reference Nepomnaschy, Welch, McConnell, Low, Strassmann and England2006; Williams, Reference Williams1966), behavioral divestment strategies such as limiting the mother's own food intake to feed her existing children more, or emotional divestment strategies. The area of women's mental health where these ideas have received the most attention is PPD (Thornhill & Furlow, Reference Thornhill and Furlow1998). If the mother receives cues during (after) pregnancy of low fetal (neonatal) vigor, emotional and behavioral divestment from that one offspring may strategically enhance a woman's lifetime fertility (number of offspring) by allowing her to shift allocation of resources to instead invest in current or future offspring with higher likelihood of survivorship (Hagen, Reference Hagen1999, Reference Hagen2002). In this way, selection may have favored for women to respond to signals of poor fetal growth, for instance, with downregulated emotional bonding or even prenatal depression, in preparation for limiting investment in the fetus once it is born (via neglect, or in extreme cases, infanticide (Hagen, Reference Hagen1999, Reference Hagen2002). While these ideas remain controversial, they highlight how viewing maternal psychobiology within the context of evolutionary theory can be useful. The expectation that it is “natural” for women to bond with their fetus/baby and aberrations from this phenotype represent pathology that could be treated by tapping into some deep, innate instinct is misguided, and may partly explain the lack of efficacy in clinical strategies for preventing or treating antepartum and PPD (Dennis, Reference Dennis2005).
Intergenerational Transmission of Maternal Phenotypes
Understanding developmental transitions requires a consideration of both proximal and distal influences (Lewis, Reference Lewis1999). While this manuscript mostly describes events that occur during a woman's parturient years of life, it is also important to acknowledge the fetal and early life development of a person who will later become a mother. An important characteristic of prenatal programming is that, while some of its influence is evident from the time of birth, it also exerts influence on neuro-psycho-physiologic systems in ways that manifest much later in life. In particular, prenatal programming influences lifelong responsivity to both intrinsic and extrinsic conditions, which may only become relevant once the apposite conditions are encountered. Maternal phenotypes appear to be plastic in a way that is sensitive to conditions the mother encountered during her own prenatal and early life development. In this way, prenatal programming and early life experiences of one generation can influence the prenatal programming of the next generation.
Women vary widely in the attributes that are involved in the psychological, behavioral, and physiological traits, proclivities, and abilities involved in motherhood. For example, women are differentially adept at sensing infant needs, communicating with and comforting their infants (Ainsworth, Reference Ainsworth1979; Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley, & Tuckey, Reference Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley and Tuckey2001), have variation in postpartum sleep difficulties (Hunter, Rychnovsky, & Yount, Reference Hunter, Rychnovsky and Yount2009), and are differentially physiologically efficient with milk letdown (Neifert, Seacat, & Jobe, Reference Neifert, Seacat and Jobe1985). The variance in human maternal phenotypes can be partially ascribed to intergenerational trends (Belsky, Jaffee, Sligo, Woodward, & Silva, Reference Belsky, Jaffee, Sligo, Woodward and Silva2005; Chen & Kaplan, Reference Chen and Kaplan2001; Smith & Farrington, Reference Smith and Farrington2004; Thornberry, Freeman-Gallant, Lizotte, Krohn, & Smith, Reference Thornberry, Freeman-Gallant, Lizotte, Krohn and Smith2003). These maternal characteristics may be perpetuated across generations by a dynamic combination of environmental, educational, cultural, and biological influences. Although there is substantial evidence that these continuities can be attributed in part to learning processes, we emphasize that key aspects of maternal phenotypes are plastic and sensitive to early life programming. These include executive function (Buss, Davis, Hobel, & Sandman, Reference Buss, Davis, Hobel and Sandman2011), social competence, and affective profiles (Davis, Glynn, Waffarn, & Sandman, Reference Davis, Glynn, Waffarn and Sandman2011; Howland, Sandman, Glynn, Crippen, & Davis, Reference Howland, Sandman, Glynn, Crippen and Davis2016; Lombardo et al., Reference Lombardo, Ashwin, Auyeung, Chakrabarti, Lai, Taylor and Baron-Cohen2012), as well as the physiological mediators of mothering behavior, including stress endocrinology (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Glynn, Waffarn and Sandman2011; Glover, O'Connor, & O'Donnell, Reference Glover, O'Connor and O'Donnell2010), neural circuitry (Babenko, Kovalchuk, & Metz, Reference Babenko, Kovalchuk and Metz2015; Meaney, Szyf, & Seckl, Reference Meaney, Szyf and Seckl2007), and neurotransmitter/receptor sensitivity and expression (Herlenius & Lagercrantz, Reference Herlenius and Lagercrantz2004). Evidence from various mammalian species indicates that maternal behavioral phenotypes are plastic, sensitive to early life programming effects, and can be broadcast across generations without alteration to genetic code (Francis, Diorio, Liu, & Meaney, Reference Francis, Diorio, Liu and Meaney1999; Gonzalez, Lovic, Ward, Wainwright, & Fleming, Reference Gonzalez, Lovic, Ward, Wainwright and Fleming2001; Maestripieri, Lindell, & Higley, Reference Maestripieri, Lindell and Higley2007). A developmental psychology approach emphasizes these ontogenic, dynamically interacting aspects of psychological development (i.e., an ecological–transactional model of development; Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti, Toth, Luthar, Burack, Cicchetti and Weisz1997). The multilevel, developmental programming influences on the psychological, behavioral, and physiological traits, proclivities, and abilities involved in motherhood exemplify the importance of acknowledging the dynamic interplay between influences and processes across not only an individual's life span but also across generations to understand the origins of maternal phenotypes.
Maternal Programming: Implications for Psychopathology
As discussed above, there is substantial plasticity in neurobiological systems regulating affective, cognitive and social functioning during the perinatal period (Duan, Cosgrove, & Deligiannidis, Reference Duan, Cosgrove and Deligiannidis2017; Kim, Strathearn, & Swain, Reference Kim, Strathearn and Swain2016; Lonstein, Maguire, Meinlschmidt, & Neumann, Reference Lonstein, Maguire, Meinlschmidt and Neumann2014; Moses-Kolko, Horner, Phillips, Hipwell, & Swain, Reference Moses-Kolko, Horner, Phillips, Hipwell and Swain2014; Rutherford, Wallace, Laurent, & Mayes, Reference Rutherford, Wallace, Laurent and Mayes2015),Footnote 1 resulting in heightened vulnerability for psychopathology. Most attention on risk for psychopathology during the transition to motherhood has been directed toward perinatal depression, which affects up to 20% of women (Gavin et al., Reference Gavin, Gaynes, Lohr, Meltzer-Brody, Gartlehner and Swinson2005; O'Hara & McCabe, Reference O'Hara and McCabe2013). Shortly after delivery, up to 85% of women experience a profile of symptoms known as postpartum blues or baby blues, characterized by dysphoric mood, mood fluctuations, crying, anxiety, insomnia, and irritability (O'Hara & McCabe, Reference O'Hara and McCabe2013; O'Hara, Schlechte, Lewis, & Wright, Reference O'Hara, Schlechte, Lewis and Wright1991). These subclinical symptoms usually resolve within 10-12 days, unless they develop into PPD (O'Hara & McCabe, Reference O'Hara and McCabe2013). There is some evidence that the early postpartum period is a time of increased risk for major depressive episodes (although a debate exists regarding this issue; Di Florio et al., Reference Di Florio, Forty, Gordon-Smith, Heron, Jones, Craddock and Jones2013; O'Hara, Zekoski, Philipps, & Wright, Reference O'Hara, Zekoski, Philipps and Wright1990; Vesga-Lopez et al., Reference Vesga-Lopez, Blanco, Keyes, Olfson, Grant and Hasin2008) and that PPD may be phenotypically distinct from depression occurring outside of the perinatal period. Specifically, sad mood may be less prominent (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Rush, Yonkers, Carmody, Woo, McConnell and Trivedi2008), and anxiety symptoms appear to be more prominent (Hendrick, Altshuler, Strouse, & Grosser, Reference Hendrick, Altshuler, Strouse and Grosser2000; Ross, Gilbert Evans, Sellers, & Romach, Reference Ross, Gilbert Evans, Sellers and Romach2003).
Perinatal anxiety disorders are often overlooked (Farr, Dietz, O'Hara, Burley, & Ko, Reference Farr, Dietz, O'Hara, Burley and Ko2014; Goodman, Watson, & Stubbs, Reference Goodman, Watson and Stubbs2016; Ross & McLean, Reference Ross and McLean2006), despite that they are frequently comorbid with perinatal depression and may be more prevalent (Matthey, Barnett, Howie, & Kavanagh, Reference Matthey, Barnett, Howie and Kavanagh2003; Reck et al., Reference Reck, Struben, Backenstrass, Stefenelli, Reinig, Fuchs and Mundt2008; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Gilbert Evans, Sellers and Romach2003; Ross & McLean, Reference Ross and McLean2006; Russell, Fawcett, & Mazmanian, Reference Russell, Fawcett and Mazmanian2013). Anxieties during pregnancy and the postpartum period may be uniquely directed toward the health and safety of the fetus/infant or the maternal role/parenting abilities (Fallon, Halford, Bennett, & Harrold, Reference Fallon, Halford, Bennett and Harrold2016; Martini et al., Reference Martini, Petzoldt, Einsle, Beesdo-Baum, Hofler and Wittchen2015; Phillips, Sharpe, Matthey, & Charles, Reference Phillips, Sharpe, Matthey and Charles2009). Pregnancy-specific anxiety is defined as anxiety related to maternal and fetal health, labor and delivery, and parenting and has been identified as a separate clinical phenomenon distinct from measures of general anxiety during pregnancy (Blackmore, Gustafsson, Gilchrist, Wyman, & O'Connor, Reference Blackmore, Gustafsson, Gilchrist, Wyman and O'Connor2016; Dunkel Schetter & Glynn, Reference Dunkel Schetter, Glynn, Contrada and Baum2011; Dunkel Schetter & Tanner, Reference Dunkel Schetter and Tanner2012; Kane, Dunkel Schetter, Glynn, Hobel, & Sandman, Reference Kane, Dunkel Schetter, Glynn, Hobel and Sandman2014; Misri, Abizadeh, Sanders, & Swift, Reference Misri, Abizadeh, Sanders and Swift2015). The peripartum period is also characterized by increased risk for onset or exacerbation of obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), with 40%–80% of women experiencing symptoms at subclinical levels (Maina, Albert, Bogetto, Vaschetto, & Ravizza, Reference Maina, Albert, Bogetto, Vaschetto and Ravizza1999; Miller, Hoxha, Wisner, & Gossett, Reference Miller, Hoxha, Wisner and Gossett2015; Misri et al., Reference Misri, Abizadeh, Sanders and Swift2015; Ross & McLean, Reference Ross and McLean2006; Russell et al., Reference Russell, Fawcett and Mazmanian2013; Zambaldi et al., Reference Zambaldi, Cantilino, Montenegro, Paes, de Albuquerque and Sougey2009). The most common of these symptoms are obsessive concerns related to accidentally or intentionally harming the fetus or infant. These intrusive, violent thoughts are more prevalent in peripartum-onset OCD than in OCD without peripartum onset (Uguz, Akman, Kaya, & Cilli, Reference Uguz, Akman, Kaya and Cilli2007) and, along with other OCD symptoms, are also more common in perinatal depression as compared to nonperinatal depression (Altemus et al., Reference Altemus, Neeb, Davis, Occhiogrosso, Nguyen and Bleiberg2012; Wisner, Peindl, Gigliotti, & Hanusa, Reference Wisner, Peindl, Gigliotti and Hanusa1999).
Of the multiple forms of psychopathology for which the peripartum period confers heightened risk, the relative risk for onset and recurrence of bipolar disorder is most pronounced. The relative risk of first-time hospitalization for bipolar disorder is 23 times higher in the first postpartum month as compared to any other phase of the life span (Munk-Olsen, Laursen, Pedersen, Mors, & Mortensen, Reference Munk-Olsen, Laursen, Pedersen, Mors and Mortensen2006), and in women known to have bipolar disorder, rates of recurrence are 50%–70% (Viguera et al., Reference Viguera, Nonacs, Cohen, Tondo, Murray and Baldessarini2000, Reference Viguera, Whitfield, Baldessarini, Newport, Stowe, Reminick and Cohen2007). In addition, women with first onset of unipolar depression during the postpartum period appear to be at higher risk for subsequent conversion to bipolar disorder as compared to women with non-postpartum onset (Munk-Olsen, Laursen, Meltzer-Brody, Mortensen, & Jones, Reference Munk-Olsen, Laursen, Meltzer-Brody, Mortensen and Jones2012; Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Xie, Campbell, Penava, Hampson, Mazmanian and Pope2014). Bipolar disorder can present as postpartum psychosis (Jones & Craddock, Reference Jones and Craddock2001), which is considered a psychiatric emergency and is rare, with an estimated prevalence of 1–2 in 1,000 (Sit, Rothschild, & Wisner, Reference Sit, Rothschild and Wisner2006). Symptoms of postpartum psychosis include severe sleep disturbances, rapid fluctuations in mood, mood-incongruent delusions, hallucinations, disorganized behavior, obsessions about the infant, and cognitive symptoms like disorientation and confusion (Bergink, Rasgon, & Wisner, Reference Bergink, Rasgon and Wisner2016; Jones, Chandra, Dazzan, & Howard, Reference Jones, Chandra, Dazzan and Howard2014; Sit et al., Reference Sit, Rothschild and Wisner2006). Psychotic symptoms can also occur at subclinical levels and are more common in women with postpartum-onset depression as compared to pregnancy-onset depression (Altemus et al., Reference Altemus, Neeb, Davis, Occhiogrosso, Nguyen and Bleiberg2012).
Consistent with contemporary symptom-based, dimensional approaches to the study of psychiatric disorders (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013), a developmental psychopathology perspective recognizes that psychopathology occurs on a continuum and that contextual factors are critical in defining boundaries between adaptive and maladaptive development along this continuum. For example, somatic symptoms related to sleep, energy, weight, and appetite that are characteristic of psychopathological conditions in non-perinatal phases can result from normative physiological processes of pregnancy and postpartum (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Rush, Yonkers, Carmody, Woo, McConnell and Trivedi2008; Goodman & Dimidjian, Reference Goodman and Dimidjian2012; Howard et al., Reference Howard, Molyneaux, Dennis, Rochat, Stein and Milgrom2014; Matthey & Ross-Hamid, Reference Matthey and Ross-Hamid2011; Misri et al., Reference Misri, Abizadeh, Sanders and Swift2015; Nylen, Williamson, O'Hara, Watson, & Engeldinger, Reference Nylen, Williamson, O'Hara, Watson and Engeldinger2013) and may not necessarily indicate psychopathology. Relatedly, symptoms such as heightened anxiety or preoccupation regarding the infant may be a normative feature of new motherhood and represent a healthy maternal response to the infant initially, promoting vigilance, threat detection, and harm avoidance (Leckman et al., Reference Leckman, Mayes, Feldman, Evans, King and Cohen1999, Reference Leckman, Feldman, Swain, Eicher, Thompson and Mayes2004; Wisner et al., Reference Wisner, Peindl, Gigliotti and Hanusa1999). There is evidence of a normative trajectory of these symptoms, which appear to peak in the immediate postpartum period and then begin to diminish by 3–4 months postpartum (Fairbrother & Woody, Reference Fairbrother and Woody2008; Kim, Mayes, Feldman, Leckman, & Swain, Reference Kim, Mayes, Feldman, Leckman and Swain2013; Leckman et al., Reference Leckman, Mayes, Feldman, Evans, King and Cohen1999). Excessive and prolonged worry, or, on the other end of the continuum, an absence of worry, may indicate psychopathology (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Strathearn and Swain2016; Leckman et al., Reference Leckman, Feldman, Swain, Eicher, Thompson and Mayes2004). Furthermore, the content of these early parental preoccupations may determine their course, whereby positive, idealizing thoughts of the infant may promote positive outcomes (Leckman et al., Reference Leckman, Feldman, Swain, Eicher, Thompson and Mayes2004).
Perinatal psychopathology is complex, multiply determined, and heterogeneous in presentation and course. Multiple developmental pathways leading to various manifestations of perinatal psychopathology have been identified and include factors both specific and nonspecific to the peripartum period. Non-peripartum-specific risk factors include a history of psychopathology, low social support, stressful life events, low socioeconomic status, obstetric complications, and personality factors such as insecure attachment style and low self-esteem (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Watson and Stubbs2016; Goodman & Tully, Reference Goodman and Tully2009; Howard et al., Reference Howard, Molyneaux, Dennis, Rochat, Stein and Milgrom2014; Martini et al., Reference Martini, Petzoldt, Einsle, Beesdo-Baum, Hofler and Wittchen2015; Milgrom et al., Reference Milgrom, Gemmill, Bilszta, Hayes, Barnett, Brooks and Buist2008; O'Hara & McCabe, Reference O'Hara and McCabe2013; O'Hara & Wisner, Reference O'Hara and Wisner2014; Paschetta et al., Reference Paschetta, Berrisford, Coccia, Whitmore, Wood, Pretlove and Ismail2014; Robakis et al., Reference Robakis, Williams, Crowe, Lin, Gannon and Rasgon2016; Tebeka, Strat, & Dubertret, Reference Tebeka, Strat and Dubertret2016; Vesga-Lopez et al., Reference Vesga-Lopez, Blanco, Keyes, Olfson, Grant and Hasin2008; Yim, Tanner Stapleton, Guardino, Hahn-Holbrook, & Dunkel Schetter, Reference Yim, Tanner Stapleton, Guardino, Hahn-Holbrook and Dunkel Schetter2015). The most consistently reported perinatal-specific risk factor is an increased sensitivity to the hormone changes of the peripartum period, with both reproductive and stress hormones implicated. Most of this work has focused on PPD (Bloch et al., Reference Bloch, Schmidt, Danaceau, Murphy, Nieman and Rubinow2000, Reference Bloch, Daly and Rubinow2003; Brummelte & Galea, Reference Brummelte and Galea2010; Glynn & Sandman, Reference Glynn and Sandman2014; Rich-Edwards et al., Reference Rich-Edwards, Mohllajee, Kleinman, Hacker, Majzoub, Wright and Gillman2008; Skrundz et al., Reference Skrundz, Bolten, Nast, Hellhammer and Meinlschmidt2011), but there is also evidence for hormone sensitivity in the etiology of perinatal OCD (Labad et al., Reference Labad, Vilella, Reynolds, Sans, Cavalle, Valero and Gutierrez-Zotes2011; McDougle, Barr, Goodman, & Price, Reference McDougle, Barr, Goodman and Price1999) and psychosis (Ahokas et al., Reference Ahokas, Aito and Rimon2000; Bergink et al., Reference Bergink, Rasgon and Wisner2016; Wieck et al., Reference Wieck, Kumar, Hirst, Marks, Campbell and Checkley1991). Other less-studied but promising biomarkers include genetic and epigenetic (Costas et al., Reference Costas, Gratacos, Escaramis, Martin-Santos, de Diego, Baca-Garcia and Sanjuan2010; Guintivano, Arad, Gould, Payne, & Kaminsky, Reference Guintivano, Arad, Gould, Payne and Kaminsky2014; Jones & Craddock, Reference Jones and Craddock2007), inflammatory (Bergink et al., Reference Bergink, Burgerhout, Weigelt, Pop, de Wit, Drexhage and Drexhage2013; Kendall-Tackett, Reference Kendall-Tackett2007; Yim et al., Reference Yim, Tanner Stapleton, Guardino, Hahn-Holbrook and Dunkel Schetter2015), and circadian factors (Lewis, Foster, & Jones, Reference Lewis, Foster and Jones2016; Sharma, Reference Sharma2003). Multidisciplinary approaches to understanding perinatal risk are warranted and should examine how biological, psychological, and social factors interact to shape developmental trajectories.
The timing of onset of perinatal psychopathology may also determine symptom profiles and course (Di Florio & Meltzer-Brody, Reference Di Florio and Meltzer-Brody2015; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Wisner, Clark, Sit, Luther and Wisniewski2016; Martini et al., Reference Martini, Petzoldt, Einsle, Beesdo-Baum, Hofler and Wittchen2015; Postpartum Depression: Action Towards and Treatment Consortium, 2015). Perinatal psychopathology that is a recurrence or exacerbation of pre-peripartum symptoms may be distinguished from psychopathology with onset specifically linked to the perinatal period. For example, women with postpartum psychosis usually have one of two disease courses: postpartum psychosis as an extension of bipolar disorder, or isolated postpartum psychosis, with vulnerability only after childbirth (Bergink et al., Reference Bergink, Rasgon and Wisner2016; Sit et al., Reference Sit, Rothschild and Wisner2006; Yonkers et al., Reference Yonkers, Wisner, Stowe, Leibenluft, Cohen, Miller and Altshuler2004). Two distinct expressions of PPD are similarly indicated (Kettunen, Koistinen, & Hintikka, Reference Kettunen, Koistinen and Hintikka2014), with those women exhibiting first-onset PPD at higher risk for subsequent PPD episodes as compared to women whose PPD represents a recurrence of previous non-perinatal depression (Cooper & Murray, Reference Cooper and Murray1995).
As a sensitive window of development, the perinatal period may also confer opportunities for resilience to psychopathology. Resilience during this life phase may manifest as an absence of psychopathology despite risk, or as competence in the tasks associated with the transition to motherhood in the face of risk (cf. Masten, Reference Masten2001). While little research has focused on mechanisms of maternal resilience during the peripartum period (Dunkel Schetter, Reference Dunkel Schetter2011), several studies have identified social support as protective against perinatal depressive symptoms in high-risk women (deCastro, Hinojosa-Ayala, & Hernandez-Prado, Reference deCastro, Hinojosa-Ayala and Hernandez-Prado2011; Howell, Mora, DiBonaventura, & Leventhal, Reference Howell, Mora, DiBonaventura and Leventhal2009; Ritter, Hobfoll, Lavin, Cameron, & Hulsizer, Reference Ritter, Hobfoll, Lavin, Cameron and Hulsizer2000). Further research is needed to identify additional psychological, biological, social, and cultural factors that promote well-being in vulnerable women (Dunkel Schetter, Reference Dunkel Schetter2011). Perinatal-specific mechanisms may be particularly interesting and important to consider. Psychological resilience factors such as self-esteem, mastery, and self-efficacy may be enhanced with opportunities for success in achieving the tasks of parenthood and therefore particularly relevant (Howell et al., Reference Howell, Mora, DiBonaventura and Leventhal2009). It is also plausible that the physiological changes of pregnancy and the postpartum period protect against certain symptoms or conditions. As discussed above, pregnancy and lactation are characterized by downregulated stress responsivity, which may have direct implications for resilience to psychopathology in women at risk. Consistent with this possibility, one investigation observed markedly reduced symptomology in women with bipolar disorder, type I, during pregnancy as compared to before or after pregnancy (Grof et al., Reference Grof, Robbins, Alda, Berghoefer, Vojtechovsky, Nilsson and Robertson2000). Furthermore, breastfeeding has been prospectively linked to reduced PPD symptoms (Hahn-Holbrook, Haselton, Dunkel Schetter, & Glynn, Reference Hahn-Holbrook, Haselton, Dunkel Schetter and Glynn2013).
Collectively, existing studies highlight the importance of considering the unique features of the perinatal period that contribute to increased risk for and resilience to psychopathology and inform symptom trajectories. Furthermore, it is clear that there are multiple potential pathways of vulnerability for or protection from perinatal psychopathology. Attention to these distinctive features and differential pathways can advance our understanding not only of perinatal psychopathology but also of trajectories of risk and resilience for psychopathology across the life span. Finally, while most research has focused on the consequences of maternal perinatal psychopathology for the developmental trajectories of the offspring, consequences for the developing mother are important to consider in their own right.
Conclusions
A growing body of literature suggests a remarkable neural plasticity associated with reproductive experience. In 1971, Marian Diamond provided a striking example of such plasticity, by demonstrating that the cortical sizes of pregnant rats housed in impoverished conditions matched those of nonpregnant rats housed in enriched conditions (Diamond, Johnson, & Ingram, Reference Diamond, Johnson and Ingram1971). For the first time, Diamond's work demonstrated that pregnancy remodels the architecture of the female brain. As with other sensitive developmental periods in the human life span, the transition to motherhood represents an epoch that is well suited for the application of a developmental psychopathology perspective. This is a period in which the context, including the singular embedded relationship with the fetus and the fact that maternal and fetal programming are proceeding in parallel, coupled with the unique developmental tasks requiring mastery by the new mother, result in a significant transformation that is not yet fully understood nor properly characterized in humans. Furthering our understanding of how typical development during this transition proceeds, along with the ways in which trajectories may deviate from the normative, can aid in the understanding of psychopathology and potential interventions. Similarly, understanding how psychopathological states manifest during pregnancy and the postpartum period can inform our understanding of this most fundamental transformation in the female life span.