Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T22:58:46.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The intergenerational effects of paternal incarceration on children’s social and psychological well-being from early childhood to adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2022

Juan Del Toro*
Affiliation:
Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Adam Fine
Affiliation:
School of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
Ming-Te Wang
Affiliation:
Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Department of Psychology and School of Education, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Juan Del Toro, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The present study sought to unravel the psychological processes through which mass incarceration, specifically paternal incarceration, is negatively affecting the next generation of children. Data came from 4,327 families from 20 cities who participated in a 10-year longitudinal study. Parents and children reported on children’s rule-breaking behaviors and depressive symptoms when they were on average ages 5 (2003–2006), 9 (2007–2010), and 15 (2014–2017). Parental surveys and disposition information were combined to assess paternal incarceration at each age. Results showed that children who experienced paternal incarceration at age 5 also demonstrated more rule-breaking behaviors at age 15. Children’s age-9 depressive symptoms partially mediated our finding, such that children who experienced paternal incarceration at age 5 also showed greater depressive symptoms at age 9, which in turn predicted greater rule-breaking behaviors at age 15. Paternal incarceration predicted future rule-breaking behaviors more strongly than did other forms of father loss. Because we found paternal incarceration during childhood is associated with worsened adjustment into adolescence, we discussed the need for developmentally appropriate practices in the criminal justice system.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Mass incarceration threatens the next generation of America’s children. More than half of the people incarcerated in the United States are parents (Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010), and 91% of them are male (Loesche, Reference Loesche2017). As such, millions of children experience father loss due to the criminal justice system (i.e., father or paternal incarceration). Children with incarcerated fathers experience the trauma of father loss (Arditti, Reference Arditti2016), economic instability (Geller et al., Reference Geller, Garfinkel and Western2011), and disruption in family structure (Turney & Wildeman, Reference Turney and Wildeman2013). Because paternal incarceration reduces the interpersonal and conventional resources necessary for children’s socioemotional well-being, children growing up with incarcerated fathers tend to exhibit psychological (Gifford et al., Reference Gifford, Eldred Kozecke, Golonka, Hill, Costello, Shanahan and Copeland2019; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Testa, Semenza and Vaughn2021), physical (Del Toro et al., Reference Del Toro, Fine, Wang, Thomas, Schneper, Mitchell, Mincy, McLanahan and Notterman2021; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Fang and Luo2013), behavioral (Boch et al., Reference Boch, Warren and Ford2019; Murray, Farrington, et al., Reference Murray, Farrington and Sekol2012; Ruhland et al., Reference Ruhland, Davis, Atella and Shlafer2020), and academic maladjustment (Haskins, Reference Haskins2017; Jacobsen, Reference Jacobsen2019; Testa & Jackson, Reference Testa and Jackson2021).

The negative consequences of paternal incarceration are unequivocal, but the psychological and developmental underpinnings of these consequences warrant attention, specifically relating to how and when paternal incarceration undermines children’s development. To that end, we examine whether paternal incarceration is longitudinally linked to children’s engagement in rule-breaking behaviors. To understand mechanisms motivating this link and build on the existing literature, we test (a) whether children’s depressive symptoms mediate the link between paternal incarceration and their rule-breaking behaviors, (b) whether our findings are specific to paternal incarceration relative to other forms of father loss, and (c) whether the associated consequences of paternal incarceration are stable across childhood and adolescence.

Paternal incarceration and children’s rule-breaking behaviors

Paternal incarceration is part of a constellation of social and economic stressors that result from and perpetuate social inequalities (Pearlin, Reference Pearlin1989, Reference Pearlin2010). The criminal justice system represents a slice of a larger system disadvantaging ethnic-racial minority and low-income families. For instance, residential segregation has funneled families of color into low-income neighborhoods and White families into economically affluent ones (Massey & Denton, Reference Massey and Denton1989, Reference Massey and Denton1993). Due to the interaction between ethnicity-race and socioeconomic status in neighborhoods, ethnic-racial minority men who are taken into custody are less able to pay bail and more likely to be incarcerated as pretrial detainees (National Research Council, 2014; Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010). These inequities outside of the criminal justice system compound ethnic-racial biases within the system as young individuals of color are more likely to receive more severe sentences than their White peers for similar level infractions (National Research Council, 2014; Rattan et al., Reference Rattan, Levine, Dweck and Eberhardt2012).

Above and beyond preexisting adversities, paternal incarceration presents novel adversities that strain the family (Lee & Wildeman, Reference Lee and Wildeman2021; Turney, Reference Turney2014b). For instance, fathers with a history of incarceration are unlikely to find employment and receive employment- and family-related benefits (i.e., health insurance; Laub, Reference Laub2014; Turney, Reference Turney2017), limiting mothers views toward their partners as marriageable men who can provide financial support to the family and increasing couples’ likelihood of divorce or separation (Geller, Reference Geller2013; Widdowson et al., Reference Widdowson, Jacobsen, Siennick and Warren2020). As mothers contend with decreased romantic relationship quality and the challenges associated with a single mother status (Del Toro et al., Reference Del Toro, Fine, Wang, Thomas, Schneper, Mitchell, Mincy, McLanahan and Notterman2021; Wildeman et al., Reference Wildeman, Schnittker and Turney2012), children of incarcerated fathers are more likely to stay low-income over time (Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010), live in a single-parent household (Geller et al., Reference Geller, Cooper, Garfinkel, Schwartz-Soicher and Mincy2012), and experience neglectful or harsh parenting (Turney, Reference Turney2014a).

To no surprise, scholars have found that paternal incarceration and its related events predict increases in children’s rule-breaking behaviors (Jones & Pierce, Reference Jones and Pierce2020; Wildeman, Reference Wildeman2020). For instance, young children who experienced paternal incarceration also reported greater engagement in rule-breaking behaviors concurrently (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Testa, Semenza and Vaughn2021; Wakefield & Wildeman, Reference Wakefield and Wildeman2011) and into adulthood (McCauley, Reference McCauley2020). Many scholars have used the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing (FFCW) Study to document the criminogenic consequences of paternal incarceration among children (Turney & Haskins, Reference Turney, Haskins, Eddy and Poehlmann-Tynan2019; Wildeman, Reference Wildeman2020). Scholars using these data have found that paternal incarceration experienced between birth and childhood predicted children’s rule-breaking behaviors cross-sectionally and later in childhood (Dwyer Emory, Reference Dwyer Emory2018; Haskins, Reference Haskins2015; Wakefield & Wildeman, Reference Wakefield and Wildeman2011). Overall, one can conclude that there are mechanisms linking paternal incarceration and children’s rule-breaking behaviors (Pearlin, Reference Pearlin2010), but researchers need to disentangle the boundaries of when these links emerge.

The mediating role of children’s depressive symptoms

When paternal incarceration occurs, children may turn to rule-breaking behaviors to cope with their negative affect linked to father loss. In his General Strain Theory, Agnew (Reference Agnew2013) suggests that individuals engage in rule-breaking behaviors when they (1) dislike their unjust and involuntary conditions, (2) develop negative emotions following self-appraisal of these conditions, and (3) consider rule-breaking as an outlet to reduce stress. This framework outlines how children may respond when they are hurt and stressed due to a parent’s absence. Indeed, in response to paternal incarceration, children show greater emotional distress (Poehlmann, Reference Poehlmann2005; Wakefield & Wildeman, Reference Wakefield and Wildeman2011) and higher cumulative stress hormone concentration and blunted cortisol reactivity (Muentner et al., Reference Muentner, Kapoor, Weymouth and Poehlmann-Tynan2021). Individuals experiencing an event that causes pain and stress have a proclivity to lash out at others in response (Berkowitz, Reference Berkowitz1989; Del Toro et al., Reference Del Toro, Lloyd, Buchanan, Robins, Bencharit, Smiedt, Reddy, Pouget, Kerrison and Goff2019; Dollard, Reference Dollard1939). Children’s sadness arising from paternal incarceration may lead to aggression and other forms of reactive rule-breaking behaviors as a means of maladaptive coping (Porter & King, Reference Porter and King2015), but no study has formally tested whether these negative psychological processes shape children’s rule-breaking behaviors following paternal incarceration. Our assessment of children’s depressive symptoms as a mediator may inform interventions working to avert lifelong deficits in well-being, because paternal imprisonment during childhood has predicted greater physical health issues and psychiatric disorders in adulthood (Gifford et al., Reference Gifford, Eldred Kozecke, Golonka, Hill, Costello, Shanahan and Copeland2019).

Paternal incarceration versus other forms of father loss

The associations between paternal incarceration and children’s maladjustment may be distinct from other forms of father loss (i.e., separation, divorce, and death). Paternal incarceration carries stigma and uncertainty, as the duration of imprisonment and the rehabilitation process are often unclear. This stigma and uncertainty stress families’ emotional and financial stability (Arditti, Reference Arditti2016). In child-aged samples, participants who experienced paternal incarceration during their lifetimes exhibited more rule breaking than peers who experienced none and other paternal losses (i.e., hospitalization/death, separation, or incarceration prior to their birth; Murray, Loeber, et al., Reference Murray, Loeber and Pardini2012; Murray & Farrington, Reference Murray and Farrington2005). However, these studies comparing the consequences associated with various forms of father loss have relied on small sample sizes and on particular social groups (i.e., boys in London, Murray & Farrington, Reference Murray and Farrington2005; boys in an urban mid-Atlantic city, Murray, Loeber, et al., Reference Murray, Loeber and Pardini2012), limiting their abilities to reliably make such comparisons and their generalizability. To address these limitations, we utilize the strengths of the FFCW study as it includes a large sample of children from families who have experienced different forms of parental separation, providing us with enough power to make reliable comparisons. In addition, these children were recruited from 20 urban cities across the United States, enabling us to compare the effects of different forms of father loss among a larger and more representative sample of inner-city children.

Developmental considerations

Extant stress frameworks suggest competing hypotheses regarding the impact of adverse experiences across childhood and adolescence. For instance, a biological embedding model espouses that early childhood is a sensitive period for stress internalization as stress early in life is hypothesized to calibrate physiological stress management processes and how they function over the lifespan (Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). On the other hand, social and cognitive processes may be more volatile for adolescents, as adolescents have more autonomy to venture out into the world with less parental supervision, engage in more sensation-seeking and risk-taking behaviors (Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2008), and have a greater awareness of unjust practices in the criminal justice system (Fagan & Tyler, Reference Fagan and Tyler2005). When paternal incarceration occurs, adolescents may more likely than children to recognize unjust judicial practices and have more autonomy to engage in risky behaviors as a form of maladaptive coping.

Scholars have found empirical support for both stress models. For instance, some studies provide evidence for a biological embedding framework when paternal incarceration predicts harmful adjustment during early childhood but not during middle childhood or adolescence (Jones & Pierce, Reference Jones and Pierce2020; Kalu et al., Reference Kalu, Menon and Quinn2020; Turney, Reference Turney2021). Other studies have found the effects of paternal incarceration proliferated across developmental periods, as paternal incarceration predicted poor adjustment in samples of both children (Geller et al., Reference Geller, Cooper, Garfinkel, Schwartz-Soicher and Mincy2012; Murray, Farrington, et al., Reference Murray, Farrington and Sekol2012; Wildeman, Reference Wildeman2010) and adolescents (Boch et al., Reference Boch, Warren and Ford2019; Kjellstrand et al., Reference Kjellstrand, Yu, Eddy and Martinez2018; Ruhland et al., Reference Ruhland, Davis, Atella and Shlafer2020). These contradictory findings may be fueled by the fact that existing studies differ from each other in critical ways, including sample characteristics (e.g., adolescent girls; Kalu et al., Reference Kalu, Menon and Quinn2020), analytic approaches (e.g., inverse probability of treatment weighting; Turney, Reference Turney2021), and study designs (e.g., cross-sectional; Boch et al., Reference Boch, Warren and Ford2019). Thus, substantial work is required to further understand the pattern of extant findings.

The present study

The present study leveraged a large and longitudinal cohort study of urban American families and examined the longitudinal relations between paternal incarceration and their children’s engagement in rule-breaking behaviors during childhood and adolescence. Using three waves of data, we posed four research questions: (1) Did children who experienced paternal incarceration at ages 5 and 9 also, respectively, show greater rule-breaking behaviors at ages 9 and 15? (2) Did children’s depressive symptomatology at age 9 mediate the longitudinal link between paternal incarceration at age 5 and children’s rule-breaking behaviors at age 15? (3) Did other forms of father loss similarly predict children’s rule-breaking behaviors over time as did paternal incarceration? (4) Did the effects of paternal incarceration on children’s engagement in rule-breaking behaviors vary across childhood and adolescence? In addition to our core research questions, we tested the direction of effects in our hypotheses and explored possible social group differences in our sample. Namely, we tested whether children’s adjustment predicted paternal incarceration longitudinally and whether children’s gender and ethnicity-race moderated our results.

Methods

Participants

Our sample included 4,327 families who participated in the FFCW study, a longitudinal, cohort study of children hailing from primarily low-income and unmarried parents from 20 large urban cities (Reichman et al., Reference Reichman, Teitler, Garfinkel and McLanahan2001). Participants were selected as part of the analytic sample if they had participated in at least one wave during the ages-5, -9, and -15 assessments (see details in the supplemental digital content, SDC).

Table 1 presents demographic characteristics for the analytic sample. The sample was predominantly families of color as White families made up 16% of the sample, and many of these families were low-income as the average for maternal education suggests that mothers unlikely had a bachelor’s degree (i.e., a proxy for socioeconomic status; American Psychological Association, 2006). For the full sample, families on average lived in ethnically-racially diverse neighborhoods; however, subethnic-racial group differences revealed that families lived in ethnically-racially stratified neighborhoods. That is, families lived in neighborhoods where most residents shared their ethnic-racial identification. These neighborhoods were also socioeconomically stratified as more adults in White families’ neighborhoods had bachelor’s degrees than did adults in non-White families’ neighborhoods. The rate of paternal separation/divorce was observably low (i.e., 20%); however, this rate is expectedly low as many families were unmarried at baseline. Overall, the demographic information represents the FFCW study’s principal investigators’ mission to target socially disadvantaged families.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of demographic and key study measures for the analytic sample

Note. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Procedure

The FFCW study is a deidentified, publicly available data set that randomly sampled approximately 5,000 births from 75 hospitals in 20 large US cities between 1998 and 2000. Mothers and fathers were interviewed soon after the birth of the focal child, and follow-up interviews were conducted when children were approximately ages 1, 3, 5, 9, and 15. We focused on the last three waves, when key constructs were available. No ethics approval was sought, as the University of Pittsburgh’s Institutional Review Board does not require review for the analysis of deidentified, publicly available data [exempt Criteria 45 CFR 46.104 (d) (4)].

Measures

Paternal incarceration

Parents’ surveys and survey contractors’ disposition information were combined to assess paternal incarceration (Haskins, Reference Haskins2014; McLeod et al., Reference McLeod, Johnson, Cryer-Coupet and Mincy2019). The age-5 assessment represented whether fathers were ever incarcerated, whereas ages-9 and-15 assessments indicated whether fathers were incarcerated at the time of the survey (0 = no, 1 = yes).

Children’s rule-breaking behaviors

Primary caregivers completed the nine-item Rule-Breaking Behaviors subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/4–18; e.g., “child steals outside the home”; Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001) at ages 5, 9, and 15. Child-reported engagement in rule-breaking behaviors was measured using modified rule-breaking scales from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth and the National Longitudinal Survey of Adolescent Health (Maumary-Gremaud, Reference Maumary-Gremaud2000) at ages 9 (17-item) and 15 (13-item). The SDC contains a list of items and detailed information about how each item was a binary indicator of whether the child had engaged in each specific behavior (0 = never, 1 = ever); thereafter, each reporter’s measure was summed to generate continuous count scores at each wave. We used multigroup CFAs to assess whether parent- and child-surveys consistently measured children’s rule-breaking behaviors at ages 9 and 15. Results suggested that we met the criteria for metric invariance across time, χ2 (2) = 32.51, p < .001, RMSEA = 0.07, 90% CI [0.05, 0.09], CFI = 0.96, SRMR = 0.03; therefore, we assessed children’s engagement in rule-breaking behaviors as observed count scores using only parent-reported data at age 5 while combining parent- and child-reports at ages 9 and 15. Higher scores on rule-breaking behaviors suggested that children engaged in more types of rule-breaking behaviors.

Children’s depressive symptoms

Primary caregivers completed the Internalizing Symptoms subscale (e.g., “child is unhappy, sad or depressed”; Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001) at ages 5 (20-item; α = 0.74), 9 (21-item; α = 0.85), and 15 (8-item; α = 0.79). Child-reported depressive symptoms came from the Self-Description Questionnaire (8-item; e.g., “I often feel lonely”; Marsh, Reference Marsh1990) at age 9 and from the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (Radloff, Reference Radloff1977) at age 15. Each scale demonstrated acceptable reliability at ages 9 (eight-item; α = 0.78) and 15 (depressive symptoms: five-item; α = 0.75; anxiety: six-item, α = 0.76). The SDC contains detailed information regarding how each item across sources and waves were recoded to have consistent three-point Likert scales (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat/sometimes true, 2 = very true or very often). We used multigroup CFAs to assess whether parent- and child-surveys consistently measured children’s depressive symptoms at ages 9 and 15. Results suggested that we met the criteria for partial metric invariance across time, χ2 (3) = 13.45, p < .01, RMSEA = 0.03 90% CI [0.02, 0.05], CFI = 0.99, SRMR = 0.01. Hence, using only parent-data at age 5 and combining parent and child reports at ages 9 and 15, we assessed children’s depressive symptoms as observed mean scores. Higher scores on depressive symptoms suggested that children exhibited more frequent depressive symptoms.

Other forms of father loss

In line with prior studies (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Testa, Semenza and Vaughn2021; McCauley, Reference McCauley2020, Reference McCauley2021; Porter, Reference Porter2014; Testa et al., Reference Testa, Jackson, Vaughn and Bello2020), we used a strategic comparison framework to compare children who experienced paternal incarceration with their peers who experienced other forms of father loss. We created two comparison groups to account for some of the unobserved characteristics associated with paternal incarceration that may overlap with other forms of father loss. The first group included children who did not experience paternal incarceration between ages 5 and 15 but experienced other forms of father loss (i.e., separation, divorce, or death) between their ages 5 and 15 assessments; these children (n = 682) were assigned a value of 1, and all other children received a value of 0. The second group included children who did not experience paternal incarceration between ages 5 and 15 but their fathers were incarcerated at any point before children’s age-1 assessment; these children (n = 958) received a value of 1, and all other children were assigned a value of 0.

Covariates

On the basis of extant research (Murray & Farrington, Reference Murray and Farrington2005; Wildeman, Reference Wildeman2010), we accounted for several confounds that could bias the link between paternal incarceration and children’s outcomes. Individual-level covariates included children’s age, gender (0 = girls, 1 = boys), ethnicity-race, maternal education (1 = less than high school, 4 = college or beyond), maternal incarceration, maternal depression, paternal alcohol/drug use, and paternal lifetime incarceration at children’s age-1 assessment. Using the year 2000 decennial census data, our neighborhood-level covariates included the ethnic-racial diversity in children’s census tracts, which we generated using the Sampson Diversity Index (0 = complete ethnic-racial heterogeneity, 1 = complete ethnic-racial homogeneity; Graham, Reference Graham2016; Simpson, Reference Simpson1949), and the percentage of adult residents ages 25 and up with high school and bachelor’s degrees.

Missing data

Among the analytic sample (n = 4,327), 2,446 families participated in all three assessments, 1,316 families participated in two waves, and 565 participated in one wave. The participation rates were 73% in age 5, 87% in age 9, and 83% in age 15. Relative to families who participated in more waves, families who participated in fewer waves were more likely to be Latino than White, to have a mother with a lower educational degree, to live in a neighborhood with a greater proportion of Black and Latino residents than White residents, and to live in a neighborhood where fewer adults have advanced degrees. After accounting for our covariates, partial correlations indicated that families’ participation was related to two of the 14 key constructs; specifically, families who completed more waves had lower primary caregiver ratings of children’s engagement in rule-breaking behaviors and internalizing symptoms in age 5, and these relationships were nonsignificant in later waves. These missing data patterns reflect a common problem faced by research conducted in socioeconomically disadvantaged communities. To retain sample variability and diversity, we used FIML (Baraldi & Enders, Reference Baraldi and Enders2010). FIML and multiple imputation perform better than other missing data approaches when missing data patterns are random (e.g., listwise deletion of cases with missing values, or singly imputing missing values; Baraldi & Enders, Reference Baraldi and Enders2010). Results using FIML were similar to those using multiple imputation (see SDC for our detailed imputation procedure); thus, FIML results were retained and presented.

Analytic plan

All analyses were conducted in Mplus version 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998-2019) using weighted least square means and variance-adjusted estimation to adjust standard errors for dichotomous variables (e.g., paternal incarceration). We used a random intercept crosslagged panel model (RI-CLPM; see Figure 1 for a visual depiction; Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). The RI-CLPM builds on the traditional crosslagged and autoregressive path model to estimate both between- and within-person effects. The between-person effect is represented by the inclusion of a random intercept to partial out trait-like and time-invariant stability for each construct, and the within-person portion is reflected in the crosslagged parameters (e.g., fathers’ incarceration age 5→children’s rule-breaking age 9), stability coefficients (e.g., fathers’ incarceration at age 5→fathers’ incarceration at age 9), and within-wave correlations (e.g., fathers’ incarceration age 5←→children’s rule-breaking age 5). Notably, the inclusion of stability coefficients permits us to account for paternal incarceration that occurred across the study period, including in scenarios when paternal incarceration occurred in earlier waves only, latter waves only, each wave, or never. In addition, the RI-CLPM allowed us to treat each child as their own control group and examine the degree to which children experienced deviations on outcome variables when their fathers were incarcerated.

Figure 1. A visual depiction of the random intercept crosslagged panel model from children’s ages 5 to 15. Notes: Solid lines indicate hypothesized significant paths; dashed lines indicate our hypothesized nonsignificant paths. The longitudinal direct effects between paternal incarceration and children’s rule-breaking behaviors are suppressed to ease visual representation.

To answer our first research question, we used a RI-CLPM to examine longitudinal interrelations between fathers’ incarceration and children’s rule-breaking behaviors. For our second research question, we estimated a second RI-CLPM to examine whether children’s depressive symptoms mediated the longitudinal interrelations between fathers’ incarceration and children’s rule-breaking behaviors. To do so, we used the widely established nonparametric bootstrapping technique to generate standard errors with the multivariate delta method to examine the indirect effects of fathers’ incarceration on children’s rule-breaking behaviors via their depressive symptoms (MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West and Sheets2002). For our third research question, we performed chi-square test comparisons to examine whether other forms of father loss predicted children’s rule-breaking behaviors. All covariates were included as controls in all analyses; see SDC for output pertaining to the relations between covariates and our key constructs across all analyses. For our fourth and final research question, we tested whether the longitudinal interrelations between paternal incarceration and children’s outcomes could be constrained to be invariant over time without causing a significant decrement in model fit, in accordance with suggestions from Kenny (Reference Kenny1975) and Kenny and Harackiewicz (Reference Kenny and Harackiewicz1979).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 includes descriptive statistics, and Table 2 includes zero-order bivariate correlations. Table SDC2 presents correlations between covariates and key constructs. Approximately 45% of children experienced paternal incarceration prior to age 5, 7% experienced it between ages 5 and 9, and 15% experienced it between ages 9 and 15. Across reporters, children on average engaged in at least one rule-breaking behavior and experienced few depressive symptoms at each wave.

Table 2. Zero-order bivariate correlations among key study variables (n = 4,327)

Note. Bolded values indicate p-values less than .05; nonbolded values indicate p-values .05 and greater.

In Table 1, we also compared children who never experienced paternal incarceration (n = 2,284) with those who have experienced it at least once (n = 2,043). Relative to families who never experienced paternal incarceration, families who did were more likely to be non-White, have a mother with a less advanced educational degree, live in a neighborhood where adults unlikely had high school and bachelor’s degrees, and experience other adversities (i.e., maternal incarceration, maternal depression, and paternal alcohol and drug use). We also found that children from families who experienced paternal incarceration engaged in more rule-breaking behaviors and had higher levels of depression, regardless of children’s and parents’ self-reports, than those from families who never experienced paternal incarceration.

Paternal incarceration and children’s rule-breaking behaviors

Table SDC3 presents unstandardized estimates for the interrelations between paternal incarceration and children’s rule-breaking behaviors. Children who experienced fathers’ incarceration at ages 5 and 9 also engaged in greater rule-breaking behaviors at ages 9 and 15, respectively. Children’s rule-breaking behaviors did not predict subsequent paternal incarceration. The model fit the data well, χ2 (47) = 160.63, p < .001, RMSEA = 0.02, 90% CI [0.02, 0.03], CFI = 0.97, WRMR = 0.99.

The role of children’s depressive symptoms as a mediator

Table 3 presents unstandardized coefficients for the interrelations among paternal incarceration, children’s depressive symptoms, and children’s rule-breaking behaviors, after controlling for covariates (see Table SDC4 for estimates with covariates). Children who experienced paternal incarceration at ages 5 and 9 demonstrated greater depressive symptoms at ages 9 and 15, respectively, but depressive symptoms did not predict paternal incarceration in later assessments. In turn, greater depressive symptoms at ages 5 and 9 predicted more rule-breaking behaviors at ages 9 and 15, respectively, but rule-breaking behaviors did not predict depressive symptoms in later assessments. Paternal incarceration at age 5 had a positive and significant indirect effect on age-15 rule-breaking behaviors via age-9 depressive symptoms (B = 0.14, SE = 0.06, 95% CI [0.02, 0.26], p < .05). The effect of paternal incarceration on subsequent rule-breaking behaviors remained significant (B = 1.67, SE = 0.23, 95% CI [1.15, 2.19], p < .001), suggesting that age-9 depressive symptoms partially mediated the link between age-5 paternal incarceration and age-15 rule-breaking behaviors. The models explained a significant amount of variance in children’s depressive symptoms and rule-breaking behaviors (R 2-range = 0.14–0.30, p-range < .001), and the model fit the data well, χ2 (70) = 401.37, p < .001, RMSEA = 0.03, 90% CI [0.03, 0.04], CFI = 0.96, WRMR = 1.12.

Table 3. Unstandardized coefficients from a random intercept crosslagged panel model (n = 4,327) and after controlling for covariates (suppressed to ease visual representation)

Note. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

The specificity of paternal incarceration

We assessed whether the two strategic comparison groups predicted children’s rule-breaking behaviors to the same degree as paternal incarceration. The model in which we constrained the effects of these strategic comparison groups and paternal incarceration on children’s rule-breaking behaviors to be equivalent to each other resulted in a significant decrement in model fit, Δχ2 (2) = 46.63, p < .001. The first comparison group pertaining to paternal separation, divorce, or death was unrelated to children’s rule-breaking behaviors (B = −0.06, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [−0.14, 0.01], p = ns). The second group pertaining to paternal lifetime incarceration before the age-1 assessment predicted greater rule-breaking behaviors among children (B = 0.15, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [0.07, 0.22], p < .001), but this effect was significantly weaker than that for paternal incarceration, Δχ2 (1) = 9.23, p < .001.

The stability in effects of paternal incarceration on children’s future outcomes

In examining whether the effect of paternal incarceration on children’s outcomes waned or strengthened over time, we found that constraining the effect of paternal incarceration on children’s rule-breaking behaviors to be invariant over time did not yield a significant decrement in model fit [Δχ2 (1) = 0.05, p = ns]. Overall, these results suggest that paternal incarceration is harmful to children’s development across childhood and adolescence.

Sensitivity analyses

We tested the direction of effects among our key constructs. We found that neither depressive symptoms nor rule-breaking behaviors predicted paternal incarceration one wave later, and rule-breaking behaviors also did not predict depressive symptoms one wave later, supporting the direction of effects in our hypotheses (see Table 3).

Children’s gender and ethnicity-race may have moderated our results, as boys may have been more impacted by the loss of a gender-congruent parent. In addition, children of color may be disproportionately impacted by paternal incarceration, ethnicity-race-related factors related to paternal incarceration, or both. We used gender as a categorical grouping variable for multigroup analyses to test whether key pathways between girls and boys can be constrained to be equivalent to each other without causing a significant decrement in model fit. We found that constraining the effects of fathers’ incarceration on future children’s rule-breaking behaviors [Δχ2 (1) = 0.01, p = ns] and internalizing symptoms [Δχ2 (1) = 1.74, p = ns] to be equivalent between the two gender groups did not yield a significant decrement in model fit. Overall, these results suggest that the effects of father incarceration did not differ by children’s gender.

For children’s ethnicity-race, we focused on the four large ethnic-racial groups (i.e., 1959 Black, 696 White, 935 Latino, and 633 multiracial children). We chose to drop 104 cases where “Other” was selected as the ethnic-racial group because the sample size for Other ethnic-racial minority children was too small for us to reliably examine each group separately from the four larger ethnic-racial groups. Next, we used children’s ethnic-racial identification as a grouping variable for multigroup analyses. Ultimately, constraining the effects to be equivalent across groups did not result in a significant decrement in model fit when examining children’s rule-breaking [Δχ2 (6) = 6.10, p = ns] and internalizing symptoms as outcomes [Δχ2 (6) = 5.06, p = ns]. Thus, ethnicity-race did not significantly moderate the effects of father incarceration.

Discussion

In the United States, one-in-100 adults have been incarcerated, many of whom are fathers (Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010). Although the negative consequences of incarceration on fathers’ opportunities for upward social mobility have been documented (Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010), the mechanisms driving the intergenerational consequences of paternal incarceration are less understood. Controlling for relevant individual and neighborhood factors, we found that children who experienced paternal incarceration prior to age 5 engaged in more rule-breaking behaviors at ages 9 and 15. This link was partially mediated by children’s age-9 depressive symptoms. In other words, paternal incarceration at age 5 predicted greater depressive symptoms at age 9, which in turn predicted more rule-breaking behaviors at age 15. Notably, paternal incarceration during children’s lifetime predicted children’s rule-breaking behaviors more strongly than did other forms of father loss, and this effect of paternal incarceration on future rule-breaking behaviors was similar in size across childhood and adolescence.

Half of children experienced paternal incarceration by their adolescent years. It is important to consider this rate relative to our sample’s demographic characteristics. Specifically, our sample was predominantly low-income, with 83% identifying themselves as a member of an ethnic-racial minority group. In addition, most of our ethnic-racial minority sample lived in low-income communities, whereas our White sample lived in more economically affluent and predominantly White neighborhoods. Thus, unmeasured structural factors, such as ethnic-racial bias and punitive municipal policies, may have contributed to the observed high rate of paternal incarceration in the present study (Eberhardt, Reference Eberhardt2019; Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010).

Our findings indicated that children who experienced paternal incarceration also reported greater engagement in rule-breaking behaviors over time. Depressive symptoms partially mediated this link. These findings align with extant stress frameworks (Agnew, Reference Agnew2013; Pearlin, Reference Pearlin2010) and provide partial insight into why paternal incarceration during early childhood predicted greater rule-breaking behaviors during adolescence (Agnew, Reference Agnew2013; Arditti, Reference Arditti2016). For instance, paternal incarceration limits children’s supportive relationships due to having an absent father and a stressed mother, and it can also increase the likelihood of parental separation/divorce and decrease their likelihood of repartnering (Geller, Reference Geller2013; Widdowson et al., Reference Widdowson, Jacobsen, Siennick and Warren2020). In addition to these contentious interpersonal contexts, children of incarcerated parents are situated in neighborhoods fraught with social and economic barriers that increase risks for paternal incarceration and reduce families’ resources to navigate the criminal justice system (Christian & Thomas, Reference Christian and Thomas2009; Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010). Indeed, our sample characteristics reflected a population of families living in low-income neighborhoods. Thus, when paternal incarceration occurred in our study, children were more likely to dislike their circumstances and feel upset and angry about their father’s absence. To cope with their negative affect, as we found, children engaged in more rule-breaking behaviors across childhood and into adolescence.

Other forms of father loss did not predict children’s rule-breaking behaviors to the same degree as paternal incarceration did. Specifically, according to our strategic comparison approach, a father’s absence due to separation, divorce, or death was unrelated to children’s rule-breaking behaviors. Although children engaged in greater rule-breaking behaviors when their father had a history of incarceration prior to their age-1 assessment, this effect on children’s rule-breaking behaviors was weaker relative to that for children who experienced paternal incarceration when they were older than one. Although social and structural factors likely shaped the negative consequences of paternal incarceration before children’s age-1 assessment, the fact that the effect was stronger for children who experienced paternal incarceration after age 1 highlighted that children’s self-appraisal processes matter vis-à-vis their rule-breaking behaviors (Agnew, Reference Agnew2013). That is, when paternal incarceration occurs before the age of 1, children may unlikely recollect a father’s absence as they would during early and middle childhood.

The longitudinal effect of paternal incarceration on children’s rule-breaking behaviors is notable from a methodological standpoint. Using data from the FFCW study, scholars found that paternal incarceration during adolescence was unrelated to rule-breaking behaviors concurrently among adolescent girls (Kalu et al., Reference Kalu, Menon and Quinn2020) and the entire sample (Turney, Reference Turney2021). However, there are notable inconsistencies between these studies: One documented 2%–4% of the sample experienced paternal incarceration (Kalu et al., Reference Kalu, Menon and Quinn2020) and the other found a rate of 16% (Turney, Reference Turney2021). The latter rate is consistent with what we found in the present study. When Turney (Reference Turney2021) found that paternal incarceration was related to children’s rule-breaking behaviors during early childhood but not during middle childhood or adolescence, the question of whether the effects of paternal incarceration on children’s adjustment significantly differed across these age groups was left unaddressed until the present study. Nonetheless, we found that paternal incarceration during ages 5 and 9 predicted children’s rule-breaking behaviors during ages 9 and 15, respectively, and these findings corroborate those from extant studies using data from the FFCW study (Dwyer Emory, Reference Dwyer Emory2018; Haskins, Reference Haskins2015).

Neither children’s gender nor ethnicity-race moderated the longitudinal effects of paternal incarceration on children’s adjustment. Although the null result for gender may appear inconsistent with prior studies at face value (Geller et al., Reference Geller, Cooper, Garfinkel, Schwartz-Soicher and Mincy2012; Wildeman, Reference Wildeman2010), prior studies did not formally test whether equality constraints could be imposed between girls and boys, which may have contributed to the differential results. As for our results regarding the moderating role of children’s ethnicity-race, our findings are unsurprising. Prior studies using the FFCW data set have found nonsignificant ethnic-racial variation in the impacts of paternal incarceration on children’s adjustment (Turney & Haskins, Reference Turney and Haskins2014; Turney & Wildeman, Reference Turney and Wildeman2013). While the effects of paternal incarceration may not be relatively large among specific ethnic-racial groups, ethnic-racial disparities in rates of paternal incarceration may still spur deleterious effects among particular ethnic-racial groups if they are disproportionately exposed to the stressor.

Limitations

The present study has several limitations that can guide future research. First, we did not specify the reason for incarceration, as prior research has found that families did not experience negative consequences when a father was incarcerated for violent infractions (Wildeman, Reference Wildeman2010). Second, we assessed depressive symptoms among a vulnerable sample of low-income children; it is possible that the low levels of depressive symptoms among our sample may be attributed to their resilience and reluctance to report on their symptoms. Third, our children primarily had unwedded parents; therefore, our findings may be weaker than those derived from children with wedded parents due to the loss of stronger, more proximal relationships with their fathers. Finally, the observed effect sizes were small, which may be due to the 4-to-6-year time intervals between key measures and the omission of specific facets of paternal incarceration that are harmful (e.g., children’s number of trial/court hearings, number of jail/prison visitations, and duration of a fathers’ absence due to incarceration). Future research should use more frequent observations to unpack nuanced changes in children’s adjustment during and following paternal incarceration while also exploring whether these factors moderate the associations between paternal incarceration and children’s developmental outcomes.

Strengths

Despite these limitations, the present study has conceptual and methodological strengths that contribute to the literature. First, the longitudinal associations between paternal incarceration and children’s developmental outcomes neither waned nor strengthened over time, showing that paternal incarceration is equally consequential to children and adolescents. Second, paternal incarceration predicted children’s rule-breaking behaviors after we controlled for the consequences of fathers’ risky behaviors (i.e., alcohol and drug use); in doing so, were able to provide a more robust estimate of the effects of paternal incarceration net of other possibilities tinged to the imprisonment experience. Third, most key measures in the present study relied on multiple informants, minimizing self-report bias. Lastly, extant studies have either unspecified paternal incarceration or aggregated it with multiple types of adverse experiences during childhood (Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards, Koss and Marks1998). This practice is problematic because one form of father loss (i.e., paternal incarceration) may predict different outcomes than other forms (i.e., separation, divorce, or death). By including multiple forms of father loss in our analyses, we can more accurately reveal how paternal incarceration uniquely affects America’s children.

Implications

Our finding that children’s depressive symptoms emerged as a partial mediator provides interventionists with information on how to reduce the stress linked to paternal incarceration (e.g., via school-based support groups; Hairston, Reference Hairston2007). In line with General Strain Theory (Agnew, Reference Agnew2013), children’s psychological affect may be a means to deter their engagement in rule-breaking behaviors. As children progress into adolescence, they may have more opportunities to engage in maladaptive approaches (e.g., substance use) to cope with the trauma of paternal incarceration and its related circumstances (e.g., harsh/neglectful parenting, loss of income; Dwyer Emory, Reference Dwyer Emory2018; Pew Charitable Trusts, 2010; Turney, Reference Turney2014a). Therefore, identifying ways to reduce family tension and discomfort in the wake of paternal incarceration can protect children against depressive symptomology during childhood, which may in turn reduce their likelihood of engaging in rule-breaking behaviors as adolescents (Agnew, Reference Agnew2013). Practitioners should develop strategies that are sensitive to the needs of children and their families, including approaches that combat incarceration-based stigma (e.g., via counseling/support groups), promote family visits (e.g., affordable transportation, community-based residential facilities, and developmentally appropriate family visiting spaces), and reduce fathers’ likelihood of entry and reentry into the criminal justice system (e.g., facilitate rehabilitation through education, job training, and employment following release; Hairston, Reference Hairston2007).

Conclusion

Paternal incarceration may compromise children’s well-being and social adjustment. Stakeholders need to explore less punitive approaches to criminal rehabilitation to break the cycle in which institutional structures and policies contribute to the over-representation of socially disadvantaged communities in the criminal justice system (Agnew, Reference Agnew2013; National Research Council, 2014). In one study examining the consequences linked to alternatives to incarceration, scholars found that boys of fathers sentenced to a community probation program (compared to those who experienced paternal incarceration) were 15% less likely to be charged with a crime during early adulthood (Wildeman & Andersen, Reference Wildeman and Andersen2017). Future research should also explore how social resources (e.g., social workers, school psychologists) can help children manage the stress and the uncertainty of paternal incarceration. Research regarding these resources can inform policymakers’ and practitioners’ efforts to effectively promote positive developmental competencies among children who experience parental incarceration.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001693

Funding statement

This research was supported by Grant 202100287 from the Spencer Foundation to Dr. Juan Del Toro.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

References

Achenbach, T., & Rescorla, L. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont. Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families, 1617.Google Scholar
Agnew, R. (2013). When criminal coping is likely: An extension of general strain theory. Deviant Behavior, 34(8), 653670. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2013.766529 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychological Association. (2006). Report of the APA Task Force on Socioeconomic Status. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Arditti, J. A. (2016). A family stress-proximal process model for understanding the effects of parental incarceration on children and their families. Couple and Family Psychology: Research and Practice, 5(2), 6588. https://doi.org/10.1037/cfp0000058 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baraldi, A. N., & Enders, C. K. (2010). An introduction to modern missing data analyses. Journal of School Psychology, 48(1), 537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2009.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106(1), 5973. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.1.59 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boch, S. J., Warren, B. J., & Ford, J. L. (2019). Attention, externalizing, and internalizing problems of youth exposed to parental incarceration. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 40(6), 466475. https://doi.org/10.1080/01612840.2019.1565872 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christian, J., & Thomas, S. S. (2009). Examining the intersections of race, gender, and mass imprisonment. Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal Justice, 7(1), 6984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Toro, J., Fine, A., Wang, M.-T., Thomas, A., Schneper, L. M., Mitchell, C., Mincy, R. B., McLanahan, S., & Notterman, D. A. (2021). The longitudinal associations between paternal incarceration and family well-being: Implications for ethnic/racial disparities in health. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2021.08.005 Google ScholarPubMed
Del Toro, J., Lloyd, T., Buchanan, K. S., Robins, S. J., Bencharit, L. Z., Smiedt, M. G., Reddy, K. S., Pouget, E. R., Kerrison, E. M., & Goff, P. A. (2019). The criminogenic and psychological effects of police stops on adolescent black and Latino boys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(17), 82618268. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1808976116 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., Sears, R. R., Yale University., Institute of Human Relations. (1939). Frustration and aggression. Pub. for the Institute of human relations by Yale University Press ; H. Milford, Oxford University Press; /z-wcorg/. http://search.ebscohost.com/direct.asp?db=pzh&jid=200416227&scope=site CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer Emory, A. (2018). Explaining the consequences of paternal incarceration for children’s behavioral problems. Family Relations, 67(2), 302319. https://doi.org/10.1111/fare.12301 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eberhardt, J. L. (2019). Biased: Uncovering the hidden prejudice that shapes what we see, think, and do. Viking.Google Scholar
Fagan, J., & Tyler, T. R. (2005). Legal socialization of children and adolescents. Social Justice Research, 18(3), 217242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-005-6823-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., Koss, M. P., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geller, A. (2013). Paternal incarceration and father–child contact in fragile families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 75(5), 12881303. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12056 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geller, A., Cooper, C. E., Garfinkel, I., Schwartz-Soicher, O., & Mincy, R. B. (2012). Beyond absenteeism: Father incarceration and child development. Demography, 49(1), 4976. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-011-0081-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geller, A., Garfinkel, I., & Western, B. (2011). Paternal incarceration and support for children in Fragile Families. Demography, 48(1), 2547. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-010-0009-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gifford, E. J., Eldred Kozecke, L., Golonka, M., Hill, S. N., Costello, E. J., Shanahan, L., & Copeland, W. E. (2019). Association of parental incarceration with psychiatric and functional outcomes of young adults. JAMA Network Open, 2(8), e1910005. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.10005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, S. (2016). Commentary: The role of race/ethnicity in a developmental science of equity and justice. Child Development, 87(5), 14931504. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12602 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hairston, C. F. (2007). Focus on children with incarcerated parents: An overview of the research literature. aecf.org/m/resourcedoc/aecf-FocusonChildrenwith_ncarceratedParentsOverviewofLiterature-2007.pdf#page=3 Google Scholar
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haskins, A. R. (2014). Unintended consequences: Effects of paternal incarceration on child school readiness and later special education placement. Sociological Science, 1, 141158. https://doi.org/10.15195/v1.a11 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haskins, A. R. (2015). Paternal incarceration and child-reported behavioral functioning at age 9. Social Science Research, 52, 1833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2015.01.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haskins, A. R. (2017). Paternal incarceration and children’s schooling contexts: Intersecting inequalities of educational opportunity. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 674(1), 134162. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716217732011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, D. B., Testa, A., Semenza, D. C., & Vaughn, M. G. (2021). Parental incarceration, child adversity, and child health: A strategic comparison approach. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(7), 3384. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18073384 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobsen, W. C. (2019). The intergenerational stability of punishment: Paternal incarceration and suspension or expulsion in elementary school. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 56(5), 651693. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022427819829794 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, M. S., & Pierce, H. (2020). Early exposure to adverse childhood experiences and youth delinquent behavior in Fragile Families. Youth & Society. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118x20908759 Google Scholar
Kalu, S. R., Menon, S. E., & Quinn, C. R. (2020). The relationship between externalizing behavior and school and familial attachments among girls from diverse backgrounds. Children and Youth Services Review, 116, 105170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105170 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. A. (1975). Cross-lagged panel correlation: A test for spuriousness. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 887903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.82.6.887 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1979). Cross-lagged panel correlation: Practice and promise. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64(4), 372379. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.64.4.372 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kjellstrand, J., Yu, G., Eddy, J. M., & Martinez, C. R. (2018). Children of incarcerated parents: Developmental trajectories of externalizing behavior across adolescence. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 45(11), 17421761. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854818785400 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laub, J. H. (2014). Understanding inequality and the justice system response: Charting a new way forward . William T. Grant Foundation.Google Scholar
Lee, H., & Wildeman, C. (2021). Assessing mass incarceration’s effects on families. Science, 374(6565), 277281. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abj7777 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, R. D., Fang, X., & Luo, F. (2013). The impact of parental incarceration on the physical and mental health of young adults. Pediatrics, 131(4), e1188e1195. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-0627 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 83104. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. (1990). Manual for the self-description questionnaire-I. University of Western Sydney, Campbelltown, NSW, Australia.Google Scholar
Massey, D. S., & Denton, N. A. (1989). Hypersegregation in U.S. metropolitan areas: Black and Hispanic segregation along five dimensions. Demography, 26(3), 373391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massey, D. S., & Denton, N. A. (1993). American Apartheid: Segregation and The Making of the Underclass. http://hdl.handle.net/2027/heb.31525.0001.001 Google Scholar
Maumary-Gremaud, A. (2000). Things that you have done: Fast track project technical report. Durham, NC: Fast Track Data Center, Conduct Problems Preventions Research Group.Google Scholar
McCauley, E. (2020). Beyond the classroom: The intergenerational effect of incarceration on children’s academic and nonacademic school-related outcomes in high school. Socius, 6, 2378023120915369. https://doi.org/10.1177/2378023120915369 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCauley, E. (2021). The role of stress and absence: How household member incarceration is associated with risky sexual health behaviors. Social Science & Medicine, 272, 113718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113718 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, B. A., Johnson, W. E., Cryer-Coupet, Q. R., & Mincy, R. B. (2019). Examining the longitudinal effects of paternal incarceration and coparenting relationships on sons’ educational outcomes: A mediation analysis. Children and Youth Services Review, 100, 362375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.03.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muentner, L., Kapoor, A., Weymouth, L., & Poehlmann-Tynan, J. (2021). Getting under the skin: Physiological stress and witnessing paternal arrest in young children with incarcerated fathers. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(5), 15681582. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22113 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (2005). Parental imprisonment: Effects on boys’ antisocial behaviour and delinquency through the life-course. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 46(12), 12691278. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01433.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, J., Farrington, D. P., & Sekol, I. (2012). Children’s antisocial behavior, mental health, drug use, and educational performance after parental incarceration: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 175210. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026407 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, J., Loeber, R., & Pardini, D. (2012). Parental involvement in the criminal justice system and the development of youth theft, marijuana use, depression, and poor academic performance. Criminology, 50, 255302. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00257.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus user’s guide. (5th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
National Research Council. (2014). The growth of incarceration in the United States: Exploring causes and consequences. The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/18613 Google Scholar
Pearlin, L. I. (1989). The sociological study of stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 30(3), 241256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearlin, L. I. (2010). The life course and the stress process: Some conceptual comparisons. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65B(2), 207215. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp106 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pew Charitable Trusts. (2010). Collateral costs: Incarceration’s effect on economic mobility. The Pew Charitable Trusts.Google Scholar
Poehlmann, J. (2005). Representations of attachment relationships in children of incarcerated mothers. Child Development, 76(3), 679696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, L. C. (2014). Incarceration and post-release health behavior. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 55(2), 234249. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022146514531438 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, L. C., & King, R. D. (2015). Absent fathers or absent variables? A new look at paternal incarceration and delinquency. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 52(3), 414443. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022427814552080 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rattan, A., Levine, C. S., Dweck, C. S., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2012). Race and the fragility of the legal distinction between juveniles and adults. PLOS ONE, 7(5), e36680. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036680 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichman, N. E., Teitler, J. O., Garfinkel, I., & McLanahan, S. S. (2001). Fragile families: Sample and design. Children and Youth Services Review, 23, 303326. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0190-7409(01)00141-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruhland, E. L., Davis, L., Atella, J., & Shlafer, R. J. (2020). Externalizing behavior among youth with a current or formerly incarcerated parent. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 64(1), 321. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X19855317 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, E. H. (1949). Measurement of diversity. Nature, 163, 688. https://doi.org/10.1038/163688a0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28(1), 78106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Testa, A., & Jackson, D. B. (2021). Parental incarceration and school readiness: Findings from the 2016 to 2018 National Survey of Children’s Health. Academic Pediatrics, 21(3), 534541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2020.08.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Testa, A., Jackson, D. B., Vaughn, M. G., & Bello, J. K. (2020). Incarceration as a unique social stressor during pregnancy: Implications for maternal and newborn health. Social Science & Medicine, 246, 112777. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112777 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turney, K. (2014a). The consequences of paternal incarceration for maternal neglect and harsh parenting. Social Forces, 92(4), 16071636. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/sot160 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turney, K. (2014b). Stress proliferation across generations? Examining the relationship between parental incarceration and childhood health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 55(3), 302319. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022146514544173 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turney, K. (2017). Unmet health care needs among children exposed to parental incarceration. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 21(5), 11941202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-2219-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turney, K. (2021). Chains of adversity: The time-varying consequences of paternal incarceration for adolescent behavior. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-020-09485-3 Google Scholar
Turney, K., & Haskins, A. R. (2014). Falling behind? Children’s early grade retention after paternal incarceration. Sociology of Education, 87(4), 241258. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038040714547086 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turney, K., & Haskins, A. R. (2019). Parental incarceration and children’s well-being: Findings from the Fragile Families and Child Well-being Study. In Eddy, J. M. & Poehlmann-Tynan, J. (Eds.), Handbook on children with incarcerated parents: Research, policy, and practice (pp. 5364). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-16707-3_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turney, K., & Wildeman, C. (2013). Redefining relationships: Explaining the countervailing consequences of paternal incarceration for parenting. American Sociological Review, 78(6), 949979. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122413505589 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wakefield, S., & Wildeman, C. (2011). Mass imprisonment and racial disparities in childhood behavioral problems. Criminology & Public Policy, 3, 793. British Library Document Supply Centre Inside Serials & Conference Proceedings.Google Scholar
Widdowson, A. O., Jacobsen, W. C., Siennick, S. E., & Warren, P. Y. (2020). Together despite the odds: Explaining racial and ethnic heterogeneity in union dissolution after incarceration. Criminology, 58(1), 129155. https://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12232 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wildeman, C. (2010). Paternal incarceration and children’s physically aggressive behaviors: Evidence from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study. Social Forces, 89(1), 285309. https://doi.org/10.1353/sof.2010.0055 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wildeman, C. (2020). The intergenerational transmission of criminal justice contact. Annual Review of Criminology, 3(1), 217244. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-criminol-011419-041519 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wildeman, C., & Andersen, S. H. (2017). Paternal incarceration and children’s risk of being charged by early adulthood: Evidence from a Danish policy shock. Criminology, 55(1), 3258. https://doi.org/10.1111/1745-9125.12124 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wildeman, C., Schnittker, J., & Turney, K. (2012). Despair by association? The mental health of mothers with children by recently incarcerated fathers. American Sociological Review, 77(2), 216243. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122411436234 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of demographic and key study measures for the analytic sample

Figure 1

Figure 1. A visual depiction of the random intercept crosslagged panel model from children’s ages 5 to 15. Notes: Solid lines indicate hypothesized significant paths; dashed lines indicate our hypothesized nonsignificant paths. The longitudinal direct effects between paternal incarceration and children’s rule-breaking behaviors are suppressed to ease visual representation.

Figure 2

Table 2. Zero-order bivariate correlations among key study variables (n = 4,327)

Figure 3

Table 3. Unstandardized coefficients from a random intercept crosslagged panel model (n = 4,327) and after controlling for covariates (suppressed to ease visual representation)

Supplementary material: File

Del Toro et al. supplementary material

Del Toro et al. supplementary material
Download Del Toro et al. supplementary material(File)
File 55.9 KB