Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T08:43:03.151Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Intentional reasoning: A developmental study of behaviorally aggressive and normal boys

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Anne McKeough*
Affiliation:
The University of Calgary
Tim Yates
Affiliation:
Alberta Children's Hospital
Anthony Marini
Affiliation:
The University of Calgary
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Anne McKeough, Department of Educational Psychology, The University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive NW, Calgary, Alberta, Canada T2N 1N4.

Abstract

The purpose of this work was to investigate the way in which boys, ages 6, 8, and 10 years, who are behaviorally disturbed, understand motives behind human behavior, compared to normally functioning peers. Four tasks were administered that differed in surface features but that shared an underlying conceptual structure. A structural analysis of response protocols was undertaken to assess the level of cognitive complexity of their productions. Age-appropriate performance required varying degrees of intentional understanding (i.e., the reciprocal causal relations between action and mental states such as feelings and desires). The results of this analysis supported our predictions that behaviorally disturbed children use developmentally naive reasoning in the domain of conflict resolution, compared with their normal peers. Additionally, a thematic analysis of the content of responses was performed. The results of this analysis showed that the two groups' reasoning also differed qualitatively, in that the aggressive boys showed greater evidence of socially maladaptive thought, whereas the comparison group's performance was largely adaptive. We propose that early-formed primitive defense mechanisms may interfere with the aggressive group's construction of prosocial mental models of the social world. The results suggest that this line of research, which integrates developmental and psychoanalytic theory, has the potential to offer insight into the mechanisms underlying behavioral aggression.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, C., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment assessed in the Strange Situation and at home. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Astington, J., Olson, D., & Harris, P. (Eds.). (1988). The child's developing theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bear, G. C., & Richard, H. C. (1981). Moral reasoning and conduct problems in the classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 644670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blishen, B. R., Carroll, W. K., & Moore, K. (1987). The 1981 socioeconomic index for occupations in Canada. Canadian Review of Sociology and Anthropology, 24, 465488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1975). Separation: Anxiety and anger. London: Pelican Press.Google Scholar
Bruchkowsky, M. (1992). The development of empathic understanding in early and middle childhood. In Case, R. (Ed.), The mind's staircase: Intentional reasoning Stages in the development of human intelligence (pp. 153170). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Campagna, A. F., & Harter, S. (1975). Moral judgement in sociopathic and normal children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 199205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Case, R. (1988). The whole child: Toward an integrated view of young children's cognitive, social, and emotional development. In Pellegrini, A. (Ed.), Psychological bases for early education (pp. 155184). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Case, R. (1991a, 04). The role of primitive defenses in the regulation and representation of early attachment relations. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Case, R. (1991b). Stages in the development of the young child's first sense of self. Developmental Review, 11, 210230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Case, R. (Ed.). (1992). The mind's staircase: Stages in the development of human intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Case, R. (1993, 07). Conceptualizing the notion of stages and structures in children's development. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the International Society of Behavioural Development,Recife, Brazil.Google Scholar
Case, R., Hayward, S., Lewis, M. D., & Hurst, P. (1988). Toward a neo-Piagetian theory of cognitive and emotional development. Developmental Review, 8, 151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Case, R., & McKeough, A. (1990). Schooling and the development of central conceptual knowledge structures: An example from the domain of children's narrative. International Journal of Education, 13(8), 835855.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & White, J. (1989). Emotional development and affective disorders. In Damon, W. (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 177198). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.Google Scholar
Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1982). The development of self-understanding from infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 53, 841864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior. Child Development, 51, 162170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A. (1985). Attributional bias in aggressive children. Advances in Cognitive-Behavioral Research and Therapy, 4, 73110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A., & Newman, J. P. (1981). Biased decision-making processes in aggressive boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 90(4), 375379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, P. L. (1982). Empathy: A factor in antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 385395.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Lamborn, S. (1989). Mechanisms of variation in developmental levels: Cognitive and emotional development during adolescence. In de Ribaupierre, A. (Ed.), Transition mechanisms in child development (pp. 3367). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Pipp, S. L. (1984). Development of the structures of unconscious thought. In Bowers, K. & Meichenbaum, D. (Eds.), The unconscious re-considered (pp. 88148). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W., Shaver, P. R., & Carnochan, P. (1990). How emotions develop and how they organize development. Cognition and Emotion, 4(2), 81127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg-Reitman, J. (1992). Young girls' conception of their mother's role: A neo-structural analysis. In Case, R. (Ed.), The mind's staircase: Stages in the development of human intelligence (pp. 135152). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Griffin, S. (1992). Young children's awareness of their inner world: A neo-structural analysis of the development of interpersonal intelligence. In Case, R. (Ed.), The mind's staircase: Stages in the development of human intelligence (pp. 189206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Halford, G. (1982). The development of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hersov, L., Berger, M., & Shaffer, D. (1978). Aggressive and antisocial behaviour in childhood and adolescence. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Howard, M. (1993, 06). The meaning of behaviourally-aggressive boys'stories. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society,Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Jurkovic, G. J., & Prentice, N. M. (1977). Relation of moral and cognitive development to dimensions of juvenile delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 414420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kauffman, J. M. (1977). Characteristics of children's behaviour disorders. Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar
Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (1993). Emotion-cognition interactions in early infant development. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 145170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lytton, H. (1990). Child and parent effects on boys' conduct disorder: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology, 26(5), 683697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In Bretherton, I. & Walters, E. (Eds.), Growing points in attachment and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (Serial No. 209), 66–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKeough, A. (1992). Testing for the presence of a central social structure: Use of the transfer paradigm. In Case, R. (Ed.), The mind's staircase: Stages in the development of human intelligence (pp. 207225). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Milich, R., & Dodge, K. A. (1984). Social information processing in child psychiatric populations. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 171189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nasby, W., Hayden, B., & De Paulo, B. M. (1980). Attributional bias among aggressive boys to interpret unambiguous social stimulus as displays of hostility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 495–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1978). How children represent knowledge of their world in and out of language: A prelimi-nary report. In Siegler, R. (Ed.), Children's thinking: What develops? (pp. 255274). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1981). Social cognition in a script framework. In Flavell, J. H. & Ross, L. (Eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures (pp. 97118). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Noam, G. G. (1988). The theory of biography and transformation: Foundation for clinical-developmental therapy. In Shirk, S. (Ed.), Cognitive development in child psychotherapy (pp. 273317). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. P., & Herman, S. (1982). Behavioral disordered children's conceptions of moral, conventional, and personal issues. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 411426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, D. (1988). On the origins of beliefs and other intentional states in children. In Astington, J., Olson, D., & Harris, P. (Eds.), The child's developing theory of mind (pp. 115). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paulauskas, F. L., & Campbell, S. B. (1979). Social Perspective-taking and teacher ratings of peer interaction in hyperactive boys. Journal of Adolescent and Child Psychology, 7(4), 483493.Google ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. (1975). Early influences on development: Factor or fancy? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 263294.Google Scholar
Saltier, J. M. (1988). Assessment of children's intelligence and special abilities (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Selman, R. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Selman, R., Jacquette, D., & Lavin, D. (1977). Interpersonal awareness in children: Toward an integration of developmental and clinical child psychology. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 47(1), 264274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, M. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1983). Attributional bias in aggressive adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 1, 312321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stott, D. H., & Marston, N. C. (1970). Bristol social adjustment guide. San Diego, CA: Educational & Industrial Testing Service.Google Scholar
Waterman, J., Sobesky, W., Silvern, L., Aoki, B., & McCauley, M. (1981). Social perspective taking in emotionally disturbed, learning disabled, and normal children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 9, 133148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Zahn-Waxier, C., Cole, P., Lehman, K., & Junker, M. (1991). Understanding of emotions: Relations to social competence. Representations of interpersonal conflict and distress in children at risk for behavior problems. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,Seattle, WA.Google Scholar