Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T18:40:02.386Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emergence and evolution of developmental resilience science over half a century

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 March 2024

Ann S. Masten*
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota Twin Cities, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This reflection on the history and future of developmental resilience science (DRS) highlights its co-emergence with developmental psychopathology (DP), as well as the roles of this journal and its founding editor, Dante Cicchetti, in the evolution of these intertwined domains of scholarship. A remarkable constellation of scholars at the University of Minnesota shaped the course of both conceptual frameworks and their dissemination. I describe fundamental assumptions common to DP and DRS frameworks that reflect their common roots and the pervasive influence of systems theory on developmental science. I describe four waves of DRS and key principles of DRS at the present time. In conclusion, I consider the possibility that a fifth wave of DRS is emerging with a focus on understanding patterns of multisystem, multilevel processes of resilience and their implications for interventions in the context of interacting, interdependent, and complex adaptive systems. I close this commentary with questions for future research and a hopeful outlook on the future of human resilience.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Developmental psychopathology (DP) and developmental resilience science (DRS) co-emerged during the 1970s (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990). A remarkable network of scholars concentrated at the University of Minnesota (UMN) influenced many of the core ideas common to these interrelated domains of developmental science. These ideas spread widely over the ensuing decades through the activities of their students and colleagues. The proximity of two distinct departments of psychology at UMN played a central role, with faculty and students in the Department of Psychology (in Elliott Hall) and the Institute of Child Development (ICD, next door) interacting in courses, colloquia, and collaborative research, as well as at parties and in the cafeteria located between the two buildings (in Shevlin Hall). Many students training in clinical and developmental psychology during the 1970s took courses taught by Thomas Bouchard, James Butcher, Norman Garmezy, Irving Gottesman, David Lykken, Paul Meehl, and Auke Tellegen in Psychology as well as William Charlesworth, Andrew Collins, Willard Hartup, and Alan Sroufe in ICD, among others. Byron Egeland and Richard Weinberg, initially faculty in Elliott Hall, moved to ICD as the two departments launched a joint training program in clinical child psychology. This specialty track, more formally known as the Developmental Psychopathology and Clinical Science program, combined training in clinical psychology and developmental science.

The network that gave momentum to DP as well as DRS included close colleagues of UMN faculty at other universities, such as Jeanne and Jack Block, Mavis Hetherington, Gerald Patterson, Michael Rutter, Arnold Sameroff, and Edward Zigler. This network of mentors trained a host of students in DP and DRS, both at UMN (e.g., Thomas Achenbach, Dante Cicchetti, Judy Garber, Margaret O’Dougherty Wright) and elsewhere, such as Yale (e.g., Suniya Luthar). These students, in turn, disseminated developmental models of psychopathology and resilience through their own scholarship and mentoring over the years, resulting in a new generation of scholars with developmental perspectives on psychopathology and resilience. Many of these descendants are represented in this special issue honoring the seminal role of Dante Cicchetti in establishing DP as a distinct and cohesive, multidisciplinary developmental framework for research on pathways toward and away from psychopathology. Cicchetti exemplifies the extraordinary legacy of UMN graduate training at UMN during the 1970s, mentored by faculty in clinical psychology, including Paul Meehl and Norman Garmezy, as well as in developmental psychology, including Alan Sroufe. Cicchetti’s integrative vision of DP would have transformative consequences for multiple domains of science, including resilience research.

Under Cicchetti’s leadership, a special issue he edited of Child Development set the early course for DP (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984). In 1987, Cicchetti held the first in a series of symposia on DP in Rochester, New York, while he was on the faculty at the University of Rochester. The Rochester Symposia on DP (modeled on the Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology), led to a series of edited books based on this annual conference (see Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti and Cicchetti1989). The growth and influence of DP was further spurred by a series of literally and figuratively hefty, multivolume books entitled Developmental Psychopathology that Cicchetti edited in collaboration with child psychiatrist Donald Cohen, the renowned Director of the Yale Child Study Center, from 1995 until Cohen’s untimely death. The most recently edition of this compendium includes four large volumes, solo edited by Cicchetti (Reference Cicchetti2016).

One of the core features of DP as championed by Cicchetti was the attention to positive pathways and adaptive processes as well as pathways to psychopathology and maladaptive functioning. For the 1984 special issue of Child Development, Cicchetti invited a paper focused on resilience (Garmezy et al., Reference Garmezy, Masten and Tellegen1984). For the first Rochester Symposium in 1987 on DP, he included me as a speaker and I subsequently wrote a chapter on resilience (Masten, Reference Masten and Cicchetti1989) for the ensuing first volume stemming from that initial symposium. All three of the editions of Developmental Psychopathology edited by Cicchetti included the topic of resilience (Luthar, Reference Luthar, Cicchetti and Cohen2006; Masten & Coatsworth, Reference Masten, Coatsworth, Cicchetti and Cohen1995; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016).

Development and Psychopathology, the journal Cicchetti founded in 1989, has continued to play a vital role in the ongoing evolution of DRS, with numerous articles and special issues highlighting theory and research on DRS. The most cited article to date in the journal is a review of the first generation of resilience research (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990). Additionally, there have been three important special issues on resilience over the past three decades: “Milestones in the Development of Resilience” (Cicchetti & Garmezy, Reference Cicchetti and Garmezy1993), “A Multilevel Approach to Resilience” (Cicchetti & Curtis, Reference Cicchetti and Curtis2007); and most recently, “Resilience in Development: Pathways to Multisystem Integration” (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Tyrell and Cicchetti2023). As a result of this journal and Cicchetti’s other influential publications, Cicchetti’s influence in the advancement and dissemination of DRS cannot be overestimated.

Shared assumptions and concepts of DP and DRS

Given their closely intertwined origins, it is not surprising that DP and DRS share fundamental assumptions (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016). These ideas reflect the profound influence of developmental systems theory as well as a growing body of research with implications for both domains, such as studies of gene–environment interplay, developmental neurobiology, and the effects of context and trauma on development (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerner2006; Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti, Cicchetti and Cohen2006, Reference Cicchetti2013; Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009; Cummings & Valentino, Reference Cummings, Valentino, Overton, Molenaar and Lerner2015; Egeland et al., Reference Egeland, Carlson and Sroufe1993; Garmezy & Rutter, Reference Garmezy and Rutter1983; Gottesman, Reference Gottesman and Pick1974; Gottlieb, Reference Gottlieb2007; Lerner, Reference Lerner, Lerner, Damon and Lerner2006; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010; Masten, Reference Masten2007; Masten & Kalstabakken, Reference Masten, Kalstabakken, Butcher and Kendall2018; Meaney, Reference Meaney2010; Sameroff, Reference Sameroff2010; Overton, Reference Overton2013; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984; Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2005; Zelazo, Reference Zelazo and Zelazo2013).

Examples of guiding themes shared by DP and DRS, likely due to their common roots and interactions, include the following (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016; Masten & Kalstabakken, Reference Masten, Kalstabakken, Butcher and Kendall2018).

  • Understanding variations and change in adaptive behavior over the life course requires a developmental perspective.

  • Living systems are self-organizing with emergent properties that can be surprising or unpredictable from lower levels of analysis.

  • Development is shaped by many interacting systems within and around the organism and these interactions are multidirectional.

  • Behavior emerging from multisystem and multilevel interactions over time can be described as a pathway.

  • As a result of ongoing multisystem interactions and experiences, developmental pathways can converge from disparate origins (equifinality) or diverge from similar origins (multifinality), resulting in many pathways toward or away from positive adaptation or psychopathology.

  • Development is probabilistic rather than deterministic.

  • Developmental timing of experiences can have differential effects on development.

  • Plasticity of individuals with regard to the effects of experiences varies over the course of development.

  • Judgments about the quality of adaptation are typically based on expectations about human development and behavior (e.g., developmental tasks or behavioral norms) that are influenced by context, culture, and history, and thus vary over time and context.

  • Knowledge of positive development and psychopathology are mutually informative for theory, prevention, and treatment.

Four waves of developmental resilience science

Research on resilience in human development began in earnest during the 1970s. When scholars reflected on the history of DRS after three or more decades of research, they delineated four distinct waves or phases of research (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Narayan, Wright, Goldstein and Brooks2023; Masten, Reference Masten2021; Wright et al.., Reference Wright, Masten, Narayan, Goldstein and Brooks2013). The first wave was largely descriptive, as investigators sought to identify the correlates and predictors associated with better adaptation following exposure to significant adversity or high-risk circumstances. This wave underscored the idea that operationalizing the study of resilience required two basic judgments and corresponding measurement strategies for assessing threats and outcomes in human adaptation and development: (1) exposure to risks or challenges that threatened adjustment or development and (2) criteria for judging how well the person was doing in the aftermath of this risk or adversity exposure (Luthar et al., Reference Luthar, Cicchetti and Becker2000; Masten & Coatsworth, Reference Masten and Coatsworth1998; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Narayan, Wright, Goldstein and Brooks2023). The key aim of early studies was to identify predictors of good or relatively better adaptation among groups of individuals exposed to significant threats or risk factors known to pose potential harm to function or development –in other words, to answer a question motivating much of this research: What makes a difference? Scholars hoped to identify key factors and processes associated with better adaptation to adversity, with the ultimate translational objective of fostering adaptive responses to adverse life experiences and informing interventions to facilitate resilience.

Eventually, the descriptive literature identified two major kinds of resilience factors associated with good or better outcomes in the context of risk, classified by the nature of their effects (Masten, Reference Masten2001). Promotive factors (Sameroff, Reference Sameroff2000) – also called compensatory factors (Garmezy et al., Reference Garmezy, Masten and Tellegen1984) or simply assets – referred to predictors showing generally positive effects on adjustment: in statistical terms, positive main effects on outcome criteria. Protective factors referred to predictors showing mitigating or buffering effects, reducing negative effects associated with risk indicators on the adjustment outcome variables of interest: in statistical terms, moderating effects on risk (consistent with greater effects when risk is high, over and above any main effects that might be observed at low as well as high-risk conditions). It was recognized early in DRS that the same variable or attribute (of the person or their environs) could have varying effects depending on the adjustment criteria, timing, or circumstances (Rutter, Reference Rutter1987). However, elucidating these variations proved to be complex and important in the ensuing years. Of particular note is the concept of differential susceptibility or sensitivity to experience (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn2011), where the effects of a particular attribute appear to depend on whether the environmental conditions are advantageous or adverse, with observable effects of the same attribute varying from negative to positive depending on the favorability of the environment. Sensitive people, who appear to be particularly vulnerable to adversity, may also be more responsive to favorable experiences and possibly also to interventions (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2015).

Diverse research on resilience has suggested a consistent “short list” of widely observed resilience factors associated with better adapting in children and youth who were faced with adversity, presumably reflecting fundamental human adaptive systems and processes that arose through processes of biological and cultural evolution (Masten, Reference Masten2001, Reference Masten2007). Over the years, considerable support has accrued for many of these resilience factors, as reported in narrative reviews (e.g., Doom et al., Reference Doom, Deer, Dieujuste, Han, Rivera and Scott2023; Mesman et al., Reference Mesman, Vreeker and Hillegers2021; Ungar & Theron, 2022), systematic reviews (e.g., Gartland et al., Reference Gartland, Riggs, Muyeen, Giallo, Afifi, MacMillan, Herrman, Bulford and Brown2019; Meng et al., Reference Meng, Fleury, Xiang, Li and D’Arcy2018;) and many other reports focused on specific populations or situations (e.g., Arakelyan & Ager, Reference Arakelyan and Ager2021; Marquez et al., Reference Marquez, Francis-Hew and Humphrey2023; Werner, Reference Werner1993, Reference Werner2012). Evidence continues to accumulate in support of the idea that human resilience depends substantially on ordinary human adaptive systems that often were implicated in early descriptive studies. Over time, however, the focus of research shifted from the “what questions” of descriptive identification to “how questions” in a search for deeper understanding of resilience processes (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990, Reference Masten, Narayan, Wright, Goldstein and Brooks2023). Subsequently, in later phases of resilience science, investigators noted compelling similarities in resilience factors identified in systems studied at different levels of analysis (e.g., family, community), described further below.

When the focus of resilience studies shifted from what to how, the second wave of DRS began. This wave of theory and research emphasized resilience as a complex process (or set of processes) and also aligned DRS more closely with a broader transformation in developmental science toward systems thinking (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Narayan, Wright, Goldstein and Brooks2023). The infusion of systems theory in studies of human development transformed general developmental science and DP as well as DRS. The shared assumptions of DP and DRS listed above reflect this transformation. Efforts to investigate processes of adaptation in the wake of adversity also focused attention on the need for better methods to measure dynamic change over time. Interest surged in concepts focused on dynamic patterns of change over time, such as pathways and trajectories (e.g., Betancourt & Khan, Reference Betancourt and Khan2008; Bonanno, Reference Bonanno2004; Masten & Powell, Reference Masten, Powell and Luthar2003; Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2005), equifinality and multifinality (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996), and developmental cascades (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010). The second wave also brought greater attention to the roles of context and culture in resilience processes (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Narayan, Wright, Goldstein and Brooks2023; Panter-Brick, Reference Panter-Brick, Theron, Liebenberg and Ungar2015; Ungar, Reference Ungar2008; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Masten, Narayan, Goldstein and Brooks2013).

In the search for better understanding of resilience processes, both promotive and protective effects of resilience factors observed in longitudinal studies held special interest as potential clues to how resilience might work. Potentially malleable resilience factors were particularly important to investigators who had intervention in mind, with the goal of fostering better adaptation in the aftermath of adversity, or prevention in the form of building protection against future adversity. The third wave of DRS began when investigators started to test the possibilities of fostering resilience through intervention studies aiming to mitigate risk, enhance protection, or favorably alter mediators of risk/adversity in the lives of children and their families.

From the outset of DRS, a central goal of investigators was gaining knowledge to guide practice and policy (Masten, Reference Masten2011). This was not surprising, given that many of the pioneers who led the way were trained in clinical fields, including Garmezy (clinical psychology) and Rutter (psychiatry). As knowledge accumulated about the processes that appeared to counter or mitigate the risks associated with adversity, there was a surge in intervention research that could simultaneously test the causal models emerging from the first two waves of DRS while also offering help to children or their families potentially affected by adverse experiences. Intervention studies of resilience represent the third wave of DRS, with randomized controlled trials (RCTs) and quasi-experimental designs offering the most compelling tests of resilience theory about processes of resilience.

These studies generally reflect three basic strategies of intervention suggested by this author and others based on resilience frameworks, targeting risk, promotive, or protective processes (Masten, Reference Masten2011). Some interventions focus on lowering risk or adversity exposure, for example by preventing maltreatment (e.g., van IJzendoorn et al., Reference van IJzendoorn, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Coughlan and Reijman2020). Others focus on adding supportive or promotive resources to the lives of children, such as cash transfers to impoverished families with children (e.g., Troller-Renfree et al., Reference Troller-Renfree, Costanzo, Duncan, Magnuson, Gennetian, Yoshikawa, Halpern-Meekin, Fox and Noble2022). During the COVID-19 pandemic, many schools as well as local, state, and federal government agencies acted to provide additional resources for children to promote health and learning, including food and technology needed for distance learning. Long before the pandemic, schools played a central role in designing and testing resilience-based interventions of many kinds and also served as a setting for interventions, perhaps because schools are not only accessible to most children and families, but also because schools are perceived as a normal and safe environment by families and communities alike. Dray and colleagues (Reference Dray, Bowman, Campbell, Freund, Wolfenden, Hodder, McElwaine, Tremain, Bartlem, Bailey, Small, Palazzi, Oldmeadow and Wiggers2017) reviewed the literature on universal resilience-focused interventions targeting mental health in children and youth in school settings.

Interventions focused on protective processes for children and youth often have targeted parenting or parent–child relationships (Sandler et al., Reference Sandler, Ingram, Wolchik, Tein and Winslow2015; Toth et al., Reference Toth, Gravener-Davis, Guild and Cicchetti2013) and youth mentoring (Raposa et al., Reference Raposa, Rhodes, Stams, Card, Burton, Schwartz, Sykes, Kanchewa, Kupersmidt and Hussain2019). Robust evidence, for example, has accumulated in support of interventions that enhance the quality of caregiving and parenting skills in the families of children at risk due to adverse experiences or toxic environments, supporting basic research findings on the protective effects of effective and supportive family care against risks posed by various forms of adversity in childhood (Doty et al., Reference Doty, Davis and Arditti2017; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016; Masten & Palmer, Reference Masten, Palmer and Bornstein2019). Cicchetti, Toth, and collaborators at the Mt. Hope Family Center, for example, have conducted rigorous RCTs on preventive interventions to test the effects of infant–parent psychotherapy and other parenting interventions on secure attachment relationships in maltreating families and also in families where the mother has a diagnosis of Major Depressive Disorder (e.g., Cicchetti et al., Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch and Toth2006; Toth et al., Reference Toth, Rogosch, Manly and Cicchetti2006). Fisher and colleagues (Reference Fisher, Stoolmiller, Gunnar and Burraston2007) demonstrated through an RCT that a family-based therapeutic foster care intervention (in comparison with routine foster care) showed protective effects against dysregulated diurnal cortisol patterns of activity associated with stress. Sandler, Wolchik, and their colleagues have demonstrated in another RCT (with multiple follow-ups) the short- and long-term effects of a preventive parenting intervention for bereaved families (loss of a parent), providing evidence of intergenerational effects on parenting attitudes as well as shorter-term effects on parenting and youth behavior (Rhodes et al., Reference Rhodes, Wolchik, Uhlman, O’Hara, Sandler, Tein and Porter2023). Such RCTs offer compelling evidence that parent–child relationships central to theories of psychopathology and resilience in DP and DRS are malleable, with potentially cascading effects (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016).

In addition to supporting specific hypotheses about resilience, intervention research also has suggested that it is effective to combine multiple approaches, coordinated (stacked or aligned) to create synergy (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2013; Hostinar & Miller, Reference Hostinar and Miller2019; Masten, Reference Masten2011, Reference Masten2014; Wyman et al., Reference Wyman, Sandler, Wolchik, Nelson, Cicchetti, Rappaport, Sandler and Weissberg2000; Yoshikawa, Reference Yoshikawa1994). The humanitarian sector of international aid and development has played a leading role in demonstrating the power of coordinated multi-sector, multi-generational, and/or multilevel interventions for child well-being in populations of children at high risk due to extreme poverty, armed conflicts, famine, and natural disasters (Britto et al., Reference Britto, Lye, Proulx, Yousafzai, Matthews, Vaivada, Perez-Escamilla, Rao, Ip, Fernald, MacMillan, Hanson, Wachs, Yao, Yoshikawa, Cerezo, Leckman and Bhutta2017; Huebner et al., Reference Huebner, Boothby, Aber, Darmstadt, Diaz, Masten, Yoshikawa, Sachs, Redlener, Emmel, Pitt, Arnold, Barber, Berman, Blum, Canavera, Eckerle, Fox, Gibbons, Hargarten and Zeanah2016; Panter-Brick, Reference Panter-Brick2023; Richter et al., Reference Richter, Daelmans, Lombardi, Heymann, Boo, Behrman, Lu, Lucas, Perez-Escamilla, Dua, Bhutta, Stenberg, Gertler and Darmstadt2017). Synergistic effects of coordinated multi-sector and/or multilevel strategies are not surprising given early observations in the literature on risk and resilience that children in situations of high threat or deprivation often face multiple and accumulating risks (Rutter, Reference Rutter, Kent and Rolf1979; see Masten et al., Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990). Cumulative risk situations appear to benefit from cumulative protection (Masten, Reference Masten2001; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Hays-Grudo, Zapata, Treat and Kerr2021; Wyman et al., Reference Wyman, Sandler, Wolchik, Nelson, Cicchetti, Rappaport, Sandler and Weissberg2000; Yoshikawa, Reference Yoshikawa1994).

By the time I wrote a commentary on the state of DRS for a resilience-focused special issues of this journal (Masten, Reference Masten2007), it was clear that a new wave of work was emerging; hence the title: “Resilience in Developing Systems: Progress and Promise as the Fourth Wave Rises.” The shift in DRS to full-blown systems theory had taken hold while at the same time, powerful new methods of research and analysis made it possible to study resilience at multiple levels of analysis, spanning genes to socioecological contexts (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Lucke, Nelson and Stallworthy2021). In addition, multisystem catastrophes, including 9–11 in 2001, the tsunami in 2004 triggered by an earthquake in the Indian Ocean, and Hurricane Katrina in 2005, underscored the necessity of understanding resilience at macrosystem levels (e.g., city, region, country) as well as in individuals and families (Masten, Reference Masten2014). A fourth wave was emerging with a focus on multiple levels of functioning and the dynamics of adaptation.

My views about resilience at this time were profoundly influenced by interactions with researchers studying disasters, including work with Joy and Howard Osofsky before and after Hurricane Katrina, and interactions with multidisciplinary researchers studying resilience. Beginning in 2005, I was part of a small network on “Building an Interdisciplinary Study of Resilience” (Longstaff, Reference Longstaff2009), funded by the US National Science Foundation as part of their Human Dynamics initiative. Our goal was to pave the way for integrating theory and findings on resilience from disparate disciplines, including ecology, psychology, immune function, communications, computer engineering, and urban planning, in order to improve disaster preparation and responses. It quickly became apparent that this goal required better aligning of our definitions of resilience as well as embracing systems thinking. Discussing disaster scenarios, such as a pandemic or natural disaster, helped us learn to communicate. This experience motivated me to adopt a definition of resilience applicable to complex adaptive systems that I thought would be portable and scalable across levels of analysis and disciplines. Thus, I began to define resilience broadly in the following or similar terms: “the capacity of a dynamic system to adapt successfully to significant challenges that threaten the function, survival, or development of the system” (Masten, Reference Masten2007, Reference Masten2011, Reference Masten2014, Reference Masten2018, Reference Masten2021; Masten & Obradović, Reference Masten and Obradović2008).

The fourth wave in DRS rose in conjunction with multilevel research in DP as advancing theory and new research tools made it possible to study more complex processes linking different levels of interaction that were shaping the course of development (Cicchetti & Curtis, Reference Cicchetti and Curtis2007; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016). Multisystem research was spurred, for example, by accessible tools for measuring function and change in activities of genes, neurobiological stress regulation systems, brain function and circuitry, immune function, cognitive and emotional self-regulation, family dynamics, behavior, community supports and other complex systems, both in labs and in the field, in the context of intervention studies as well as observational studies; and also by powerful analytic tools for testing more dynamic and complex models of psychopathology and resilience (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Narayan, Wright, Goldstein and Brooks2023; McEwen, Reference McEwen and Chen2020; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Colich, Rodman and Weissman2020; Ungar, Reference Ungar2018).

The fourth wave highlighted multisystem influences on resilience, underscored the dynamic nature of resilience, and brought much greater attention to resilience processes at both micro and macro levels of analysis. Studies of neurobiological as well as cultural influences surged. Research on the neurobiology of resilience blossomed when the tools for reliable assessments of processes within the human body became more feasible, including assessments of stress biology, gene methylation, immune function, and brain function (Feder et al., Reference Feder, Fred-Torres, Southwick and Charney2019; Feldman, Reference Feldman2021; Gunnar, Reference Gunnar2020; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten, Cicchetti and Cicchetti2016; McEwen, Reference McEwen and Chen2020; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Colich, Rodman and Weissman2020; Shonkoff et al., Reference Shonkoff, Boyce, Levitt, Martinez and McEwen2021). Research focused on sociocultural processes, acculturation, discrimination, social justice, and historical trauma gained more attention (e.g., Jones et al., Reference Jones, Simon, Yadeta, Patterson and Anderson2023; Marks et al., Reference Marks, Woolverton and García Coll2020; Murry et al., Reference Murry, Nyanamba, Hanebutt, Debreaux, Gastineau, Goodwin and Narisetti2023; Panter-Brick, Reference Panter-Brick2023; Spencer, Reference Spencer2023; Motti-Stefanidi, Reference Motti-Stefanidi2023; Suárez-Orozco et al., Reference Suárez-Orozco, Motti-Stefanidi, Marks and Katsiaficas2018; Wilbur & Gone, Reference Wilbur and Gone2023). Multisystem measures of protective factors became prominent, such as the Child and Youth Resilience Measure (Ungar & Liebenberg, Reference Ungar and Liebenberg2011) and indices of positive (rather than adverse) childhood experiences, such as the Benevolent Childhood Experiences scale (Narayan et al., Reference Narayan, Merrick, Lane and Larson2023). The 2023 special issue of this journal on multisystem resilience highlights major themes in the fourth wave of DRS (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Tyrell and Cicchetti2023).

The COVID-19 pandemic undoubtedly stimulated broad interest in resilience, accelerated adoption of multisystem models in resilience theory, and led to a host of new research on resilience in the wake of this global catastrophe. Prime and colleagues (Reference Prime, Wade and Browne2020), for example, proposed a multisystem Family Disruption Model on the effects of risk and protective processes during the pandemic. In short order, Shoychet and colleagues conducted a systematic review of publications testing the model (Reference Shoychet, Kimber, Weiss, Honest and PrimeShoychet Reference Shoychet, Kimber, Weiss, Honest and Primeet al., Reference Shoychet, Kimber, Weiss, Honest and Prime2023). Masten and Motti-Stefanidi (Reference Masten and Motti-Stefanidi2020) proposed in a paper on resilience during the pandemic that the “short list” of resilience factors identified earlier in DRS through an individual resilience lens, were observable at other system levels, in the literature on resilience in family, school, and community systems. The striking alignment of resilience factors across system levels suggested that the capacities associated with resilience in these interacting systems reflect interconnected processes that likely co-evolved, providing far more potential for resilience in human adaptation and development than individuals would be able to muster on their own. We argued that the resilience of children to current threats depends heavily on resilience of key systems in their lives, which also serve to nurture future resilience in children.

Resilience in the context of complex multisystem calamities – like a pandemic or the disasters associated with climate change – require multisystem responses, evidenced around the world during the COVID-19 pandemic. Resilience was mobilized and often bolstered at virtually every level of human adaptive function, ranging from immune function or individual problem-solving to the operations of families, schools, healthcare systems, manufacturing, and research teams, as well as governmental and humanitarian agencies. Impressive successes and disturbing gaps were revealed in the pandemic response across many systems and also in the coordination of multisystem responses (or its lack). Hopefully, lessons will be carried forward to disaster planning and preparation at every system level for future challenges, including the looming crises posed by climate change (Sanson & Masten, Reference Sanson and Masten2023).

The pandemic also highlighted the dynamic nature of resilience during an extended and complex disaster and, concurrently, the challenges in modeling change in complex adaptive systems that are continually interacting. The pandemic had a “roller coaster” quality, not only in regard to the waxing and waning prevalence rates of infection, but also in terms of perceived resilience and adaptive functioning by individuals coping with pandemic challenges (Masten, Reference Masten2021; Walsh, Reference Walsh2020). Parents, for example, could surge capacity to support and protect their children during difficult periods of lock-down, at least for some period of time, but parents also reported periods of feeling depleted or exhausted as the pandemic unfolded (He et al., Reference He, Cabrera, Renteria, Chen, Alonso, McDorman, Kerlow and Reich2021). The idea that resilience fluctuates between surges and depleted capacity underscores the limitations of simple pathway models of resilience that were popular during earlier times. In addition, the goal of assessing roller-coaster patterns in research on resilience demands better methods for capturing dynamic patterns of resilience and adaptive functioning over time in complex adaptive systems.

There certainly are additional signs of progress in the effort to study multisystem processes with strategies that embrace the complexity of multisystem thinking about resilience (Ioannidis et al., Reference Ioannidis, Askelund, Kievit and van Harmelen2020; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Tyrell and Cicchetti2023). In the recent special issue of this journal on multisystem resilience, Hasselman (Reference Hasselman2023) described a toolbox of methods that hold promise for meeting the assumptions of what he terms “strong complexity” models. Nonetheless, collecting empirical data suitable for these methods can be daunting. Additional signs of progress can be found in papers applying network analysis to capture patterns of coordinated resilience resources or processes (e.g., Höltge et al., Reference Höltge, Theron, Cowden, Govender, Maximo, Carranza, Kapoor, Tomar, van Rensburg, Lu, Hu, Cavioni, Agliati, Grazzani, Smedema, Kaur, Hurlington, Sanders, Munford and Ungar2021; Ungar et al., Reference Ungar, Theron and Höltge2023) or linked patterns of stressors, symptoms, and resilience factors (e.g., Kalisch et al., Reference Kalisch, Cramer, Binder, Fritz, Leerouwer, Lunansky, Meyer, Timmer, Veer and van Harmelen2019); multilevel latent class analysis to identify profiles of linked risks, assets, services, and/or adaptive behavior (e.g., Cutuli et al., Reference Cutuli, Herbers, Vrabic and Baye2023) or multiple trajectory models (e.g., Wiglesworth et al., Reference Wiglesworth, Butts, Carosella, Mirza, Papke, Bendezú, Klimes-Dougan and Cullen2023). Nevertheless, multisystem theory about resilience remains substantially ahead of the empirical evidence to date on the patterns and processes of integrated multisystem resilience.

Principles of DRS at the present time

Contemplating the central ideas characterizing DRS at the present time, well over a decade after signs of the fourth wave emerged, the following ten principles stand out to me.

1. DRS is focused on understanding potential and manifested human capacity to respond effectively to major life challenges over the life span, the processes involved, how such capacity develops, and how it can be nurtured and supported.

2. Human resilience is emergent: As in human development more broadly, resilience arises from many processes that span systems and levels of function, biological through ecological.

3. Resilience reflects the properties of a complex adaptive system and its interactions with many systems within and around the individual or social system of interest (e.g., family, community).

4. Resilience changes over multiple time scales, ranging from evolutionary and historical to developmental to short-term or momentary intervals.

5. Resilience changes as a function of development, experience, context, and ongoing multisystem interactions.

6. Many of the systems and processes involved in human resilience are malleable and therefore potentially open to change through nurturing, socialization, intervention, or related efforts to enhance resilience.

7. Due to the many processes involved in human resilience, there are many possibilities for intervention at different levels and through varying processes, although effects will vary depending on leverage, timing, and so forth.

8. Resilience, as with other complex processes in human functioning, can cascade across domains, levels, cultures, and generations.

9. Major engines of resilience reflect fundamental human adaptive systems widely observed across cultures although there also are unique protective factors observed in specific situations or cultures.

10. Similar and analogous resilience factors and processes are observed at multiple system levels, from the individual or family level to the cultural or community level, suggesting that fundamental human adaptive systems co-evolved at multiple system levels, affording possibilities for coordinated responses that yield high potential for collective human adaptive capacity.

Is a fifth wave emerging?

Four waves of DRS have revealed a complex set of insights and findings about human resilience and the multiple systems and processes that shape resilience in human adaptation and development. However, evidence and theory to date point to higher-order multisystem processes and patterns involved in human capacity to adapt, particularly in situations of cascading multisystem disasters, that to date are not well understood. As efforts to measure and model complexity emerge in the study of human resilience, multisystem threats facing humanity worldwide add a rising sense of urgency to the original and overarching goal of resilience research to improve human adaptation to adversity. The growing focus on multisystem methods and processes in DRS may signal the rise of a fifth wave focused on multisystem integration and the dynamics of system interactions, with the possibility of informing further development of interventions directed at changing the interplay of multiple systems or coordinating multiple targets of intervention for synergy.

Whether this shift in focus to multisystem processes and patterns represents a fifth wave or the maturation of the fourth wave, the following questions will need to be addressed.

  • What are the greatest multisystem threats to human development?

  • How can schools and communities implement multisystem prevention and intervention strategies to bolster resilience in children, youth, and their families?

  • When in development and how do challenges and stress exposure enhance resilience?

  • Given the biological wear and tear that can arise within individuals in contexts of structural racism or discrimination, marginalization, and oppression (even among high achieving individuals), how can schools, communities, and/or states confront and dismantle structural and deeply embedded forms of racism, injustice, trauma, or persecution that can pose lifelong threats to healthy development and well-being?

  • Relatedly, what interventions work to prevent intergenerational transmission of trauma and promote healthy development in communities affected by historical trauma?

  • What are practical and valid ways to measure and study complex adaptive processes in action and the “roller coaster” pathways of human development in the context of multisystem threats and protective processes?

  • Which interventions and targets for change, at which points in development for whom, have the most leverage or potential to trigger developmental cascades that alter development in favorable directions, interrupting negative cascades across systems and levels and/or promoting positive cascades?

  • Which malleable targets for intervention show transdiagnostic promotive or protective effects that mitigate or counter threats to development?

  • What changes are needed in societies, higher education, and the priorities of funders to prepare a multidisciplinary workforce of researchers, educators, practitioners, inventors, policymakers, and other essential experts in the arts and sciences to meet the challenges posed to human life and development by multisystem disasters, expected and unexpected?

  • What are the unique contributions that developmental scientists and science organizations can make to enhancing human capacity for resilience at multiple system levels?

The study of resilience always comes back to these questions: What makes a difference? and What can we do about it? These simple questions harbor enormous complexity for scientific inquiry about resilience processes, how they work, and how this knowledge can best be harnessed to protect and enhance the healthy development and well-being of people everywhere. There are many difficult questions ahead, as well as challenges inherent in studying the behavior of complex adaptive systems. Yet there has been observable progress. There are consistent findings and intriguing parallels across levels and disciplines, evidence on interventions that work, and a growing body of research as well as a growing cadre of young scholars engaged in research on resilience. There has been a profound shift in the focus of many disciplines concerned with the development and well-being of children, away from models that narrowly focused on vulnerabilities, stressors, risks, symptoms, and strategies for “fixing” problems or treating deficits toward models that include or even emphasize multisystem strengths, resources, promotive and protective influences and positive processes in adjustment, development, and achievements, along with strategies for preventing or mitigating risk and adversity exposure, boosting resources, building protections, and enhancing opportunities.

Families, schools, communities, cultures, and governments all play vital roles in the development of resilience in their children, who in turn protect the future resilience of societies. DRS continues to evolve toward deeper understanding of multisystem and multilevel processes that support and nurture human capacity for surviving, learning, and even flourishing in response to adversity –on small and large scales, spanning challenges from the cellular to global level, and momentary to evolutionary time scales. The progress and evolution in multisystem resilience science and its promise for informing prevention and practice offers hope for future human well-being and development during these exceptionally turbulent times.

Acknowledgements

This commentary is a tribute to Dante Cicchetti for his profound and far-reaching contributions to developmental resilience science as well as developmental psychopathology, his remarkable mentoring and support of multiple generations of developmental and clinical scholars (including myself), and for his friendship over nearly five decades.

Funding statement

Not applicable for this commentary.

Competing interests

The author declares none.

References

Arakelyan, S., & Ager, A. (2021). Annual research review: A multilevel bioecological analysis of factors influencing the mental health and psychosocial well-being of refugee children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(5), 484509. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13355 Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). The hidden efficacy of interventions: Gene-environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 381409. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015407 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Betancourt, T. S., & Khan, K. T. (2008). The mental health of children affected by armed conflict: Protective processes and pathways to resilience. International Review of Psychiatry, 20(3), 317328. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540260802090363 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma and human resilience: Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, 59(1), 2028.Google Scholar
Britto, P. R., Lye, S. J., Proulx, K., Yousafzai, A. K., Matthews, S. G., Vaivada, T., Perez-Escamilla, R., Rao, N., Ip, P., Fernald, L. C. H., MacMillan, H., Hanson, M., Wachs, T. D., Yao, H., Yoshikawa, H., Cerezo, A., Leckman, J. F., & Bhutta, Z. A. (2017). Nurturing care: Promoting early childhood development. The Lancet, 389(10064), 91102. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31390-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 793828). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0114 Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129830 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1989). Developmental psychopathology: Past, present, and future. In Cicchetti, D. (Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology (pp. 112). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2006). Development and psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 123). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939383 Google ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (2013). Annual research review: Resilient functioning in maltreated children—past, present, and future perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(4), 402422. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02608.x Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2016). Developmental psychopathology (3rd ed.). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Curtis, W. J. (2007). Multilevel perspectives on pathways to resilient functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 19(3), 627629. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407000314 Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Garmezy, N. (1993). Prospects and promises in the study of resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 497502. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400006118 Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 597600. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007318 Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Fostering secure attachment in infants in maltreating families through preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 18(3), 623648. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579406060329 Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1-2), 1625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, E. M., & Valentino, K. (2015). Developmental psychopathology. In Overton, W. F., Molenaar, P. C. M., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (pp. 566606). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy115 Google Scholar
Cutuli, J. J., Herbers, J. E., Vrabic, S. C., & Baye, O. (2023). Families with young children in homeless shelters: Developmental contexts of multisystem risks and resources. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 24302443. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000871 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doom, J. R., Deer, L. B. K., Dieujuste, N., Han, D., Rivera, K. M., & Scott, S. R. (2023). Youth psychosocial resilience during the COVID-19 pandemic. Current Opinion in Psychology, 53, 101656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2023.101656 Google Scholar
Doty, J. L., Davis, L., & Arditti, J. A. (2017). Cascading resilience: Leverage points in promoting parent and child well-being. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 9(1), 111126. https://doi.org/10.1111/jftr.12175 Google Scholar
Dray, J., Bowman, J., Campbell, E., Freund, M., Wolfenden, L., Hodder, R. K., McElwaine, K., Tremain, D., Bartlem, K., Bailey, J., Small, T., Palazzi, K., Oldmeadow, C., & Wiggers, J. (2017). Systematic review of universal resilience-focused interventions targeting child and adolescent mental health in the school setting. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(10), 813824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.07.780 Google Scholar
Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1993). Resilience as process. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 517528. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400006131 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary-neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000611 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feder, A., Fred-Torres, S., Southwick, S. M., & Charney, D. S. (2019). The biology of human resilience: Opportunities for enhancing resilience across the life span. Biological Psychiatry, 86(6), 443453.Google Scholar
Feldman, R. (2021). Social behavior as a transdiagnostic marker of resilience. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 153180. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-102046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, P. A., Stoolmiller, M., Gunnar, M. R., & Burraston, B. O. (2007). Effects of a therapeutic intervention for foster preschoolers on diurnal cortisol activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32(8-10), 892905. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.06.008 Google Scholar
Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study of stress and competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 97111. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129837 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garmezy, N., & Rutter, M. (1983). Stress, coping, and development in children. McGraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
Gartland, D., Riggs, E., Muyeen, S., Giallo, R., Afifi, T. O., MacMillan, H., Herrman, H., Bulford, E., & Brown, S. J. (2019). What factors are associated with resilient outcomes in children exposed to social adversity? A systematic review. BMJ Open, 9(4), e024870. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-024870 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottesman, I. I. (1974). Developmental genetics and ontogenetic psychology: Overdue détente and propositions from a matchmaker. In Pick, A. D. (Eds.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology(pp. 5580). University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R. (2020). Early adversity, stress, and neurobehavioral development. Development and Psychopathology, 32(5), 15551562. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001649 Google Scholar
Hasselman, F. (2023). Understanding the complexity of individual developmental pathways: A primer on metaphors, models, and methods to study resilience and fragility. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 21862198. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001281 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, M., Cabrera, N., Renteria, J., Chen, Y., Alonso, A., McDorman, S. A., Kerlow, M. A., & Reich, S. M. (2021). Family functioning in the time of COVID-19 among economically vulnerable families: Risks and protective factors. Frontiers in Psychology, 12(730447), 116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.730447 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Höltge, J., Theron, L., Cowden, R. G., Govender, K., Maximo, S. I., Carranza, J. S., Kapoor, B., Tomar, A., van Rensburg, A., Lu, S., Hu, H., Cavioni, V., Agliati, A., Grazzani, I., Smedema, Y., Kaur, G., Hurlington, K. G., Sanders, J., Munford, R., …Ungar, M. (2021). A cross-country network analysis of adolescent resilience. Journal of Adolescent Health, 68(3), 580588. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.07.010 Google Scholar
Hostinar, C. E., & Miller, G. E. (2019). Protective factors for youth confronting economic hardship: Current challenges and future avenues in resilience research. American Psychologist, 74(6), 641652.Google Scholar
Huebner, G., Boothby, N., Aber, J. L., Darmstadt, G. L., Diaz, A., Masten, A. S., Yoshikawa, H., Sachs, J., Redlener, I., Emmel, A., Pitt, M., Arnold, L., Barber, B., Berman, B., Blum, R., Canavera, M., Eckerle, J., Fox, N. A., Gibbons, J. L., Hargarten, S. W., …Zeanah, C. H.. Beyond survival: The case for investing in young children globally. (2016). https://doi.org/10.31478/201606bCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, K., Askelund, A. D., Kievit, R. A., & van Harmelen, A.-L. (2020). The complex neurobiology of resilient functioning after childhood maltreatment. BMC Medicine, 18(1), 32, 116. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-020-1490-7 Google ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. C. T., Simon, C. B., Yadeta, K., Patterson, A., & Anderson, R. E. (2023). When resilience is not enough: Imagining novel approaches to supporting black youth navigating racism. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 21322140. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000986 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalisch, R., Cramer, A. O. J., Binder, H., Fritz, J., Leerouwer, I., Lunansky, G., Meyer, B., Timmer, J., Veer, I. M., & van Harmelen, A.-L. (2019). Deconstructing and reconstructing resilience: A dynamic network approach. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(5), 765777. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619855637 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, R. M. (2006). Developmental science, developmental systems, and contemporary theories of human development. In Lerner, R. M., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 117). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0101 Google Scholar
Longstaff, P. H. (2009). Managing surprises in complex systems: Multidisciplinary perspectives on resilience. Ecology and Society, 14(1), 49. http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol14/iss1/art49/ CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luthar, S. S. (2006). Resilience in development: A synthesis of research across five decades. In Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 739795). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939406.ch20 Google Scholar
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71(3), 543562. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00164 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, A. K., Woolverton, G. A., & García Coll, C. (2020). Risk and resilience in minority youth populations. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 151163. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071119-115839 Google Scholar
Marquez, J., Francis-Hew, L., & Humphrey, N. (2023). Protective factors for resilience in adolescence: Analysis of a longitudinal dataset using the residuals approach. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 17(1), 124. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-023-00687-8 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. (1989). Resilience in development: Implications of the study of successful adaptation for developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. (Eds.), The emergence of a discipline: Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology (pp. 261294). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist, 56(3), 227238.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. (2007). Resilience in developing systems: Progress and promise as the fourth wave rises. Development and Psychopathology, 19(3), 921930. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407000442 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2011). Resilience in children threatened by extreme adversity: Frameworks for research, practice, and translational synergy. Development and Psychopathology, 23(2), 493506. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000198 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. (2014). Global perspectives on resilience in children and youth. Child Development, 85(1), 620. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12205 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2018). Resilience theory and research on children and families: Past, present, and promise. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 10(1), 1231. https://doi.org/10.1111/jftr.12255 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. (2021). Resilience of children in disasters: A multisystem perspective. International Journal of Psychology, 56(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12737 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2(4), 425444. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400005812 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2016). Resilience in development: Progress and transformation. In Cicchetti, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp.271333). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy406 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1995). Competence, resilience, and psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 715752). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons from successful children. American Psychologist, 53(2), 205220. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.205 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Kalstabakken, A. W. (2018). Developmental perspectives on psychopathology in children and adolescents. In Butcher, J. N., & Kendall, P. C. (Eds.), APA handbook of psychopathology (pp. 1536). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000065-002 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Lucke, C. M., Nelson, K. M., & Stallworthy, I. C. (2021). Resilience in development and psychopathology: Multisystem perspectives. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 521549. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-120307 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Motti-Stefanidi, F. (2020). Multisystem resilience for children and youth in disaster: Reflections in the context of COVID-19. Adversity and Resilience Science, 1(2), 95106. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42844-020-00010-w Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Narayan, A. J., & Wright, M. O.’D. (2023). Resilience processes in development: Multisystem integration emerging from four waves of research. In Goldstein, S., & Brooks, R. B. (Eds.), Handbook of resilience in children (pp. 1946). Springer Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-14728-9_2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Obradović, J. (2008). Disaster preparation and recovery: Lessons from research on resilience in human development. Ecology and Society, 13(1), 9. http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol13/iss Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Palmer, A. (2019). Parenting to promote resilience in children. In Bornstein, M. H. (Eds.), Handbook of parenting (pp. 156188). https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429440847 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Powell, J. L. (2003). A resilience framework for research, policy, and practice. In Luthar, S. S. (Eds.), Resilience and vulnerabilities: Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities (pp. 125). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Tyrell, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2023). Resilience in development: Pathways to multisystem integration. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 21032112. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001293 Google Scholar
McEwen, B. S. (2020). A life-course, epigenetic perspective on resilience in brain and body. In Chen, A. (Eds.), Stress resilience: Molecular and behavioral aspects (pp. 121). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-813983-7.00001-X Google Scholar
McLaughlin, K. A., Colich, N. L., Rodman, A. M., & Weissman, D. G. (2020). Mechanisms linking childhood trauma exposure and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic model of risk and resilience. BMC Medicine, 18(1), 96. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-020-01561-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene x environment interactions. Child Development, 81(1), 4179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01381.x Google Scholar
Meng, X., Fleury, M.-J., Xiang, Y.-T., Li, M., & D’Arcy, C. (2018). Resilience and protective factors among people with a history of child maltreatment: A systematic review. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 53(5), 453475. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-018-1485-2 Google Scholar
Mesman, E., Vreeker, A., & Hillegers, M. (2021). Resilience and mental health in children and adolescents: An update of the recent literature and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 34(6), 586592. https://doi.org/10.1097/yco.0000000000000741 Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Hays-Grudo, J., Zapata, M. I., Treat, A., & Kerr, K. L. (2021). Adverse and protective childhood experiences and parenting attitudes: The role of cumulative protection in understanding resilience. Adversity and Resilience Science, 2(3), 181192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42844-021-00036-8 Google Scholar
Motti-Stefanidi, F. (2023). Acculturation and resilience of immigrant-origin youth: Do their school experiences reflect nonimmigrants’ “native supremacy”? Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 21552167. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000895 Google Scholar
Murry, V. M. B., Nyanamba, J. M., Hanebutt, R., Debreaux, M., Gastineau, K. A. B., Goodwin, A. K. B., & Narisetti, L. (2023). Critical examination of resilience and resistance in African American families: Adaptive capacities to navigate toxic oppressive upstream waters. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 21132131. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001037Google Scholar
Narayan, A. J., Merrick, J. S., Lane, A. S., & Larson, M. D. (2023). A multisystem, dimensional interplay of assets versus adversities: Revised benevolent childhood experiences (BCEs) in the context of childhood maltreatment, threat, and deprivation. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 24442463. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000536 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overton, W. F. (2013). A new paradigm for developmental science: Relationism and relational– developmental systems. Applied Developmental Science, 17(2), 94107. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2013.778717 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panter-Brick, C. (2015). Culture and resilience: Next steps for theory and practice. In Theron, L. C., Liebenberg, L., & Ungar, M. (Eds.), Youth resilience and culture: Commonalities and complexities (pp. 233244). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panter-Brick, C. (2023). Pathways to resilience and pathways to flourishing: Examining the added-value of multisystem research and intervention in contexts of war and forced displacement. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 22142225. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942300113X Google Scholar
Prime, H., Wade, M., & Browne, D. T. (2020). Risk and resilience in family well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. American Psychologist, 75(5), 631643. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000660 Google Scholar
Raposa, E. B., Rhodes, J., Stams, G. J. J. M., Card, N., Burton, S., Schwartz, S., Sykes, L. A. Y., Kanchewa, S., Kupersmidt, J., & Hussain, S. (2019). The effects of youth mentoring programs: A meta-analysis of outcome studies. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48(3), 423443. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-00982-8 Google Scholar
Rhodes, C. A., Wolchik, S. A., Uhlman, R. N., O’Hara, K. L., Sandler, I. N., Tein, J.-Y., & Porter, M. M. (2023). Effects of a preventive parenting intervention for bereaved families on the intergenerational transmission of parenting attitudes: Mediating processes. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 24822498. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000925 Google Scholar
Richter, L. M., Daelmans, B., Lombardi, J., Heymann, J., Boo, F. L., Behrman, J. R., Lu, C., Lucas, J. E., Perez-Escamilla, R., Dua, T., Bhutta, Z. A., Stenberg, K., Gertler, P., & Darmstadt, G. L. (2017). Investing in the foundation of sustainable development: Pathways to scale up for early childhood development. The Lancet, 389(10064), 103118. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31698-1 Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors in children’s responses to stress and disadvantage. In Kent, M. W., & Rolf, J. E. (Eds.), Primary prevention of psychopathology (pp. 4974). University Press of New England.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57(3), 316331. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1987.tb03541.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Development, 81(1), 622. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01378.x Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. J. (2000). Developmental systems and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 297312. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400003035 Google Scholar
Sandler, I., Ingram, A., Wolchik, S., Tein, J.-Y., & Winslow, E. (2015). Long-term effects of parenting-focused preventive interventions to promote resilience of children and adolescents. Child Development Perspectives, 9(3), 164171. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12126 Google Scholar
Sanson, A. V., & Masten, A. S. (2023). Climate change and resilience: Developmental science perspectives. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 48(2), 93102. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254231186332 Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P., Boyce, W. T., Levitt, P., Martinez, F. D., & McEwen, B. (2021). Leveraging the biology of adversity and resilience to transform pediatric practice. Pediatrics, 147(2), e20193845. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2019-3845 Google Scholar
Shoychet, G., Kimber, M., Weiss, J., Honest, O., & Prime, H. (2023). Empirical support for a model of risk and resilience in children and families during COVID-19: A systematic review & narrative synthesis. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 24642481. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000767 Google Scholar
Spencer, M. B. (2023). Interrogating multisystem intended pathways to youth thriving and resilience: Benefits of inclusive human development theoretical framing. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 21412154. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001104 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 1729. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129832 Google Scholar
Suárez-Orozco, C., Motti-Stefanidi, F., Marks, A., & Katsiaficas, D. (2018). An integrative risk and resilience model for understanding the adaptation of immigrant-origin children and youth. American Psychologist, 73(6), 781796. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000265 Google Scholar
Toth, S. L., Gravener-Davis, J. A., Guild, D. J., & Cicchetti, D. (2013). Relational interventions for child maltreatment: Past, present, and future perspectives. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4pt2), 16011617. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000795 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, S. L., Rogosch, F. A., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (2006). The efficacy of toddler-parent psychotherapy to reorganize attachment in the young offspring of mothers with major depressive disorder: A randomized preventive trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(6), 10061016. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.1006 Google Scholar
Troller-Renfree, S. V., Costanzo, M. A., Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K., Gennetian, L. A., Yoshikawa, H., Halpern-Meekin, S., Fox, N. A., & Noble, K. G. (2022). The impact of a poverty reduction intervention on infant brain activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(5), e2115649119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2115649119 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungar, M. (2008). Resilience across cultures. British Journal of Social Work, 38(2), 218235. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjsw/bcl343 Google Scholar
Ungar, M. (2018). Systemic resilience: Principles and processes for a science of change in contexts of adversity. Ecology and Society, 23(4), 34. https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-10385-230434 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ungar, M., & Liebenberg, L. (2011). Assessing resilience across cultures using mixed methods: Construction of the child and youth resilience measure. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 5(2), 126149. https://doi.org/10.1177/1558689811400607 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ungar, M., & Theron, L. (2020). Resilience and mental health: How multisystemic processes contribute to positive outcomes. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(5), 441448. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30434-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungar, M., Theron, L., & Höltge, J. (2023). Multisystemic approaches to researching young people’s resilience: Discovering culturally and contextually sensitive accounts of thriving under adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 21992213. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000469 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J., Coughlan, B., & Reijman, S. (2020). Annual research review: Umbrella synthesis of met-analyses on child maltreatment antecedents and interventions: Differential susceptibility perspective on risk and resilience. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(3), 272290. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13147 Google Scholar
Walsh, F. (2020). Loss and resilience in the time of COVID-19: Meaning making, hope, and transcendence. Family Process, 59(3), 898911. https://doi.org/10.1111/famp.12588 Google Scholar
Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, and recovery: Perspectives from the kauai longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4), 503515. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940000612X Google Scholar
Werner, E. E. (2012). Children and war: Risk, resilience, and recovery. Development and Psychopathology, 24(2), 553558. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000156 Google Scholar
Wiglesworth, A., Butts, J., Carosella, K. A., Mirza, S., Papke, V., Bendezú, J. J., Klimes-Dougan, B., & Cullen, K. R. (2023). Stress system concordance as a predictor of longitudinal patterns of resilience in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 23842401. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000731 Google Scholar
Wilbur, R. E., & Gone, J. P. (2023). Beyond resilience: A scoping review of indigenous survivance in the health literature. Development and Psychopathology, 35(5), 22262240). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000706 Google Scholar
Wright, M. O.’D., Masten, A. S., & Narayan, A. J. (2013). Resilience processes in development: Four waves of research on positive adaptation in the context of adversity. In Goldstein, S., & Brooks, R. B. (Eds.), Handbook of resilience in children (pp. 1537). Springer.Google Scholar
Wyman, P. A., Sandler, I., Wolchik, S., & Nelson, K. (2000). Resilience as cumulative competence promotion and stress protection: Theory and intervention. In Cicchetti, D., Rappaport, J., Sandler, I., & Weissberg, R. P. (Eds.), The promotion of wellness in children and adolescents (pp. 133184). Child Welfare League of America Press.Google Scholar
Yoshikawa, H. (1994). Prevention as cumulative protection: Effects of early family support and education on chronic delinquency and its risks. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 2854. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.28 Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Developmental psychology: A new synthesis. In Zelazo, P. D. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 312). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958450.013.0001 Google Scholar