Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:22:42.508Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of childhood maltreatment on adult survivors’ parental reflective function, and attachment of their children: A systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 April 2023

Elmie Janse van Rensburg*
Affiliation:
Embrace, Telethon Kids Institute, WA, Australia School of Psychological Science, The University of Western Australia, WA, Australia Youth Mental Health Team, Telethon Kids Institute, WA, Australia
Alix Woolard
Affiliation:
Embrace, Telethon Kids Institute, WA, Australia Youth Mental Health Team, Telethon Kids Institute, WA, Australia Medical School, University of Western Australia, WA, Australia
Nicole T.M. Hill
Affiliation:
Youth Mental Health Team, Telethon Kids Institute, WA, Australia Centre for Child Health Research, University of Western Australia, WA, Australia
Carol Reid
Affiliation:
Judith Lumley Centre, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Helen Milroy
Affiliation:
Embrace, Telethon Kids Institute, WA, Australia Youth Mental Health Team, Telethon Kids Institute, WA, Australia Medical School, University of Western Australia, WA, Australia
Jeneva L. Ohan
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, The University of Western Australia, WA, Australia
Ashleigh Lin
Affiliation:
Youth Mental Health Team, Telethon Kids Institute, WA, Australia Centre for Child Health Research, University of Western Australia, WA, Australia
Catherine Chamberlain
Affiliation:
Judith Lumley Centre, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Melbourne School of Population and Global Health, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
*
Corresponding Author: Elmie Janse van Rensburg, email: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Parental reflective function (PRF) is a candidate mechanism in the transmission of intergenerational trauma. This systematic review examined (1) the association between parental history of childhood maltreatment and PRF, (2) how PRF relates to attachment in children of parent survivors, and (3) whether PRF moderates the association between parental maltreatment history and child attachment.

Methods:

Ten databases were searched (from inception to 10th November 2021). Inclusion criteria were primary study, quantitative, parent participants, measures of childhood maltreatment, and postnatal PRF. Exclusion criteria were qualitative, intervention follow-up, gray literature, or a review study. Risk of bias was assessed using recommended tools. Data were narratively synthesized.

Results:

One-thousand-and-two articles were retrieved, of which eleven met inclusion criteria (N = 974 participants). Four studies found a significant association between parental childhood maltreatment and disrupted PRF, six did not, one found mixed results. One study reported the association between childhood maltreatment and attachment (nonsignificant results).

Discussion:

There is no clear evidence PRF is routinely disrupted in parent survivors, though there is high heterogeneity in studies. Future research should standardize design to better understand whether PRF is a candidate mechanism in intergenerational trauma.

Other:

PROSPERO CRD42020223594

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Highlights

  • This is the first systematic review of the relationship between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment, their parental reflective function, and attachment of their children

  • Studies did not consistently find that when a parent was maltreated as a child, they had poorer reflective function.

  • Most studies excluded participants with serious mental health issues, even though this is common in survivors of childhood maltreatment (i.e., they excluded a large portion of the target population).

Introduction

Approximately one in three people are survivors of childhood maltreatment (Stoltenborgh et al., Reference Stoltenborgh, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Alink and Van Ijzendoorn2015). The World Health Organization (WHO) defines childhood maltreatment as “all forms of physical and/or emotional ill-treatment, sexual abuse, neglect or negligent treatment… resulting in actual or potential harm to the child’s health, survival, development or dignity in the context of a relationship of responsibility, trust or power” (Horswell & Istfan, Reference Horswell and Istfan2006, p. 3). The consequences of being exposed to maltreatment include a variety of physical health (especially inflammatory) diseases, abnormalities in brain and endocrine structure and function, psycho-social difficulties (e.g., difficulties in relationships, aggression), and specific outcomes related to specific forms of maltreatment experienced (e.g., aggression and physical abuse, emotional abuse, and mental health issues; Carr et al., Reference Carr, Duff and Craddock2020; Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards, Koss and Marks2019).

Children of survivors of childhood maltreatment have also been found to have higher rates of mental illness, physical illness, disability, and familial dysfunction (Montgomery et al., Reference Montgomery, Just-Østergaard and Jervelund2019; Woods-Jaeger et al., Reference Woods-Jaeger, Cho, Sexton, Slagel and Goggin2018). The transmission of consequences of childhood maltreatment has been referred to as intergenerational trauma. It is therefore imperative to better understand target factors to halt the transmission of intergenerational trauma, ideally, as early in the parenting journey as possible.

The perinatal and postnatal period of parenthood is one of the most important opportunities to break the cycle of intergenerational trauma (Chamberlain, Gee, Harfield, et al., Reference Chamberlain, Gee, Harfield, Campbell, Brennan, Clark, Mensah, Arabena, Herrman, Brown, Atkinson, Nicholson, Gartland, Glover, Mitchell, Atkinson, McLachlan, Andrews, Hirvoven and Dyall2019). This is because the transition to becoming a parent is both a time of risk and healing for the parent. Factors associated with pregnancy, birth and early parenting may trigger memories of past traumatic experiences (e.g., loss of bodily autonomy and extreme stress), though also represent an opportunity for the parent to resolve and learn from their past experiences (e.g., Iyengar et al., Reference Iyengar, Rajhans, Fonagy, Strathearn and Kim2019).

Theoretical background: reflective function and attachment

Reflective function (also known as mentalization) is defined as an individual’s ability to understand the association between inner mental states (thoughts, feelings, emotions, etc.) and behavior (Fonagy & Target, Reference Fonagy and Target1997). Examples of poor reflective function include difficulty understanding thoughts and intentions of others, or in understanding one’s own motivation for saying or doing things. For individuals with difficulty in reflective function, interacting with others can be highly confusing and distressing, and they can have a higher risk of experiencing severe mental health issues (Slade, Reference Slade2005). Reflective function is thought to be a key skill in facilitating secure attachment of infants of parent survivors of childhood maltreatment, which in turn is associated with better mental and physical health outcomes across the lifespan (Doyle & Cicchetti, Reference Doyle and Cicchetti2017).

Secure attachment refers to an individual’s adaptive pattern of beliefs, expectations, and interactions with others in close relationships and forms in response to nurturing, attentive, and responsive caregiving from infancy (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1988). Although attachment style is malleable to change with experiences across the lifespan, children who have been maltreated are significantly less likely to be securely attached to caregivers, which in turn creates risk for relational and mental health difficulties (Widom et al., Reference Widom, Czaja, Kozakowski and Chauhan2018).

Further theoretical evidence for the role of reflective function in the intergenerational transmission of childhood maltreatment consequences (i.e., insecure attachment, increased risk for adverse mental, physical, and social health outcomes) comes from research which found that reflective function ability may be "transmitted" from a parent to their child (Pajulo et al., Reference Pajulo, Tolvanen, Pyykkönen, Karlsson, Mayes and Karlsson2018), such that parents with lower levels of reflective function have children with relatively lower levels of reflective function. This is thought to result from the lack of a safe environment and/or responsive caregiver to be attuned to the child and help them to develop knowledge of inner mental states (Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017). Studies have found that not only can reflective function ability be passed from parents to their children, but parental reflective function (PRF) can be an important predictor of emotion regulation and attachment in infants, school-aged children, and adolescents (Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017; Slade, Reference Slade2005).

Parental reflective function as a mechanism of intergenerational trauma

PRF is a related but not identical construct to reflective function in general (Anis et al., Reference Anis, Perez, Benzies, Ewashen, Hart and Letourneau2020) and refers to a parent’s ability to treat the child as a psychological agent (i.e., having their own thoughts and motivations; Slade, Reference Slade2005). A parent with healthy PRF will therefore see their child as a separate entity from themselves, with a unique inner world and motivations which they are curious to better understand. This is thought to be a specific context and application of the core socio-cognitive skill of reflective function, without which, the development of secure attachment is significantly less likely (Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Normandin, Plamondon, Berthelot and Fonagy2016).

In the initial validation study of the Parental Reflective Function Questionnaire (PRFQ), Luyten et al. (Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017) conducted an exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis in clinical and nonclinical populations. They found three main dimensions of PRF across populations: level of parental curiosity and interest in the child’s mental states (e.g., “I am often curious to find out how my child feels”), prementalizing modes (e.g., “When my child is fussy he/she does this just to annoy me”), and degree of certainty of mental states (e.g., “I always know why my child acts the way he/she does”) (Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017). According to this measure, a parent is classified as having healthy or “high” PRF when they are high on parental curiosity, but low on degree of prementalizing and certainty modes. Luyten et al. (Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017) found that poorer PRF showed convergent validity through significant associations with poor parental attachment to their own child, high stress, and low emotional availability.

Traditionally, as in the PRFQ (Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017), PRF has been measured through questionnaires and interviews which aim to quantify the “mind-mindedness” or extent to which a parent (typically the mother) communicates interest in or awareness of the child’s inner states (Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017). However, researchers and clinicians have increasingly noted that sole reliance on verbal measures of PRF may be, at best, inadequate in capturing the true presence of reflective function, and at worst, biased against disadvantaged groups of parents (e.g., linguistically diverse or low socioeconomic status; Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017). Shai and Belsky (Reference Shai and Belsky2011) were among the first to raise this issue, and as a solution operationalized a nonverbal measure of PRF – Parental Embodied Mentalizing – which aims to capture implicit evidence of PRF via patterns of bodily attunement between the parent and child (e.g., the parent treating an infant as owning its own body). Although parental embodied mentalizing has shown significant associations with child secure attachment (Gagné et al., Reference Gagné, Lemelin and Tarabulsy2021) and skills such as toddler emotion recognition (Afek et al., Reference Afek., Lev-Wiesel, Federman and Shai.2022), it remains seldom used and inadequately explored in PRF and childhood maltreatment research.

There are a variety of ways in which childhood maltreatment is thought to effect PRF. These include a lack of opportunity to practice emotional co-regulation with an attachment figure (Bethell et al., Reference Bethell, Gombojav, Solloway and Wissow2016), the absence of a safe and nurturing environment in which to explore their inner world safely (Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Sleed, Midgley, Fearon, Mein, Bateman and Fonagy2019), or a frightening caregiver who does not provide predictability or comfort in their response patterns (Luyten & Fonagy, Reference Luyten and Fonagy2015).

While each of these situations is thought to lead to disrupted PRF and consequently attachment relationship difficulties, they entail vastly different caregiving environments and suggest the possibility that qualitative differences in experiences of childhood maltreatment lead to different adult outcomes (Infurna et al., Reference Infurna, Reichl, Parzer, Schimmenti, Bifulco and Kaess2016; Messman-Moore & Bhuptani, Reference Messman-Moore and Bhuptani2017). Although cumulative exposure to maltreatment has been strongly associated with adverse outcomes (Sheridan et al., Reference Sheridan, Shi, Miller, Salhi and McLaughlin2020), a plethora of research document differences in predicted outcomes according to the type, timing, severity, or frequency of maltreatment experienced in addition to multi-victimisation or poly-victimization status (Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, McGuire, Tunno and Makanui2019; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Bae, Rim, Lee, Chang, Kim and Won2018). Despite this, the predominant approach in the childhood maltreatment literature has been to consider it as a unidimensional experience (Toth & Manly, Reference Toth and Manly2019). Consideration for the nuance in form of childhood maltreatment experienced is therefore an integral part of understanding the association between parental history of childhood maltreatment and their PRF.

Parental reflective function-based clinical interventions

The promising evidence for PRF as a mechanism in the intergenerational transmission of trauma consequences has led to a variety of PRF-based parenting interventions (e.g., Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Sleed, Midgley, Fearon, Mein, Bateman and Fonagy2019; Cohen, Reference Cohen2016; Stob et al., Reference Stob, Slade, Brotnow, Adnopoz and Woolston2019). Preliminary results of such interventions have shown significant improvements in PRF and attachment (Ashton et al., Reference Ashton, O’Brien-Langer and Silverstone2016), as well as parenting confidence and sensitivity (Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Sleed, Midgley, Fearon, Mein, Bateman and Fonagy2019). However, no systematic review or meta-analysis has examined the association between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment, their PRF when they themselves become parents, and subsequent impact on their own children. There is therefore a clear and imminent need to systematically evaluate this association, given the substantial risk of intergenerational harm for parents and children, and the fact that PRF therapies are already being incorporated into research and practice with vulnerable families.

Aims

To address gaps in understanding of the role of PRF in perpetuating or interrupting cycles of intergenerational trauma, the present study had four main aims: 1) to systematically examine the consistency, strength, and direction of an association between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment and their PRF; 2) to examine the association between PRF of maltreatment survivors and the attachment of their children; 3) to explore the potential for PRF to moderate the association between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment and the attachment status of their children; and 4) to examine the effect of qualitative differences in the experience of child maltreatment (e.g., type, timing, and severity) upon the aforementioned associations. Addressing these questions will help to understand if childhood maltreatment is consistently associated with lower PRF in parent survivors, and attachment outcomes of their children. Understanding of how qualitative differences in childhood maltreatment experiences effect this relationship will facilitate targeted interventions for survivors. This will provide the much-needed high level evidence to inform PRF-informed interventions and research.

Method

Protocol and registration

This review was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow, Shamseer, Tetzlaff, Akl, Brennan, Chou, Glanville, Grimshaw, Hróbjartsson, Lalu, Li, Loder, Mayo-Wilson, McDonald and Moher2021). A protocol for the review was registered on PROSPERO (ID CRD42020223594).

Participants

The population of interest were postnatal parents who had experienced childhood maltreatment prior to age 18 (even if only a subsample of a larger population). All parent populations were considered.

Exposure

The exposure of interest was childhood maltreatment (physical abuse, physical neglect, emotional abuse, emotional neglect, and/or sexual abuse during childhood prior to age 18 years), as defined by the WHO (2016).

Outcomes

The primary outcome was PRF in parents who had survived childhood maltreatment and attachment of their children. However, after the initial search it became clear that very few studies measured attachment in addition to childhood maltreatment and PRF, therefore only the latter two outcomes were made compulsory. Studies were ultimately included if they employed a formal measure of PRF and childhood maltreatment. The secondary outcome was qualitative differences in the type, timing or frequency/severity of childhood maltreatment.

Study designs

All primary studies with an observational and quantitative design were included, including longitudinal and intervention studies (using only first wave and baseline data, to be comparable to other single-wave observational studies). Results were not limited to English, with the understanding that articles in other languages would initially be screened using Google Translate, and that the full translation would be requested from authors if the article made it to the full-text screening stage. Inclusion of articles in various languages was preferable to reduce the risk of publishing bias (Dobrescu et al., Reference Dobrescu, Nussbaumer-Streit, Klerings, Wagner, Persad, Sommer, Herkner and Gartlehner2021).

Exclusion criteria

Studies were excluded if they measured prenatal PRF, based on literature which documents substantial identity reorganization during childbirth and initial experiences as a parent (Amos et al., Reference Amos, Segal and Cantor2015; Chamberlain, Gee, Brown, et al., Reference Chamberlain, Gee, Brown, Atkinson, Herrman, Gartland, Glover, Clark, Campbell, Mensah, Atkinson, Brennan, McLachlan, Hirvonen, Dyall, Ralph, Hokke and Nicholson2019). Studies were also excluded if a formal measure of childhood maltreatment or PRF was not used; they reported results from the second wave of an intervention study; the exposure to maltreatment did not occur before age 18; maltreatment was not reported specific to the parent population; or childhood maltreatment was not measured separately from traumatic events not related to parent–child relationship behavior (e.g., accidents, family deaths). Articles were excluded if they had a purely qualitative or case study design; consisted of editorials, letters, or conference abstracts; were from gray literature; or if a review article.

Search strategy and data collection

A search strategy was created by combining terms related to the following key concepts with “AND”: childhood maltreatment, parenting, and reflective function. Search terms included both keyword and thesaurus (Medical Subject Headings; MeSH) terms for “childhood maltreatment” AND “parenting” AND “reflective function.” This strategy was developed in collaboration between three of the authors (EvR, CC, and AW) and a research librarian. The original search strategy was piloted in PsycINFO (final strategy publicly documented on PROSPERO). Ten databases were searched for studies published before November 10, 2021: CINAHL, The Cochrane Library, Embase, JBI, ProQuest, PubMed, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, Scopus, and Web of Science. Results from this search are outlined in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Flowchart for the inclusion of articles. Note. Parental reflective function is represented by the acronym PRF.

References were downloaded into a citation manager (Zotero) and de-duplicated. Two reviewers (EvR and AW) independently screened titles and abstracts. An overly inclusive approach was taken, such that discrepancies were carried over into the full-text review stage. The full texts of identified studies were again independently screened (EvR and AW). Articles which resulted in discrepancies between the first two reviewers (n = 8) were sent to an independent third reviewer, who assessed them autonomously (CC). Papers which resulted in further uncertainty (n = 4) were then discussed by all three reviewers. The references of the final articles were then searched for any additional relevant studies (n = 14; EvR), to follow the same review process.

Data extraction

A data extraction template was created between EvR, CC, and NH. This was prototyped on three selected papers before being applied to others. Extraction was undertaken independently by two reviewers (EvR and AW), with discrepancies discussed between the reviewers, and referred to a third author (CC) where necessary. Fields of data extraction included: country of study, design and setting, participants (number, recruitment source, parent age, child age, inclusion and exclusion criteria); information about measures of childhood maltreatment, PRF, and attachment (name of measure, reference, what it captures, descriptive scores where available, information about validation); conclusions and results relevant to our research questions (i.e., association between childhood maltreatment, PRF, and attachment); and limitations reported by the study.

Assessment of quality of studies and risk of bias

Quality of included studies (and therefore risk of bias) was assessed using the Study Quality Assessment Tools as published by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2020). Specifically, we used their quality assessment tools for case control, observational cohort, and cross-sectional studies. Quality was assessed separately (EvR and AW) at the same time as data was extracted. Discrepancies were resolved by discussion between the two reviewers (EvR and AW).

Data synthesis

Data were extracted by two researchers (EvR and AW) and compared for mismatches (by discussion) before being compiled into an overall summary table under each of the extraction fields. This information was then summarized and narratively synthesized (EvR).

Results

Study selection

The initial search retrieved 1,002 potentially relevant articles (see Figure 1). After duplicates were removed, 696 articles remained for title and abstract screening. From these, 106 articles were approved for full-text review, with 12 of these articles meeting all criteria for inclusion. A text review of the final 12 articles resulted in 14 additional full-texts sought for retrieval, of which only one study met all criteria. The total number of articles included in the final review was therefore 14, representing 11 unique studies (summarized in Table 1). There were three instances where two studies reported on the same dataset. In these instances, one of the studies was chosen to report upon depending on which study format more similarly resembled our research questions (Table 1).

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies (N = 11)

Note. Parental reflective function is represented by the acronym PRF, childhood maltreatment is represented by the acronym CM, substance use disorder is represented by the acronym SUD.

a This information was provided by the corresponding author via email and is not observable in the published manuscript.

Two studies were excluded despite meeting most criteria, due to measuring PRF prenatally (Berthelot et al., Reference Berthelot, Lemieux, Garon-Bissonnette, Lacharité and Muzik2019; Ensink, Leroux, et al., Reference Ensink, Leroux, Normandin, Biberdzic and Fonagy2017). As stated in the exclusion criteria, research suggests that PRF changes rapidly after an individual becomes a parent, and that prenatal and postnatal PRF can vary substantially within the same individual (limiting the appropriateness of comparing studies that measure PRF before versus after the child is born). The study by Kolomeyer et al. (Reference Kolomeyer, Renk, Cunningham, Lowell and Khan2016) similarly met almost all criteria; however, it was ultimately excluded because it was not possible to separate childhood maltreatment scores from other adverse childhood experiences not explicitly related to abuse or neglect by a caregiver (e.g., presence of parental mental illness, exposure to parental substance abuse, and criminal behavior). Another study by Borelli et al. (Reference Borelli, Cohen, Pettit, Normandin, Target, Fonagy and Ensink2019) met all predefined criteria; however, it was excluded due to purposely recruiting parents and children who had experienced sexual abuse, which was considered to be an unanticipated confounding factor (none of the other studies specified abuse history in the children of survivors).

Meta-analyses were not deemed appropriate due to significant heterogeneity in study design, the proportion of maltreated parents in the sample commonly being unreported (n = 4 studies), varying cutoffs for maltreatment scores (e.g., any instance of maltreatment versus moderate-severe cutoffs), and incompatible outcomes reported (discussed in-depth in study characteristics section below). It was decided that a meta-analysis of the available literature would be unlikely to be representative of the true relationship between childhood maltreatment, PRF, and attachment.

Characteristics of included studies

Characteristics of included studies are summarized in Table 1. Four of the 11 studies (36%) were conducted in the United States of America, four were conducted in Europe (two in Norway, one each from Italy and Switzerland; 36%), and one each from Australia, Canada, and Chile (27%). None of the studies recruited a population where all parents had a history of childhood maltreatment. Two studies did not report any inclusion or exclusion criteria. Of the nine studies that did report criteria, the most common exclusion criteria were current or prior serious mental illness (n = 6; 54%), followed by having a child with a disability (n = 4; 36%), substance use (n = 3; 27%), characteristics of the child’s birth (e.g., premature, or multiparity; n = 3; 27%), or the parent experiencing a serious physical health illness (n = 3; 27%).

Design

Nine of the 11 studies (82%) used a cross-sectional design and two (18%) were cross-sections within longitudinal studies. Of the cross-sectional studies, only two (18%) involved a case–control design, of which, only one was matched, while the remaining studies used cohort designs in a variety of settings (e.g., university laboratory, online questionnaires).

Participants

In total, 974 parents were included in the studies selected for review. Four studies (36%) did not report the prevalence of childhood maltreatment within their sample and measures and thresholds to determine prevalence varied considerably. Therefore, it is not possible to confidently report the number of participants who experienced childhood maltreatment within the 11 studies. These studies were still included, however, given that it is highly likely that at least 30% of their total samples included parents with a history of childhood maltreatment (the global average norm in community samples; Stoltenbergh et al., Reference Stoltenborgh, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Alink and Van Ijzendoorn2015), and that in clinical samples the proportion of parents with a history of childhood maltreatment is typically even higher than 30% (Devi et al., Reference Devi, Shahwan, Teh, Sambasivam, Zhang, Lau, Ong, Fung, Gupta, Chong and Subramaniam2019).

Of the four studies which did not report prevalence of childhood maltreatment within the parent population, two reported a range, standard deviations and mean score of childhood maltreatment which indicated it was present in high levels. Two of these four studies did not give any such indication. However, the studies were kept for inclusion due to the theoretically high likelihood of substantial levels of childhood maltreatment history being present in parents (particularly in clinical samples), and the need to generate what evidence is available in this area given that there is currently no systematic review of the relationship between childhood maltreatment history and PRF. However, this was a primary reason for not conducting a meta-analysis.

Eight studies (73%) were conducted with mothers, two (18%) with fathers, and one (0.09%) with a variety of primary caregivers. The exact population of mothers varied considerably, with four of the eight studies investigating mothers from a community sample; two examining clinical samples (one looking at parents with substance use disorder, the other borderline personality disorder features) one examining a sample of mothers with childhood maltreatment histories versus a control group; and one study examining adolescent or young adult mothers only.

The two studies investigating solely fathers involved (1) fathers who had perpetrated interpersonal violence and (2) fathers with substance use disorder (SUD) or violence issues (using a control group of fathers without SUD or violence issues). Finally, only one study examined a variety of forms of caregiving (mother, father, step-parents, grandparent, aunts, or uncles), consisting of 51.8% mothers and 40.2% fathers.

Five studies asked parents about infant-aged children (3–16 months), one looked at toddlers (19–35 months), three involved young children (2.5–8 years old), one examined older children (8 years and older), and one study did not report the mean age of children.

Measures

Only one study used a measure which explored all types of childhood maltreatment, as well as timing and frequency/severity (Traumatic Antecedents Questionnaire; Luxenberg et al., Reference Luxenberg, Spinazzola and van der Kolk2001). The most common measure of childhood maltreatment was the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia, Stokes, Handelsman, Medrano, Desmond and Zule2003; n = 5, 45%), followed by the interview version of the Childhood Experiences of Care and Abuse (Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Brown and Harris1994; n = 2, 18%). All studies employed self-report, retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment history. However, some studies only reported on total childhood maltreatment scores despite having the capacity to also examine subtypes. Of the 11 studies, seven (64%) reported only on total scores of childhood maltreatment, two (18%) reported on total scores and subtypes of childhood maltreatment, one (0.09%) examined only subtypes, and finally one (0.09%) study examined only two types of childhood maltreatment (physical abuse and sexual abuse; Moser et al., Reference Moser, Suardi, Rossignol, Vital, Manini, Serpa and Schechter2019).

Most studies (n = 10, 91%) asked about a parent’s PRF in relation to their own child (as opposed to a general measure of reflective function, or a measure that asks about the parent’s relationship with their own caregiver. Five studies (45%) used the Parent Development Interview (Slade et al., Reference Slade, Aber, Bresgi, Berger and Kaplan2004); however, the format of this measure varied substantially (e.g., from 20 to 45 items depending on the study). Three studies (27%) used the PRFQ (Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017); however, each study only reported on one subdomain of the PRFQ (either the certainty domain or the prementalization). One study assessed a parent’s reflective function with their own caregiver through the Adult Attachment Interview (George et al., Reference George, Kaplan and Main1996) with the Reflective Functioning Scale (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Target, Steele and Steele1998) applied. Only two of the 11 studies (18%) involved a measure of child attachment, with both using the Strange Situation paradigm (Ainsworth et al., Reference Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall1978).

Risk of bias

Five studies were assessed as being of “good” quality (i.e., low risk of bias) according to the NHLBI criteria (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2020). Five studies were assessed as being of “fair” quality and having some risk of bias, predominantly due to not describing results by subtypes of childhood maltreatment (“levels of exposure,” criteria 10), absence of inclusion or exclusion criteria (criteria four), or using measures without supported validity (e.g., preliminary validation or using subscales of validated measures; criteria nine). One study was assessed as being of “poor” quality in relation to our research questions, primarily due to a lack of inclusion/exclusion criteria, and inability to interpret results (labels did not match those in the method, and results regarding sexual abuse were omitted without explanation).

Narrative synthesis findings

Childhood maltreatment and PRF

Major outcomes and quality of studies are summarized in Table 2. Four of the 11 studies (36%) found a significant association between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment with PRF, while six (54%) did not. One study (0.09%) found mixed results, such that the total maltreatment score and most subtypes of maltreatment (emotional abuse, emotional neglect, and sexual abuse) were not significantly correlated with PRF, except for childhood physical abuse, which was negatively correlated with PRF.

Table 2. Major outcomes and quality rating of included studies

Note. Parental reflective function is represented by the acronym PRF, childhood maltreatment is represented by the acronym CM.

a National Heart Lung and Blood Institute measure (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2020).

Significant associations between childhood maltreatment and PRF. All four studies which found a significant association between a parent’s maltreatment history and PRF (one of these being an indirect effect; Wang, Reference Wang2021) showed a negative association (i.e., higher childhood maltreatment scores were associated with poorer PRF). Three of the four studies reported the number of parents with a history of childhood maltreatment within the larger sample, ranging from 26 to 50 participants (total sample range = 36–202 participants). Only one of these studies was conducted with a clinical population (mothers with SUD). Two of these studies examined overall childhood maltreatment scores, while the other two explored specific subtypes of childhood maltreatment.

Håkansson et al. (Reference Håkansson, Watten, Söderström, Skårderud and Øie2018) found a moderate-strong negative association between four subtypes of childhood maltreatment (emotional abuse, neglect, sexual abuse, physical abuse) and PRF, and that combined these subtypes explained 45% of the variance in PRF (see Table 2). However, this study also found that after controlling for mental health and executive function, only emotional abuse significantly predicted PRF.

Two studies found a significant association between childhood physical abuse and PRF, with one of these (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Suardi, Rossignol, Vital, Manini, Serpa and Schechter2019) noting differences in neuroimaging of brain regions associated with PRF (specifically the regions subserving empathy and emotion regulation), when comparing mothers with and without a history of childhood physical abuse. Finally, one of these studies (Wang, Reference Wang2021) did not examine whether childhood maltreatment history had a direct effect upon PRF, but did find a significant indirect effect on two subdomains of PRF (positive for prementalization, negative for interest/curiosity) via parental emotion regulation (nonsignificant indirect effect for certainty). In other words, the study by Wang (Reference Wang2021) found that higher scores of childhood maltreatment predicted difficulties with emotion regulation in parents, which in-turn predicted a higher likelihood of misattributing their child’s inner states (e.g., “they are fussy to annoy me”; prementalizing domain), a lower likelihood of being interested and curious about their child’s inner mental states, though did not predict over-certainty regarding what the child was thinking and feeling.

Nonsignificant associations between childhood maltreatment and PRF. Five of the six studies which found a nonsignificant association between childhood maltreatment and PRF showed a negative relationship. One study reported a nonsignificant positive relationships between childhood maltreatment scores and PRF (i.e., higher scores of childhood maltreatment were associated with better PRF); however, this study reportedly had few fathers with severe childhood maltreatment histories (exact prevalence not reported; Stover & Kiselica, Reference Stover and Kiselica2014).

Three of the six studies which found a nonsignificant association did not report the sample size of parents with a history of maltreatment within their overall sample. Of those that did report the number of maltreated parents in their sample (range = 26–31 participants with maltreatment history, overall sample range = 36–125 participants), all three were nonclinical populations, and one was with adolescent and young mothers (aged 14–21 years old; Riva Crugnola et al., Reference Riva Crugnola, Ierardi, Bottini, Verganti and Albizzati2019). Most of these six studies (n = 5) reported upon total childhood maltreatment scores, while one reported on both total scores and subtypes (San Cristobal et al., Reference San Cristobal, Santelices and Fuenzalida2017).

Modifiable factors

None of the included studies examined potential moderating or mediating factors between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment and their PRF. However, of the studies that found a significant association between childhood maltreatment and PRF, the following extraneous variables were found to be significantly associated with childhood maltreatment and PRF: mental health and executive function (Håkansson et al., Reference Håkansson, Watten, Söderström, Skårderud and Øie2018); alcohol and substance abuse (Mohaupt & Duckert, Reference Mohaupt and Duckert2016), and emotion regulation skills (Wang, Reference Wang2021). These variables are therefore important to investigate in future studies examining the relationship between childhood maltreatment and PRF.

The role of attachment

Only two studies measured the attachment of the child, in addition to a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment and PRF ability (Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Borelli, Roy, Normandin, Slade and Fonagy2019; Stacks et al., Reference Stacks, Muzik, Wong, Beeghly, Huth-Bocks, Irwin and Rosenblum2014). Ensink et al. (Reference Ensink, Borelli, Roy, Normandin, Slade and Fonagy2019) did not report statistical results in relation to childhood maltreatment as preliminary analyses showed no significant association with other study variables. Neither study explored the potential moderating role of PRF between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment and the attachment of their own child. Stacks et al. (Reference Stacks, Muzik, Wong, Beeghly, Huth-Bocks, Irwin and Rosenblum2014) reported a significant association between secure child attachment style and maternal PRF (see Table 2), however, no significant associations were found between child attachment style and a mother’s childhood maltreatment history. This relationship was negative, however, suggesting that there was a nonsignificant trend towards secure attachment styles of children and lower maltreatment history scores in mothers. This study also excluded mothers if they had a history of “non-prescription drugs, maternal history of bipolar or psychotic mental illness” (Stacks et al., pp. 518). The results from Stacks et al. (Reference Stacks, Muzik, Wong, Beeghly, Huth-Bocks, Irwin and Rosenblum2014) therefore suggest that there may be factors other than PRF which influence the relationship between childhood maltreatment history of parent survivors and attachment of their children, though PRF was supported as being a key skill for facilitating secure attachment in survivor’s children.

Discussion

The present study sought to systematically review and meta-analyse (1) the association between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment and their PRF, (2) whether parent survivors’ PRF predicts their child’s attachment, and (3) whether PRF moderates the association between parental history of childhood maltreatment and poorer PRF. We also sought to explore whether qualitative differences in the form of childhood maltreatment experienced (e.g., type, timing, frequency/severity) influenced these relationships. We identified 11 studies which met our criteria, however, only our first research question could be reasonably addressed, due to a lack of studies which measured child attachment in addition to parental childhood maltreatment history and PRF. A meta-analysis was not deemed appropriate due to heterogeneity in many aspects of study design (e.g., population type, level of maltreatment severity), and insufficient information about the number of participants who had experienced childhood maltreatment within the overall study samples.

Four of 11 studies found a significant association between higher levels of childhood maltreatment and poorer PRF in parent survivors of childhood maltreatment, while six found a nonsignificant association. One study found mixed associations, depending on the level of childhood maltreatment evaluated, such that total maltreatment scores did not significantly predict poorer PRF, but history of childhood physical abuse did (Mohaupt & Duckert, Reference Mohaupt and Duckert2016). However, almost all studies showed a negative association (whether significant or nonsignificant) between more severe experiences of childhood maltreatment in parents, and poorer PRF, except for one (Stover & Kiselica, Reference Stover and Kiselica2014). The study which did not find a negative relationship also did not report the prevalence of childhood maltreatment in the sample, was of "fair" quality, and the authors noted that there were few fathers with severe childhood maltreatment experiences (Stover & Kiselica, Reference Stover and Kiselica2014). This poses the possibility that a negative association does exist between higher childhood maltreatment scores and poorer PRF in parents, but that this effect is hidden by heterogeneity in study designs, small samples, and a lack of clarity regarding the exact proportion of the study populations that meet criteria for moderate-severe childhood maltreatment history. Alternatively, childhood maltreatment and PRF may be inconsistently associated, or there may be a third variable influencing this relationship that we have not yet identified.

No systematic review has been conducted on this topic for comparison; however, previous studies that have acknowledged inconsistencies in the association between parental childhood maltreatment history and PRF have suggested that one reason for this inconsistency may be over-reliance on language requirements inherent in verbal measures of PRF (Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017). Studies included in the present review all employed verbal (e.g., self-report, interview-based) measures of childhood maltreatment history and PRF. It is therefore possible that the results were biased due to a lack of nonverbal measures of PRF, as per findings regarding parental embodied mentalization (Gagné et al, Reference Gagné, Lemelin and Tarabulsy2021). Among the verbal PRF measures used, there was no clear distinction between studies that did or did not find a significant association. It is further possible that language requirements may also impact results on the self-report measures of childhood maltreatment (e.g., for parents from linguistically diverse backgrounds), and that the results of the present review should be considered within this context.

Another potential explanation for the inconsistent relationship between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment and their PRF may be that these constructs have different patterns of interaction in clinical versus nonclinical populations (Fonagy & Target, Reference Fonagy and Target1997). This does not appear to be supported by evidence within the present study, given that there were both clinical and nonclinical populations in the group that found a significant association between childhood maltreatment and PRF, as well as the group of studies that did not. Of the five studies which excluded parents with current or prior serious mental health issues, three found a nonsignificant association between childhood maltreatment and PRF, while two found a significant relationship for childhood physical abuse only. Of the three studies which excluded caregivers with substance use difficulties, two found a nonsignificant association and one found a significant association for physical abuse only (Moser et al., Reference Moser, Suardi, Rossignol, Vital, Manini, Serpa and Schechter2019; also excluded parents with serious mental health issues). The mixed results therefore do not indicate a clear role of a clinical versus nonclinical population in the pattern of association between childhood maltreatment and PRF.

An additional confounding factor in understanding the role of parent mental health is that only four of the 11 included studies measured and reported the prevalence of mental health symptoms in their population, with mixed findings about the joint versus differential effects of mental health symptoms and childhood maltreatment on PRF (Håkansson et al., Reference Håkansson, Watten, Söderström, Skårderud and Øie2018; Moser et al., Reference Moser, Suardi, Rossignol, Vital, Manini, Serpa and Schechter2019; Newman-Morris et al., Reference Newman-Morris, Simpson, Gray, Perry, Dunlop and Newman2020; Stacks et al., Reference Stacks, Muzik, Wong, Beeghly, Huth-Bocks, Irwin and Rosenblum2014). The lack of clarity regarding these factors in the research populations is a key finding, because of the possibility of mental health and substance use issues to act as proxies for poor PRF (i.e., a parent who experiences mental illness or substance use is less likely to have capacity for mind-mindedness; Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017; Håkansson et al., Reference Håkansson, Watten, Söderström, Skårderud and Øie2018). The final point is of considerable note, given that the majority of individuals who experience childhood maltreatment will also experience mental illness in their lifetime (Doyle & Cicchetti, Reference Doyle and Cicchetti2017). Despite this, six of the 11 studies in our present review excluded parents with current or prior severe mental illness from analysis.

Factors such as parent gender, study quality, measurement tool used, and analysis type (e.g., correlation versus regression) similarly did not appear to influence whether the study found a significant relationship or not between higher childhood maltreatment scores and poor PRF. Studies did appear to differ slightly in their pattern of results, however, depending on whether they reported the prevalence of childhood maltreatment within the sample or reported on subtype of childhood maltreatment instead of an overall score. The small sample number of studies again did not permit statistical analysis of these hypotheses; therefore, future studies should explore this further once more studies are available for meta-analyses.

The finding that only two of the 11 studies included a measure of childhood attachment in addition to measures of parental history of childhood maltreatment and PRF is of great importance. As mentioned, this area of research was instigated because of the theoretical importance of PRF to foster secure attachment in children of parent survivors to avoid the intergenerational transmission of insecure attachment (Fonagy & Target, Reference Fonagy and Target1997). Camoirano (Reference Camoirano2017) found in their narrative review that, while most studies reported improvements in PRF following mentalization-based interventions, this did not consistently predict secure attachment in children. For example, Sadler et al. (Reference Sadler, Slade, Close, Webb, Simpson, Fennie and Mayes2013) conducted a cluster randomized controlled trial of their PRF intervention program in a community sample of mothers and found no significant differences in improvement of PRF between the intervention group and the control. However, mothers in the intervention group did have significantly more securely attached children (i.e., even though PRF did not statistically improve, attachment of survivor’s children did). This finding highlights the importance of continually evaluating PRF and the attachment of children of parent survivors of childhood maltreatment side-by-side, to better understand if PRF is the skill which should be targeted to improve attachment outcomes or if other variables should be targeted. Inconsistencies in the association between parental childhood maltreatment history and attachment status of their children highlight that there are potentially other factors of greater importance in predicting secure attachment. It is crucial to identify these factors to ensure targeted and effective parenting programs in high-risk populations.

As previously stated, some studies which measured child attachment alongside PRF in dedicated samples of parents with a history of childhood maltreatment were excluded from the present review due to measuring PRF prenatally (Berthelot et al., Reference Berthelot, Ensink, Bernazzani, Normandin, Luyten and Fonagy2015; Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Bégin, Normandin and Fonagy2017). These two excluded studies found significantly lower PRF in mothers with a history of childhood maltreatment, and that mothers with poorer PRF were more likely to have children with insecure attachment. Due to the limited sample size, however, it is not possible to draw inferences between prenatally and postnatally measured PRF and their relationship with parental history of childhood maltreatment and child attachment. As mentioned above, these studies were excluded due to prior research which indicates that PRF can change significantly and rapidly after the birth of a child (Amos et al., Reference Amos, Segal and Cantor2015; Chamberlain, Gee, Brown, et al., Reference Chamberlain, Gee, Harfield, Campbell, Brennan, Clark, Mensah, Arabena, Herrman, Brown, Atkinson, Nicholson, Gartland, Glover, Mitchell, Atkinson, McLachlan, Andrews, Hirvoven and Dyall2019), and that prenatal and postnatal PRF can not only predict different outcomes but consist of different substructures (Smaling et al., Reference Smaling, Huijbregts, van der Heijden, van Goozen and Swaab2016). Another potential explanation for inconsistent findings in the present systematic review may therefore also be due to different components of PRF being reported upon (e.g., certainty domain of PRF as reported in Milan et al., Reference Milan, Carlone, Printz and Perez2020).

Without a clear understanding of the association between childhood maltreatment and PRF, it is not possible or useful to examine the potential moderating role of PRF (or other modifiable factors) between parental history of childhood maltreatment and insecure attachment of survivors’ children. It may be that other factors relating to the experience of becoming a parent are more important in determining whether intergenerational trauma is perpetuated (e.g., Piccini, Reference Piccini2021). However, it would appear premature to disregard PRF as a candidate mechanism without addressing important inconsistencies in research in this area. Key inconsistencies identified in the present review which may influence inconsistent results include: level of childhood maltreatment reported, language-based measurements of PRF, a lack of reporting sample size of maltreated parents within the greater study population, and including participants with mental health difficulties as this is representative of the true population of parents with a history of childhood maltreatment.

Further, no study in the present review examined the role of timing of childhood maltreatment on the association between childhood maltreatment scores and PRF. This is of note, given that childhood maltreatment is a research topic due to the established importance of traumatic events during the ‘sensitive’ periods of development (e.g., Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2016). The mixed findings regarding the association between parental history of childhood maltreatment and PRF are an important contribution to literature in this area, especially considering that PRF-based parenting interventions are widely, and by many accounts, effectively employed (Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017).

The results of this review should be considered within the context of limitations of the included studies. There was substantial heterogeneity in most aspects of study design (e.g., populations, sample sizes, measures). However, these inconsistencies provide an important base for future research. Future studies will allow for the potential of meta-analysis if they use measures of childhood maltreatment which capture type, timing, and frequency (and report on each aspect); report the exact proportion of participants with a history of childhood maltreatment (and the cutoffs used to determine this); explain why variations of established measures were used (e.g., the Parental Development Interview); report on all subdomains of PRF; include parents with mental illness to have a representative sample of childhood maltreatment survivors; and include validated measurements of childhood attachment alongside those of childhood maltreatment and PRF (ideally comparing postnatal and prenatal samples). Findings from the present study have also shown the need for structural exploration of the mediating or moderating effects of various confounding factors, including mental health. Further, future studies are strongly encouraged to incorporate nonverbal measures of PRF, such as parental embodied mentalization (Afek et al., Reference Afek., Lev-Wiesel, Federman and Shai.2022), to ensure that this skill is accurately captured and mechanisms further understood.

Finally, there is a severe lack of representation of fathers in this literature, and no representation of Indigenous families who have been shown to be disproportionality effected by intergenerational trauma (Chamberlain et al., Reference Chamberlain, Gray, Gee and Atkinson2022).

Conclusion

Intergenerational trauma is a pervasive societal issue that creates significant economic, psychological, and medical burden (Chamberlain, Gee, Harfield, et al., Reference Chamberlain, Gee, Harfield, Campbell, Brennan, Clark, Mensah, Arabena, Herrman, Brown, Atkinson, Nicholson, Gartland, Glover, Mitchell, Atkinson, McLachlan, Andrews, Hirvoven and Dyall2019). The mechanisms which perpetuate the intergenerational transmission of trauma consequences remain poorly understood, although PRF has been suggested as a candidate and is incorporated into current interventions. Findings from this systematic review have shown mixed evidence for an association between a parent’s history of childhood maltreatment and later PRF, however, this should be interpreted within the context of extreme heterogeneity of included studies. A very important finding is that many studies excluded parents with current or previous serious mental health issues, despite the reality that a significant proportion of survivors of childhood maltreatment report mental health issues. The present review was not able to comment on the role of PRF in the intergenerational transmission of insecure attachment, due to the quality and current availability of evidence. Future studies should adopt lessons from the present review to facilitate replicable research and more in-depth systematic analyses in the future that would lend themselves to meta-analysis. Should PRF be conclusively excluded as a candidate mechanism in intergenerational trauma (either in clinical, nonclinical populations, or both), other candidate mechanisms should be emphasized and current parenting programs employing PRF re-examined. We hope that future research will assist in answering these questions.

Funding statement

This research was carried out while the primary author (Elmie Janse van Rensburg) was in receipt of an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship at the University of Western Australia and a Stan and Jean Perron Top-Up Scholarship.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Aber, J. L., Slade, A., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., & Kaplan, M. (1985). The parent development interview. Unpublished manual. The Psychological Center of the City College of New York.Google Scholar
Afek., E., Lev-Wiesel, R., Federman, D., & Shai., D. (2022). The mediating role of parental embodied mentalizing in the longitudinal association between prenatal spousal support and toddler emotion recognition. Infancy, 27(3), 609629. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12462 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: a psychological study of the Strange Situation,Google Scholar
Albizzati, A., Ierardi, E., Moioli, M., Crugnola, C. R., Territoriale, U., Santi, A., Psicologia, D., & Milano-bicocca, U. (2020). Profilo psicopatologico e di rischio psicosociale in madri adolescenti e di giovane età nel postpartum. Medico e Bambino, 39(7), 445451.10.53126/MEB39445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amos, J., Segal, L., & Cantor, C. (2015). Entrapped mother, entrapped child: Agonic mode, hierarchy and appeasement in intergenerational abuse and neglect. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(5), 14421450. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-014-9950-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anis, L., Perez, G., Benzies, K. M., Ewashen, C., Hart, M., & Letourneau, N. (2020). Convergent validity of three measures of reflective function: Parent development interview, parental reflective function questionnaire, and reflective function questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(December), 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574719 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashton, C. K., O’Brien-Langer, A., & Silverstone, P. H. (2016). The CASA Trauma and Attachment Group (TAG) program for developmental trauma. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(1), 3542.Google ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, D., & Fink, L. (1998). Childhood trauma questionnaire: A retrospective self-report. The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., Stokes, J., Handelsman, L., Medrano, M., Desmond, D., Zule, W. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse and Neglect, 27(2), 169190. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthelot, N., Ensink, K., Bernazzani, O., Normandin, L., Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of attachment in abused and neglected mothers: The role of trauma-specific reflective functioning. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36(2), 200212. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21499 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthelot, N., Lemieux, R., Garon-Bissonnette, J., Lacharité, C., & Muzik, M. (2019). The protective role of mentalizing: Reflective functioning as a mediator between child maltreatment, psychopathology and parental attitude in expecting parents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 95(April 2019), 104065. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104065 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bethell, C., Gombojav, N., Solloway, M., & Wissow, L. (2016). Adverse childhood experiences, resilience and mindfulness-based approaches: Common denominator issues for children with emotional, mental, or behavioral problems. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 25(2), 139156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2015.12.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bifulco, A., Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1994). Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse (CECA): A retrospective interview measure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(8), 14191435.10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01284.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borelli, J. L., Cohen, C., Pettit, C., Normandin, L., Target, M., Fonagy, P., & Ensink, K. (2019). Maternal and child sexual abuse history: An intergenerational exploration of children’s adjustment and maternal trauma-reflective functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(May), 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01062 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. Routledge.Google Scholar
Byrne, G., Sleed, M., Midgley, N., Fearon, P., Mein, C., Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2019). Lighthouse Parenting Programme: Description and pilot evaluation of mentalization-based treatment to address child maltreatment. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 680693. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104518807741 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camoirano, A. (2017). Mentalizing makes parenting work: A review about parental reflective functioning and clinical interventions to improve it. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(JAN), 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, A., Duff, H., & Craddock, F. (2020). A systematic review of reviews of the outcome of noninstitutional child maltreatment. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 21(4), 828843, https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838018801334,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Center for Disease and Prevention. (2003). ACE reporter: Origins and essence of the study. Retrieved from https://www.ashlandmhrb.org/upload/origins_and_essence_of_the_ace_study.pdf Google Scholar
Chamberlain, C., Gee, G., Brown, S. J., Atkinson, J., Herrman, H., Gartland, D., Glover, K., Clark, Y., Campbell, S., Mensah, F. K., Atkinson, C., Brennan, S. E., McLachlan, H., Hirvonen, T., Dyall, D., Ralph, N., Hokke, S., Nicholson, J. (2019). Healing the past by nurturing the future - Co-designing perinatal strategies for Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander parents experiencing complex trauma: Framework and protocol for a community-based participatory action research study. BMJ Open, 9(6), 111. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-028397 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, C., Gee, G., Harfield, S., Campbell, S., Brennan, S., Clark, Y., Mensah, F., Arabena, K., Herrman, H., Brown, S., Atkinson, J., Nicholson, J., Gartland, D., Glover, K., Mitchell, A., Atkinson, C., McLachlan, H., Andrews, S., Hirvoven, T., …, & Dyall, D. (2019). Parenting after a history of childhood maltreatment: A scoping review and map of evidence in the perinatal period. PLoS ONE, 14(3), e0213460, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213460,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, C., Gray, P., Gee, G., & Atkinson, J. (2022). Community views on ‘Can perinatal services safely identify Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander parents experiencing complex trauma?’ Child Abuse Review, 32(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1002/car.2760 Google Scholar
Cohen, P. (2016). Dyadic treatment, reflective functioning, and videofeedback: Fostering attachment with families in the foster care system. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 15(2), 104119. https://doi.org/10.1080/15289168.2016.1167412 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devi, F., Shahwan, S., Teh, W. L., Sambasivam, R., Zhang, Y. J., Lau, Y. W., Ong, S. H., Fung, D., Gupta, B., Chong, S. A., Subramaniam, M. (2019). The prevalence of childhood trauma in psychiatric outpatients. Annals of General Psychiatry, 18(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12991-019-0239-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobrescu, A. I., Nussbaumer-Streit, B., Klerings, I., Wagner, G., Persad, E., Sommer, I., Herkner, H., & Gartlehner, G. (2021). Restricting evidence syntheses of interventions to English-language publications is a viable methodological shortcut for most medical topics: A systematic review. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 137, 209217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.04.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doyle, C., & Cicchetti, D. (2017). From the cradle to the grave: The effect of adverse caregiving environments on attachment and relationships throughout the lifespan. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 24(2), 203217. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12192 Google Scholar
Ensink, K., Bégin, M., Normandin, L., & Fonagy, P. (2017). Parental reflective functioning as a moderator of child internalizing difficulties in the context of child sexual abuse. Psychiatry Research, 257(June), 361366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.051 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Borelli, J. L., Roy, J., Normandin, L., Slade, A., & Fonagy, P. (2019). Costs of not getting to know you: Lower levels of parental reflective functioning confer risk for maternal insensitivity and insecure infant attachment. International Congress of Infant Studies, 24(2), 210227. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12263 Google Scholar
Ensink, K., Leroux, A., Normandin, L., Biberdzic, M., & Fonagy, P. (2017). Assessing reflective parenting in interaction with school-aged children. Journal of Personality Assessment, 99(6), 585595. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2016.1270289 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Normandin, L., Plamondon, A., Berthelot, N., & Fonagy, P. (2016). Intergenerational pathways from reflective functioning to infant attachment through parenting. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 48(1), 918. https://doi.org/10.1037/cbs0000030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., Koss, M. P., & Marks, J. S. (2019). REPRINT OF: Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 56(6), 774786. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2019.04.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1998). Reflective functioning scale manual,Google Scholar
Fonagy, Peter, & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in self-organization. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 679700. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579497001399 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gagné, K., Lemelin, J., & Tarabulsy, G. (2021). Non-verbal and verbal parent mentalization as predictors of infant attachment security: Contributions of parental embodied mentalizing and mind-mindedness and the mediating role of maternal sensitivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 65, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101622 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
George, C., Kaplan, M., & Main, M. (1996). Adult attachment interview,Google Scholar
Håkansson, U., Watten, R., Söderström, K., Skårderud, F., & Øie, M. G. (2018). Adverse and adaptive childhood experiences are associated with parental reflective functioning in mothers with substance use disorder. Child Abuse and Neglect, 81(May), 259273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.05.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horswell, B. B., & Istfan, S. (2006). Preventing child maltreatment: A guide to taking action and generating evidence. World Health Organization, 24(3), 511517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coms.2012.04.002 Google Scholar
Huth-Bocks, A., Muzik, M., Beeghly, M., Earls, L., & Stack, A. (2014). Secure base scripts are associated with maternal parenting behavior across contexts and reflective functioning among trauma-exposed mothers. Attachment & Human Development, 16(6), 535556. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.967787 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Infurna, M., Reichl, C., Parzer, P., Schimmenti, A., Bifulco, A., & Kaess, M. (2016). Associations between depression and specific childhood experiences of abuse and neglect: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 190, 4755. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iyengar, U., Rajhans, P., Fonagy, P., Strathearn, L., & Kim, S. (2019). Unresolved trauma and reorganization in mothers: Attachment and neuroscience perspectives. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(Jan), 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00110 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, Y., McGuire, A., Tunno, A. M., & Makanui, P. K. (2019). A reasonably large review of operationalization in child maltreatment research: Assessment approaches and sources of information in youth samples. Child Abuse and Neglect, 87(September 2018), 517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.09.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolomeyer, E., Renk, K., Cunningham, A., Lowell, A., & Khan, M. (2016). Mothers’ adverse childhood experiences and negative parenting behaviors connecting mothers’ difficult pasts to present parenting behavior via reflective functioning. Zero to Three, 513, September.Google Scholar
Kristiansen, V., Handeland, T., Lau, B., Soderstrom, K., Hakansson, U., & Oie, M. (2020). Addictive Behaviors Reports Trauma in childhood and adolescence and impaired executive functions are associated with uncertain reflective functioning in mothers with substance use disorder. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 11(December 2019), 100245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2019.100245 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kubany, E.S., Haynes, S.N., Leisen, M.B., Owens, J.A., Kaplan, A.S., Watson, S.B., Burns, K. (2000). Development and preliminary validation of a brief broad-spectrum measure of trauma exposure: the traumatic life events questionnaire. Psychological Assessment 12, 210224.10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. W., Bae, G. Y., Rim, H. D., Lee, S. J., Chang, S. M., Kim, B. S., & Won, S. (2018). Mediating effect of resilience on the association between emotional neglect and depressive symptoms. Psychiatry Investigation, 15(1), 6269. https://doi.org/10.4306/pi.2018.15.1.62 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luxenberg, T., Spinazzola, J., & van der Kolk, B. (2001). Complex trauma and the Disorders of Extreme Stress (DESNOS) diagnosis, part one: Assessment. Directions in Psychiatry, 11, 373393.Google Scholar
Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (2015). The neurobiology of mentalizing. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 6(4), 366379. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000117 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Mayes, L. C., Nijssens, L., & Fonagy, P. (2017). The parental reflective functioning questionnaire: Development and preliminary validation. PLoS ONE, 12(5), 128. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0176218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Mayes, L. C., Sadler, L., Fonagy, P., Nicholls, S., Crowley, M., & Slade, A. (2009). The parental reflective functioning questionnaire-1 (PRFQ-1). University of Leuven.Google Scholar
Marshall, R.D., Schneier, F.R., Fallon, B.A., Knight, C.B., Abbate, L.A., Goetz, D.E.A. (1998). An open trial for paroxetine in patients with noncombat-related, chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 18, 10–18.10.1097/00004714-199802000-00003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messman-Moore, T. L., & Bhuptani, P. H. (2017). A review of the long-term impact of child maltreatment on posttraumatic stress disorder and its comorbidities: An emotion dysregulation perspective. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 24(2), 154169. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12193 Google Scholar
Milan, S., Carlone, C., Printz, D., & Perez, S. D. (2020). Understanding children’s emotions: Differences in mothers with a history of childhood maltreatment. Child Maltreatment, 27(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559520972188 Google ScholarPubMed
Mohaupt, H., & Duckert, F. (2016). Parental reflective functioning in fathers who use intimate partner violence : Findings from a Norwegian clinical sample. Nordic Psychology, 2276(4), 115. https://doi.org/10.1080/19012276.2016.1162107 Google Scholar
Montgomery, E., Just-Østergaard, E., & Jervelund, S. S. (2019). Transmitting trauma: A systematic review of the risk of child abuse perpetrated by parents exposed to traumatic events. International Journal of Public Health, 64(2), 241251. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00038-018-1185-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, D. A., Suardi, F., Rossignol, A. S., Vital, M., Manini, A., Serpa, S. R., & Schechter, D. S. (2019). Parental Reflective Functioning correlates to brain activation in response to video-stimuli of mother-child dyads: Links to maternal trauma history and PTSD. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging, 293(September), 110985. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2019.09.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman-Morris, V., Simpson, K., Gray, K. M., Perry, N., Dunlop, A., & Newman, L. K. (2020). Evaluation of early relational disturbance in high-risk populations: Borderline personality disorder features, maternal mental state, and observed interaction. Infant Mental Health Journal, 41(6), 793810. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21880 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nijenhuis, E. R., Van Der Hart, O., & Kruger, K. (2002). The psychometric characteristics of the traumatic experiences checklist (TEC). First findings among psychiatric outpatients. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 9(3), 200210.10.1002/cpp.332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J. M., Akl, E. A., Brennan, S. E., Chou, R., Glanville, J., Grimshaw, J. M., Hróbjartsson, A., Lalu, M. M., Li, T., Loder, E. W., Mayo-Wilson, E., McDonald, S., …, & Moher, D. (2021). The PRISMA. 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ, 372, 19, https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71,Google ScholarPubMed
Pajulo, M., Tolvanen, M., Pyykkönen, N., Karlsson, L., Mayes, L., & Karlsson, H. (2018). Exploring parental mentalization in postnatal phase with a self-report questionnaire (PRFQ): Factor structure, gender differences and association with sociodemographic factors. The Finn Brain Birth Cohort Study. Psychiatry Research 262(August 2017), 431–439. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piccini, O. (2021). The mother’s body, the role of pleasure in the mother-infant relationship, and the traumatic risk. International Forum of Psychoanalysis, 30(3), 129138. https://doi.org/10.1080/0803706X.2021.1946140 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riva Crugnola, C., Ierardi, E., Bottini, M., Verganti, C., & Albizzati, A. (2019). Childhood experiences of maltreatment, reflective functioning and attachment in adolescent and young adult mothers: Effects on mother-infant interaction and emotion regulation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 93(August 2018), 277290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.03.024 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sadler, L. S., Slade, A., Close, N., Webb, D. L., Simpson, T., Fennie, K., & Mayes, L. C. (2013). Minding the baby: Enhancing reflectiveness to improve early health and relationship outcomes in an interdisciplinary home-visiting program. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34(5), 391405. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21406 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
San Cristobal, P., Santelices, M., & Fuenzalida, D. (2017). Manifestation of trauma: The effect of early traumatic experiences and adult attachment on parental reflective functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(March), 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00449 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shai, D., & Belsky, J. (2011). When words just won’t do: Introducing parental embodied mentalizing. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 173180. https://doi.org/8606.2011.00181.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheridan, M. A., Shi, F., Miller, A. B., Salhi, C., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2020). Network structure reveals clusters of associations between childhood adversities and development outcomes. Developmental Science, 23(5), 116. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12934 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment and Human Development, 7(3), 269281. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slade, A., Aber, J., Bresgi, I., Berger, B., & Kaplan, M. (2004). The parent development interview – Revised.Google Scholar
Smaling, H. J. A., Huijbregts, S. C. J., van der Heijden, K. B., van Goozen, S. H. M., & Swaab, H. (2016). Maternal reflective functioning as a multidimensional construct: Differential associations with children’s temperament and externalizing behavior. Infant Behavior and Development, 44, 263274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.06.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stacks, A. M., Muzik, M., Wong, K., Beeghly, M., Huth-Bocks, A., Irwin, J. L., & Rosenblum, K. L. (2014). Maternal reflective functioning among mothers with childhood maltreatment histories: Links to sensitive parenting and infant attachment security. Attachment & Human Development, 16(5), 515533. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2014.935452 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stob, V., Slade, A., Brotnow, L., Adnopoz, J., & Woolston, J. (2019). The family cycle: An activity to enhance parents’ mentalization in children’s mental health treatment. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 18(2), 103119. https://doi.org/10.1080/15289168.2019.1591887 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoltenborgh, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Alink, L. R., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2015). The prevalence of child maltreatment across the globe: Review of a series of meta-analyses. Child Abuse Review, 24(1), 3750. https://doi.org/10.1002/car CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stover, C. S., & Kiselica, A. (2014). Reflective functioning in fathers: An initial empirical examination. Infant Mental Health Journal, 35(5), 452461. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21459.Reflective CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teicher, M. H., & Samson, J. A. (2016). Annual Research Review: Enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 57, 241266, https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12507,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, S. L., & Manly, J. T. (2019). Developmental consequences of child abuse and neglect: Implications for intervention. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 5964. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12317 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
U.S. Department of Health & Human Services 2020). Study quality and assessment tools. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute.Google Scholar
Van der Kolk, B. A., Spinazzola, J., & Hopper, J. (1995). Traumatic Antecedents Questionnaire (TAQ). Boston.Google Scholar
Wang, X. (2021). Child abuse & neglect intergenerational effects of childhood maltreatment : The roles of parents’ emotion regulation and mentalization. Child Abuse & Neglect, 128, 104940. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2021.104940 2020-11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widom, C. S., Czaja, S. J., Kozakowski, S. S., & Chauhan, P. (2018). Does adult attachment style mediate the relationship between childhood maltreatment and mental and physical health outcomes? Child Abuse and Neglect, 76(May 2017), 533545. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.05.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods-Jaeger, B. A., Cho, B., Sexton, C. C., Slagel, L., & Goggin, K. (2018). Promoting resilience: Breaking the intergenerational cycle of adverse childhood experiences. Health Education and Behavior, 45(5), 772780. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198117752785 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization 2016). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems, 10th revision (ICD-10). World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Zeanah, C., Larrieu, JA, Heller, SS, & Vallier, J. (2000). Infant-parent relationship assessment. In Press, G. (Ed.), Handbook of Infant Mental Health (pp. 222–235). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Flowchart for the inclusion of articles. Note. Parental reflective function is represented by the acronym PRF.

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies (N = 11)

Figure 2

Table 2. Major outcomes and quality rating of included studies