Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:53:00.577Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Development of autobiographical memory in children with autism spectrum disorders: Deficits, gains, and predictors of performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 November 2013

Lorna Goddard*
Affiliation:
University of London, London
Barbara Dritschel
Affiliation:
University of St. Andrews, London
Sally Robinson
Affiliation:
Kings College, London
Patricia Howlin
Affiliation:
Kings College, London
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Lorna Goddard, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, New Cross Gate, London SE14 6NW, UK; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Autobiographical memory (AM) was assessed in 63 children (aged 8–17 years) with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and compared with 63 typically developing children matched for age, gender, IQ, and verbal ability. A range of methodologies was employed for eliciting past experience with particular focus on the ability to recall (a) specific events, (b) the recent and remote past, and (c) semantic versus episodic memories across different lifetime periods. Results indicated that the ASD group manifested difficulties in retrieving specific memories to word cues and had poorer access to the remote past. Deficits were found in the context of intact recent memory and preserved general memory abilities, with some impairment of visual memory. Problems in retrieving episodic and semantic AMs across the life span were also evident. Qualitative analysis of memory reports suggested that the ASD group was less likely to refer to emotion in their remote memories but more likely to describe emotions in their recent memories. Important predictors of AM performance in the ASD group were central executive abilities, in particular cognitive flexibility and verbal fluency.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, D. K., Oti, R. S., Lord, C., & Welch, K. (2009). Patterns of growth in adaptive social abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 10191034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind and autism: A review. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 169184.Google Scholar
Bekerian, D., Dhillon, D., & O'Neill, M. H. (2001). The Children's Autobiographical Memory Interview. Paper presented at the 3rd International Conference on Memory, Valencia, Spain.Google Scholar
Ben Shalom, D. (2003). Memory in autism: Review and synthesis. Cortex, 39, 11291138.Google Scholar
Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444451.Google Scholar
Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Berthollier, N. (2004). Source memory in Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 34, 533542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Grice, S. J. (2000). Episodic memory and remembering in adults with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 295304.Google Scholar
Brewer, W. F. (1986). What is autobiographical memory? In Rubin, D. C. (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 2549). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brittlebank, A. D., Scott, J., Williams, J. M. G., & Ferrier, I. N. (1993). Autobiographical memory in depression: State or trait marker? British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 118121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, B. T., Morris, G., Nida, R. E., & Baker-Ward, L. (2012). Brief Report: Making experience personal: Internal states language in the memory narratives of children with and without Asperger's disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 42, 441446.Google Scholar
Bruck, M., London, K., Landa, R., & Goodman, J. (2007). Autobiographical memory and suggestibility in children with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 7395.Google Scholar
Crane, L., & Goddard, L. (2008). Episodic and semantic autobiographical memory in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 498506.Google Scholar
Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2009). Specific and general autobiographical knowledge in adults with autism spectrum disorders: The role of personal goals. Memory, 17, 557576.Google Scholar
Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2010). Brief Report: Self-defining memories and everyday autobiographical memories in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 383391.Google Scholar
Cohen, M. (1997). Children's Memory Scale. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 497505.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000a). Neuroanatomical mapping of true and false autobiographical memories. International Journal of Psychology, 35(3–4), 113.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000b). The construction of autobiographical memory in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261288.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., Pleydell-Pearce, C. W., & Whitecross, S. E. (2001). The neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A slow cortical potential study of autobiographical memory retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 493524.Google Scholar
Crovitz, H. F., & Schiffman, H. (1974). Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 517518.Google Scholar
Culbertson, W. C., & Zillmer, E. A (2005). Tower of LondonDX (2nd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Dalgleish, T., Williams, J. M. G., Perkins, N., Golden, A. J., Barnard, P. J., Au-Yeung, C., et al. (2007) Reduced specificity of autobiographical memory and depression: The role of executive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 2342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dissanayake, C., Shembrey, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2010) Delayed video self-recognition in children with high Vo functioning autism and Asperger's disorder. Autism, 14, 5, 495508.Google Scholar
Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., Goddard, L., Robinson, S., & Howlin, P. (2010). Judgements of self-understanding in adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 14, 5, 509518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British Picture Vocabulary Scale (2nd ed.). London: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2006). Parent–child reminiscing locates the self in past. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 235251.Google Scholar
Friedman, N., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101135.Google Scholar
Gilboa, A, Winocur, G., Grady, C., Hevenor, S. J., & Moscovitch, M. (2004). Remembering our past: Functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very remote events. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 12141225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., & Burton, A. (1996) Role of autobiographical memory in social problem-solving and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 609615.Google Scholar
Goddard, L., Howlin, P., Dritschel, B., & Patel, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory and social problem-solving in young adults with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 37, 291300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. (2008). Brief Report: Narratives of personal events in children with autism and developmental language disorders: Unshared memories. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 38, 19821988.Google Scholar
Gunter, H. L., Ghaziuddin, M., & Ellis, H. D. (2002) Asperger syndrome: Tests of right hemisphere functioning and interhemispheric communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 32, 263281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R. K. (2003). WCST-64: Computerized Version 2, Research Edition [Computer software]. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Heeren, A., Van Block, N., & Philpot, P. (2009). The effect of mindfulness on executive processes and autobiographical memory specificity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 403409.Google Scholar
Hewitt, J., Evans, J. J., & Dritschel, B. (2006). Theory driven rehabilitation of executive functioning: Improving planning skills in people with traumatic brain injury through the use of an autobiographical episodic memory cueing procedure. Neuropsycholgia, 44, 14681474.Google Scholar
Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 2632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, E. L., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: Patterns of performance in multiple case series. Neuropsychologica, 44, 28222835.Google Scholar
Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1997) The emergence and early development of autobiographical memory. Psychological Review, 104, 499523.Google Scholar
Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004) Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 212229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, S., Chan, R. L., & Loftus, J. (1999). Independence of episodic and semantic self-knowledge: The case from autism. Social Cognition, 17, 413436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinhans, N. M., Richards, T., Weavers, K., Clark Johnson, L., Greenson, J., Dawson, G., et al. (2005). Association between amygdala response to emotional faces and social anxiety in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 48, 36653670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, L.-C., David, A. B., Rusyniak, J., Landa, R., & Newschaffer, C. J. (2007). Performance of the Social Communication Questionnaire in children receiving preschool special educational services. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, 126138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. J., Lee, H.-K., Kweon, Y.-S., Lee, C. T., & Lee, K.-U. (2009). The impact of executive function on emotion recognition and emotion experience in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Investigation, 6, 156162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lind, S., & Bowler, D. (2009). Delayed recognition in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 643650.Google Scholar
Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2003). Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 239251.Google Scholar
McDowd, J., Hoffman, L., Rosek, E., Lyons, K. E., Pahwa, R., Burns, J. et al. (2011). Understanding verbal fluency in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology, 25, 210225CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milders, M., Ietswaat, M., Crawford, J. R., & Currie, D. (2008). Social behaviour following traumatic brain injury and its association with emotion recognition, understanding of intentions and cognitive flexibility. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 318326.Google Scholar
Millward, C., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (2000). Recall for self and other in autism: Children's memory for events experienced by themselves and their peers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 1528.Google Scholar
Mitchell, P., & O'Keefe, K. (2008). Brief report: Do individuals with autism spectrum disorder think they know their own minds? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 15911597.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 714.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2000) Socialisation of memory. In Tulving, E., & Craik, F. J. M. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory (pp. 283295). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
O'Connor, M., Butters, N., Miliotis, P., Eslinger, P., & Cermak, L. S. (1992). The dissociation of anterograde and retrograde amnesia in a patient with herpes–encephalitis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 159178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogden, J. A. (1993). Visual object agnosia, prosopagnosia, achromatopsia, loss of visual imagery and autobiographical amnesia following recovery from cortical blindness: Case M.H. Neuropsychologia, 31, 571589.Google Scholar
O'Shea, A. G., Fein, D. A., Cillessen, A. H. N., Klin, A., & Schultz, R. T. (2005). Source memory in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 337360.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Allied Disciplines, 32, 10811105.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Kloo, D., & Gornik, E. (2007). Episodic memory development: Theory of mind is part of re-experiencing experienced events. Infant & Child Development, 16, 471490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (1995). Infants' insight into the mind: How deep? Science, 308, 214–126.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., Warren, K. L., & Short, M. M. (2011). Infantile amnesia across the years: A 2-year follow-up of children's earliest memories. Child Development, 82, 10921105.Google Scholar
Rabbitt, P., & Winthorpe, C. (1988). What do old people remember? The Galton Paradigm reconsidered. In Gruneberg, M. M., Morris, P. E., & Sykes, R. N. (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Vol. 1. Current research and issues (pp. 301307). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Raes, F., Verstraeten, K., Bijttebier, P., Vasey, M. W., & Dalgleish, T. (2010). Inhibitory control mediates the relationship between depressed mood and overgeneral recall in children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 276281.Google Scholar
Reese, E. (1999). What children say when they talk about the past. Narrative Inquiry, 9, 215241.Google Scholar
Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., & Howlin, P. (2009). Executive functions in children with autism spectrum disorder. Brain and Cognition, 71, 363368.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., LeCouteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autistic Diagnostic Interview—Revised (ADI-R). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1996). The domina of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society, 351B, 14051411.Google Scholar
Shallice, T., Marzocchi, G. M., Coser, S., Del Savio, M., Meuter, R. F., & Rumiati, R. I. (2002). Executive function profile of children with attention deficits hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 4371.Google Scholar
St. Jacques, P. L., & Levine, B. (2007). Ageing and autobiographical memory for emotional and neutral events. Memory, 15, 129144.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643662.Google Scholar
Valentino, K., Bridgett, D. J., Hayden, L. C., & Nuttall, A. K. (2012). Abuse, depressive symptoms, executive functioning and overgeneral memory among a psychiatric sample of children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 491498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Welch-Ross, M. K. (1997). Mother–child participation in conversation about the past: Relationship to pre-schooler's theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 33, 618629.Google Scholar
Williams, D. (2010). Theory of own mind in autism: Evidence of a specific deficit in self awareness? Autism, 14, 474494.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G., & Dritschel, B. (1992). Categoric and extended autobiographical memories. In Conway, M. A., Rubin, D. C., Spinnler, H., & Wagenaar, W. A. (Eds.), Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory (pp. 391412). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G., Healy, H. G., & Ellis, N. C. (1999). The effect of imageability and predictability of cues in autobiographical memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 555579.Google Scholar