Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T00:43:57.297Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Review of supporting and refuting evidence for Innovation Engineering practices

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2015

Ping Du
Affiliation:
Mechanical Engineering, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50011, USA
Chris Miller
Affiliation:
Mechanical Engineering, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC 27695, USA
Erin MacDonald*
Affiliation:
Mechanical Engineering, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
Paul Gormley
Affiliation:
CIRAS, Economic Development and Industrial Relations, Iowa State University, Cedar Rapids, IA 52402, USA
*
Email address for correspondence: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Innovation Engineering (IE) is an educational training program that presents tools and advice on product innovation in three main categories: Create, idea generation; Communicate, communicating ideas; and Commercialize, selecting ideas to invest in further. The concepts taught in IE include common suggestions for early-stage product innovation. This paper addresses a challenge of implementing the IE program, specifically that it does not provide peer-reviewed sources or adequate data to substantiate its approach. This lack of substantiation limits effective implementation at companies. This paper also takes a step in examining IE’s claims that it is ‘a new science’ and a ‘new field of academic study’, a topic motivated by the Design Science Journal’s aim to serve as the archival venue of science-based design knowledge across multiple disciplines. This paper provides a compilation of academic literature that has tested the tools and advice espoused by IE. Almost all included papers contain test-versus-control experimental evidence. A mix of supporting and refuting evidence was found. Overall, the work provides a useful compilation of evidence-of-effectiveness related to common innovation and design practices that spans different design stages and is applicable for multiple disciplines and industries. This evidence comes from a variety of sources, including design, engineering education, psychology, marketing, and management. The work can also serve as an approach to evaluate overarching approaches to design in general, specifically, testing the foundations by vetting related test-versus-control experimental studies.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
Distributed as Open Access under a CC-BY 4.0 license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2015

1. Introduction

Innovation is widely accepted as a major force of growth in national economies (Kleinknecht Reference Kleinknecht1996; Brouwer Reference Brouwer1997; Klomp & Leeuwen Reference Klomp and Leeuwen1999; Rosenberg Reference Rosenberg2004; Wong, Ho & Autio Reference Wong, Ho and Autio2005; Cohen Reference Cohen2012), and the current president of the United States speaks that ‘[t]he first step in winning the future is encouraging American innovation’ (WhiteHousend n.d.). The production of technological innovations has historically led to economic prosperity, and a new economic theory known as Innovation Economics has emerged (Atkinson & Ezell Reference Atkinson and Ezell2012). The general definition of innovation is simply the process of introducing new ideas (Merriam-Webster.com 2014b ). In this paper, we view innovation as offering meaningful uniqueness to facilitate economic growth (Eureka! Ranch n.d.; ed. Wessner Reference Wessner2013), the framing provided by the U.S. National Institute of Standards and Technology (NIST) Hollings Manufacturing Extension Partnership (MEP), which considers innovation as a driver for economic growth (ed. Wessner Reference Wessner2013). For an innovation to spur economic growth, it must be more than just new—it must also impart financial worth. An innovation without meaningfulness to the intended audience is likely to be unsuccessful. For example, in design and engineering, new products or technologies require customers to see them as important enough to purchase and adopt.

To further articulate our definition of innovation, we compare it to the term invention. The Merriam-Webster dictionary (Merriam-Webster.com 2015a ) has a number of definitions for invention that focus on discovery, finding, and imagination, but do not include the concept of economic growth or meaningfulness. Innovation and invention share the commonality of uniqueness. Invention can be important to innovation, but innovation can also exist without invention. For example, combining two different functionalities into one product can be innovative, but is not inventive. Additionally, someone can invent something that is not innovative, if that invention is not meaningful and does not provide the opportunity for economic growth. The concepts suggested by Innovation Engineering (IE) may also help with invention, but the goal of the program is to generate ideas that are unique, meaningful, and have the potential to drive economic growth.

Innovation Engineering began in 2005 (Hall & Graduates of the Innovation Engineering Leadership Institute n.d.), and it offers a set of tools and advice that comprise a systematic approach to innovation, akin to the training program Six Sigma for process improvement (Mehrjerdi Reference Mehrjerdi2011). Innovation Engineering also has similarly tiered levels of certification. From approximately 2010 to 2013, IE was a primary tool utilized by the United States Government’s NIST MEP. Associated consultants in all 50 states were trained in IE to help small- and mid-sized manufacturers produce innovative products. The motivation and structure of the IE program draws inspiration from work by W. Edwards Deming, who proposed that the lack of innovation in the market was a failure of the corporate structure rather than the individual workers (Deming Reference Deming1986). Innovation Engineering was developed by inventor Doug Hall, known for his work at Proctor and Gamble; founding the Eureka! Ranch (Eureka! Ranch n.d.); multiple appearances on reality television shows including American Inventor in the United States and Venture in Canada; and authoring a variety of books on innovation, inspiration, and marketing (Hall Reference Hall1998, Reference Hall2005a ,Reference Hall b , Reference Hall2007, Reference Hall2009; Hall & Graduates of the Innovation Engineering Leadership Institute n.d.).

The IE program has had a number of training iterations with different categorizations of the same tools and advice. A fundamental categorization summarizes the tools and advice into three main categories: Create, Communicate, and Commercialize (Hall Reference Hall2013). Program participants learn tools that aim to help them generate/create new ideas intended to grow organizations (typically to increase revenues), communicate those ideas to management and customers, and select which ideas to implement in commercially viable products. The goal of the organization and presentation of the program’s material is to be flexible to business size and goals, and useable by students, entrepreneurs, and non-governmental organizations. Innovation Engineering includes suggestions for early-stage product innovation, such as: work in groups; use mind maps; draw inspiration from outside stimuli; encourage humor; keep marketing messages simple; promise something unique and quantifiable to the customer; tell a story; verify financial viability; and maintain a mission. These suggestions are presented as tools with associated worksheets and exercises. IE Training approaches: Some IE trainees participate in a multiday workshop called the Innovation Engineering Leadership Institute, as attended by three of the authors. Advanced ‘Black Belt’ training sessions are longer programs that give participants advanced training in order to teach the advice and tools of IE to others, and these sessions were attended by one author. Additionally, the University of Maine offers both a six-course minor program and a three-course graduate certificate in IE (The University of Maine 2015). There is an online version of the training available that covers similar material.

Unlike the validation of Six Sigma, where production line improvements can be readily measured, it is extremely difficult to trace market product success back to a training program, due to the number of economic and market mitigating factors. A report by the National Research Council’s (NRC) Board on Science, Technology, and Economic Policy entitled ‘21st Century Manufacturing: The Role of the Manufacturing Extension Partnership Program’ (ed. Wessner Reference Wessner2013)) highlights the lack of assessment of IE’s effectiveness, and the need for such assessment:

‘However, unlike lean manufacturing which dates back to the 1990s in the United States and the 1960s in Japan, there is as yet limited evidence to show that this “innovation through engineering” approach is successful’ (p. 117). ‘Numerous studies have addressed their [lean manufacturing methods’] effectiveness, and they are now a staple of every MBA course. This is not yet the case with the innovation program encouraged by NIST’ (p. 118). ‘NIST MEP has invested a very substantial amount of money (apparently at least $30 million) in developing tools [of which IE is a component]…There is no track record of success, and early indications from the field suggest that gaining traction with them will be a long and difficult journey’ (p. 133).

These arguments indicate the necessity of a scientific validation of IE’s effectiveness. Further, the IE website (Innovation Engineering n.d.) claims that IE ‘is a new science’ and ‘is an accredited system and new field of academic study’. Though IE states that its teachings are based on over 20 years’ research on extensive data (Eureka! Ranch n.d.), these ‘data’ are not presented in a compelling or convincing way in the related books (Hall Reference Hall1998, Reference Hall2005a ,Reference Hall b , Reference Hall2007, Reference Hall2009), which have sparse references, or the IE seminars, which present extremely limited data in quick flashes that lack statistical verification, are not peer-reviewed, and the details of the experimental setups are not presented. Without adequate and solid proof, participants have reason to doubt the claims of testing and the small amount of data that are reported, and may feel that the testing is likely biased toward proving the success of IE methods. These doubts bring challenges in implementation at mid-level manufacturers, a goal of the NIST MEP program, as companies are understandably reluctant to take the risk of changing approaches without evidence of effectiveness.

As the aim of the Design Science Journal is to serve as the archival venue of science-based design knowledge across multiple disciplines, it serves as a uniquely qualified venue in which to examine IE’s claims. The evidence provided by IE is inadequate to demonstrate it as ‘a science’, which usually contains a body of knowledge obtained through experiments and observation as well as processes of acquiring and integrating the knowledge (Merriam-Webster.com 2015b ; Understanding Science n.d.). The contents of IE also lack some basic characteristics that an academic discipline requires, such as ‘a body of accumulated specialist knowledge’ regarding an object of research that is exclusive to this discipline, specific terminologies or technical languages, and specific research methods (Krishnan Reference Krishnan2009). There have been no peer-reviewed publications related to the IE program. These concerns together with the NRC’s report cause the tension that the IE program, which has reached out and will reach out to a large population, lacks academic support. As the first step toward this validation, it is important to ensure that its contents are based on validated knowledge. Also in line with the aims of Design Science, we review literature from a variety of disciplines, including design, engineering education, psychology, marketing, and management.

Scope of this paper: The authors investigated the effectiveness of a predefined collection of innovation-related tools and advice, namely the IE tools and advice, in an unbiased manner (note the limitations in Section 6) by performing an extensive review of related peer-reviewed literature. Each of the three IE categories was assessed separately and each tool or piece of advice in the category was addressed individually. The literature compilation focuses on test-versus-control scientific experimental studies with a statistically testable hypothesis, as opposed to observational studies.

A variety of findings are reported, including some that support, refute, and/or add on to IE recommendations. The identification of references that refute IE recommendations may also help IE to improve its training. The findings provide important academic evidence for any further judgments of IE’s claims that it is ‘a new science’ and a ‘new field of academic study’. In addition to an extensive investigation of IE, the paper provides a convenient compilation of references related to frequently espoused design and innovation practices. References for the Create category regard early-stage design, when generating creative ideas is crucial. References for the Communicate category shed some light on presenting design outcomes and how to better present designs to stakeholders. References for the Commercialize category involve making wise decisions on design selections. Addressing the different tools or advice individually allows readers to evaluate the tools and evidence for themselves, and draw their own conclusions regarding the justification of the tools to assist with innovation.

The paper proceeds as follows. Section 2 details the method and organization of the conducted literature review. Sections 35 step through the three IE categories; each of those three sections is broken into subsections to present each IE tool/advice within the category and the related literature. Section 6 presents the limitations of this study. Section 7 presents a discussion and conclusion.

2. Method

Each IE category (Create, Communicate, and Commercialize) offers both tools and advice: The validity and effectiveness of both types of information were investigated. The evidence presented here comes only from peer-reviewed journal articles and conference proceedings. The majority of the evidence presented is from scientifically designed studies that statistically tested results. Other evidence, discussed at a minimum, comes from studies that observe a subject pool and discuss overall effectiveness of a tool in anecdotal terms, as well as studies that draw recommendations from observed behavior of expert designers. The focus on test-versus-control experiments ensures that the identified evidence regarding the validity of the IE program is useful to manufacturers unfamiliar with the study of design. Outside of the theoretical engineering design community, observational studies are less well accepted.

The literature review was conducted using both print and online resources from the Iowa State University Library as well as the Google search engine. The main resource used was the Web of Knowledge, an online database of scholarly literature. Other sources include scholarly books and web articles. The online literature searches were conducted by identifying up to three keywords from each query. For example, a search for evidence showing how groups affect creative ability was conducted with the keywords: ‘Groups,’ ‘Creativity’, and ‘Experiment’. They were conducted using the Web of Knowledge, Google, and Google Scholar. Figure 1 summarizes example sets of keywords used in the online literature search. The abstracts obtained as the result of such queries were classified as relevant or irrelevant. Then, the full documents of the relevant abstracts were read and searched for evidence to either support or refute the IE tools or advice in question. References in those relevant documents were also looked through to identify other useful literature. Over 1600 abstracts were examined leading to the review of over 300 related studies. Tables 13 summarize the tools and advice given by the IE program and investigated by this study, offering summaries and references that support and refute IE teachings.

Figure 1. A summary of example sets of keywords used in the online literature search.

Figure 2. An example mind map generated with the concepts from the IE Create category.

Table 1. Literature related to the IE Create category. (Letters accompanying the references in the table refer to the setting in which the experiment was conducted. ‘c’ represents a classroom setting; ‘w’ a workplace; ‘l’ a lab or other controlled settings; and ‘o’ other—such as mail surveys. References not accompanied by letters do not include scientific experimental conditions.)

3. Create tools

Tools and advice in the Create category aim to assist in generating and refining innovative ideas. These innovations can be products, services, or business practices, but to be effective, the program asserts that they must be ‘meaningfully unique’. This agrees with the discussion of innovations for economic growth in Section 1: ‘meaningful’ implies a level of importance to the customer and ‘unique’ implies a novel idea. The following subsections step through the tools and advice of the Create category, offering supporting and/or refuting literature as summarized in Table 1.

3.1. Create tool: mind maps

Mind maps are graphical tools that organize and present information visually. Figure 2 shows an example, organizing the information in the Create category. Though the term ‘mind maps’ was popularized by Tony Buzan (ThinkBuzan n.d.), they have been used throughout history. The map has a central idea with ‘branches’ of related ideas. These grow branches of their own that continue to extend outward. Linked ideas on different branches can be connected.

This method of organization allows all ideas to be easily accessible and can help to draw connections between thoughts that may seem unrelated at first. Innovation Engineering recommends using mind maps for documenting individual and group association exercises. Kokotovich (Reference Kokotovich2008) found that mind maps are useful as design tools through the experimental introduction of mind mapping techniques to industrial design students. In this study, mind mapping techniques were introduced to first year industrial design students. The students used mind maps to help them generate individual project proposals, and maps and the creativity of these proposals was reviewed by judges, separately. The judges consistently rated work associated with higher-rated (high-quality, more complex) mind maps as more creative, and the results show a statistically significant correlation between the rating of the mind maps generated and the development of creative designs. Another study by Unalan (Reference Unalan2007) examined the ability of graphical organizational tools to convey information. Images of different organizational tools that displayed the same information were created and shown to subjects. The subjects then judged their clarity and ability to communicate creative content. Mind maps were generally ranked as the best graphical tool by the subjects.

3.2. Create tool: TRIZ

The Theory of Inventive Problem Solving, commonly known as TRIZ, is a systematic creativity tool. Genrich Altshuller and colleagues examined over two million patents to extract commonalities in innovation and formulate TRIZ (Wang, Chang & Kao Reference Wang, Chang and Kao2010). It provides problem-solving strategies to find creative solutions that would typically go unnoticed. The application of TRIZ relies on identifying contradictory goals or ‘elements’ in a design challenge, i.e. high strength and low weight. The component highlighted in IE Create is the TRIZ matrix, which maps the contradictions to a variety of engineering approaches (termed ‘inventive principles’ in TRIZ) to provoke creative designs. A simple, interactive TRIZ-matrix-lookup tool is available at the IE website as well as a number of unrestricted websites (TRIZ40 n.d.). In the IE program, when a challenge is not related to engineering, the elements are interpreted metaphorically.

Hernandez et al. (Reference Hernandez, Schmidt and Okudan2013) tested the effectiveness of TRIZ on engineering students at two universities separately using a test-versus-control design on within and between subject experiments. Exposure to TRIZ increased the average novelty per subject as well as the variety of ideas. Chulvi et al. (Reference Chulvi, González-Cruz, Mulet and Aguilar-Zambrano2013) compared the performances of TRIZ, SCAMPER (an intuitive idea-generation method), brainstorming, and a control. Seven teams used TRIZ, seven teams used SCAMPER, one team used brainstorming, and one team used no method. By judging the solutions that the teams provided to a design problem, Chulvi et al. (Reference Chulvi, González-Cruz, Mulet and Aguilar-Zambrano2013) found that (1) TRIZ outperformed SCAMPER and no method in terms of the novelty of the solutions, but TRIZ was not as good as brainstorming in this dimension; and (2) both TRIZ and SCAMPER had similar performances in terms of the utility of the solutions, and this utility was better than the performances of brainstorming and using no method. However, the inclusion of only one control team calls into question the robustness of these results. A study conducted by Birdi et al. (Reference Birdi, Leach and Magadley2012) helped identify the benefits of TRIZ in the workplace. Power systems engineers from a multinational firm who experienced a one-day TRIZ training workshop reported significantly higher levels of creative problem-solving skills, idea generation, and creative motivation than the engineers who did not attend the training, as measured by a follow-up survey many months (the average is 33 months) after the workshop. Over a period of three years, 41% of the trainees reported submitting one or more patent applications compared to 28% of the non-trainees. As part of the follow-up survey mentioned above, subjects were asked to generate as many original solutions as possible to a design problem. On average, trainees suggested two ideas each while non-trainees submitted only one. It should be noted that many events can take place during the time gap between the workshop and the survey to influence the outcomes of the survey. While investigating learning styles, León-Rovira et al. (Reference León-Rovira, Heredia-Escorza and Río2008) found that freshmen mechanical engineering students who were given an introduction to TRIZ were scored as more creative than those who did not learn about TRIZ. Ogot & Okudan (Reference Ogot and Okudan2006) reached a similar conclusion studying first year students in an ‘Introduction to Engineering Design’ course placed into groups. They were given a product design task in which the experimental course sections used TRIZ techniques while the control sections did not. The experimental sections generated more unique feasible ideas both per section and per group as compared to the control sections. The effect of using TRIZ in design teams was also examined by González-Cruz et al. (Reference González-Cruz, Aguilar-Zambrano, Aguilar-Zambrano and Colombel2008). Some sections of an undergraduate engineering design class were introduced to TRIZ while others used traditional brainstorming techniques. The sections using TRIZ methods generated 115% more unique design solutions than the others.

Arlitt et al. (Reference Arlitt, Nix and Stone2012) demonstrated that using a matrix implementation to present the TRIZ inventive principles did not improve their usefulness. The experiment included a control condition, in which the subjects used the correct TRIZ matrix, and an experimental condition, in which the subjects used a manipulated TRIZ matrix imbedded with randomly populated inventive principles. The subjects in both of the conditions used the given matrix to solve a design problem. Arlitt et al. found that using the correct TRIZ matrix had no significant advantage of facilitating idea generation over using randomly provided inventive principles.

3.3. Create tool: stimuli

Stimuli provide information to living organisms and directly influence their behavior by allowing the organisms to react accordingly (Merriam-Webster.com 2014c ). In the context of creativity, any input obtained from an outside source during the creative process can act to stimulate the mind in new ways, leading to new ideas and solutions. Finding stimuli is an important topic of IE under the general heading ‘stimulus mining’. Associated methods included: (1) insight mining, where the designer receives stimuli directly from the customer in the form of observations, conversations, opinions, and surveys; (2) market mining, meaning that ideas can come from current products that are available; and (3) technology mining, in which ideas are formed by examining emerging technology.

Fink et al. (Reference Fink, Grabner, Gebauer, Reishofer, Koschutnig and Ebner2010) investigated if creative cognition could be enhanced through learning the ideas of others. This study found that subjects in an alternative uses task were able to generate more unique ideas after they were presented with other subjects’ ideas than subjects who did not see others’ ideas. An experiment by Valacich et al. (Reference Valacich, Jung and Looney2006) used a computer-simulated group to provide subjects with different qualities of stimulation during an idea-generation exercise. Fifty ideas of varying quality, as judged by experts, were entered into a database. During the experiment, a computer program supplied these ideas to subjects in a manner that mimicked the way they would have been supplied by an actual group. They found that the highest performance in idea generation was associated with the highest quality stimuli from the database.

The Design Heuristic Cards created and used in a study by Daly et al. (Reference Daly, Christian, Yilmaz, Seifert and Gonzalez2012) provide stimuli for designers and improve ideation. The cards explain a design approach, identified from a large-scale study of academic and industry designers’ practices, and show two examples of the approach in use. In the experimental study, these cards were given to freshman engineering students along with a design task. The design solutions to the design task generated by students who used the cards were more original and better developed than those generated by students who did not use the cards, as rated by two judges using a Likert-scale assessment of creativity and diversity of solutions.

Some other studies, which do not involve statistical testing, also confirm the meaningfulness of using stimuli. In an industry-based study, Howard et al. (Reference Howard, Culley and Dekoninck2011) observed that stimuli were useful in maintaining idea-generation rates as brainstorming meetings approached their end. Feedback from the subjects confirmed that some stimuli could spark ideas and productive secondary conversations. In another study, Ocker (Reference Ocker2005) qualitatively analyzed 10 asynchronous virtual groups’ performances on initiating a computerized post office project. He found that the two groups that produced highly creative concepts both had stimulating colleagues and some initial creative example ideas. These colleagues and ideas either provided or served as stimuli that encouraged the whole group to generate creative ideas. Roy (Reference Roy1993), by interviewing individual designers and studying related materials, found that one designer would search for solutions from other areas or nature, which served as sources of stimuli.

Innovation Engineering emphasizes ethnographic studies as a source of customer insight, and academia widely accepts this approach as effective. Cooper et al. (Reference Cooper, Edgett and Kleinschmidt2002) found that companies conduct ethnographic research in a ‘Discovery Stage’ at the beginning of the product development process. An example they provided showed that by making site visits, Fluke Corporation received first-hand information about customers’ problems and needs, leading to the establishment of a very successful product line. Leonard & Rayport (Reference Leonard and Rayport1997) assert and support with examples that observing customers using products in their own environment offers five types of unique information: ‘Triggers of Use’, ‘Interactions with the User’s Environment’, ‘User Customization’, ‘Intangible Attributes of the Product’, and ‘Unarticulated User Needs’. This information can spark ideas for product concepts and improvements.

The concept of fixation presents a drawback to using stimuli to promote innovation. Design fixation occurs when a designer becomes familiar with an existing solution, and aspects of that solution then appear in their own designs. Linsey et al. (Reference Linsey, Tseng, Fu, Cagan, Wood and Schunn2010) investigated the topic of fixation with respect to engineering design. In their study, engineering design faculty members were given a design task and assigned to different task conditions. The experimental condition was given materials including an example design, which specifically violated one or more design constraints. Subjects in the experimental condition produced fewer unique solutions as compared to the control condition that received no example design. Moreover, the fixation caused by the example was so strong that these experienced designers submitted solutions that violated the same constraints.

3.4. Create tool: associations

As it relates to mental processes, association is defined as ‘the process of forming mental connections or bonds between sensations, ideas, or memories’ (Merriam-Webster.com 2014a ), which is useful for generating creative ideas. Associating stimuli that are not obviously related can result in new ideas. Innovation Engineering suggests that new ideas can be combinations of old ideas. It discusses two types of associations: free and forced. Free associations are defined as those made between closely related stimuli, such as apples and grapes. When unrelated stimuli, such as a clock and an elephant, are introduced as brainstorming stimuli to find commonalities, they are referred to as forced associations. Innovation Engineering suggests that free association leads to more ideas than forced association, but the ideas generated by forced association are generally more unique.

Association warm-up exercises were found to improve creative-thinking ability during a study by Freedman (Reference Freedman1965). Subjects in the facilitation condition were given a stimulus word and then asked to speak associated words for 30 seconds, while those in the non-facilitation condition were asked to define stimulus words but not generate associated words. On a subsequent creative-thinking test, male subjects who completed the association exercises earned an average score of 18.05 out of 30, which was significantly higher than the average score of 14.15 of the non-facilitation condition. The difference in female subjects’ scores was not as large as that for male subjects, but the trend was similar. Subjects in a study by Coskun (Reference Coskun2009) generated more ideas during a brainstorming session after performing an exercise regarding closely linked word pairs. The subjects who worked with distantly linked word pairs generated fewer ideas. This finding suggests that free association may be more effective at generating larger volumes of ideas than forced association, but it does not speak to the quality or originality of these ideas. Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Russ, Gibson and Hall1970) found that free association training improved subjects’ abilities to discover additional links between stimuli on the Remote Associates Test (RAT; Mednick Reference Mednick1962). Subjects in the experimental condition were given 30 seconds to free-associate additional words for each of 10 stimulus words. To ensure that simply being exposed to the words was not enough to significantly affect scores, each subject in the control group read the words generated by a randomly selected subject in the experimental condition. Cheng et al. (Reference Cheng, Wang, Liu and Chen2010) found association instruction directly affected creativity. After a creative-thinking pretest determined that there were no significant differences between fourth grade students, both free and forced association training was given to an experimental condition over a five-week period. This training consisted of making associations between numbers, images, stories, and music, for example, the association between the number 14 and a windsurfer. After the treatment phase, all students wrote poems to be judged on creativity. The poems by students in the experimental condition were consistently judged as more creative than those from the control.

3.5. Create tool: groups

It is commonly thought that working in groups can positively affect creative productivity, as is emphasized by IE. More people working on a common task means more opinions, experiences, thoughts, and neurons influencing the final product.

By comparing the quantities of ideas generated by groups and individuals, Paulus & Yang (Reference Paulus and Yang2000) determined that groups could outperform individuals and nominal groups (pooled individual data) in idea generation. Using a brainwriting exercise, students sharing their ideas with others in a group led to the production of more unique ideas than students working individually or pooled individuals. Another finding from this study is that ideation performance is inhibited when the group members are asked to memorize the ideas generated by teammates. Different levels of group interaction during brainstorming activities were examined by McGlynn et al. (Reference McGlynn, McGurk, Effland, Johll and Harding2004). University students were assigned either high-interaction groups (working face to face) or low-interaction groups (working individually but in the same area). Low-interaction groups generated more ideas than high-interaction groups, but the ideas of the high-interaction groups were of higher quality, in terms of plausibility and correctness as rated by research assistants. The high-interaction groups’ consensus solutions were also rated as more plausible than the group members’ individual responses. The poorer performance of low-interaction groups on generating high-quality ideas was also observed by Ocker (Reference Ocker2005) in his qualitative research on virtual groups, mentioned in Section 3.3. He found that the group that had few to none discussions and provided little feedback on group members’ inputs had very few original ideas. Coskun (Reference Coskun2011) studied the effect of group size and session length on brainstorming activities. Groups of 4, 6, 8, 10, and 12 people were given an idea-generation task and allowed to work for either 15 or 20 minutes. The number of unique ideas generated by each group increased proportionally with the size of the group, but the size did not affect the performance of the individuals who comprised the group, suggesting no synergistic or combinatorial effects. Pirola-Merlo & Mann (Reference Pirola-Merlo and Mann2004) identified that even though a majority of the variance in group creativity was accounted for by the average group member creativity, a systematic variance still existed in group creativity after controlling for individual creativity. This means that even though the individual group members’ creativity accounted for most of the group’s creativity, there was evidence of improved creative thinking due to group interaction. Taggar (Reference Taggar2002) found that working in a group can help to improve creativity if the members engage in behavior that encourages the open sharing of information. However, if group members are inadequately trained or if the group is too large, group processes can stifle creativity. This suggests that ideal creativity-fostering groups are relatively small with members who understand the importance of their behavior.

Contrary to the above findings, some previous literature (Diehl & Stroebe Reference Diehl and Stroebe1987; Mullen et al. Reference Mullen, Johnson and Salas1991; Thornburg Reference Thornburg1991; Larey & Paulus Reference Larey and Paulus1999) has shown evidence that brainstorming groups have poorer creativity performance than nominal groups. Diehl & Stroebe (Reference Diehl and Stroebe1987) examined a number of possible ways that group brainstorming could negatively affect the productivity of the members. The data they collected suggest the existence of blocking, or the inability to share ideas because another member was sharing, as a possible hindrance to group idea generation. A blocking delay between generating and sharing an idea may prevent new ideas from fully forming.

3.6. Create tool: dual brain

Innovation Engineering discusses that creative and impulsive behavior originates in the brain’s right hemisphere while logical and analytical behavior stems from the left. Although this hemispheric distinction does not actually exist, the terms ‘right brain’ and ‘left brain’ have become figures of speech pertaining to creative and analytical personality types, respectively. Innovation Engineering refers to the right brain and the left brain as descriptions of personality with remarks such as ‘logical left brain’ and ‘radical right brain’. ‘Whole brain’ thinking is also discussed, with the ability to combine the strengths of both thinking types and obtain the best results.

Harkins & Macrosson (Reference Harkins and Macrosson1990) tested if exercising the right hemisphere with a 10-week drawing course could enhance creativity. The experimental condition performed significantly better than the control condition in two dimensions as measured by the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking. Mihov et al. (Reference Mihov, Denzler and Förster2010) conducted a meta-analytic review of the literature that addressed relationships between creativity and hemispheric dominance. They provided statistical results showing that creativity was more likely to be associated with right-hemispheric than left-hemispheric activation.

Kowatari et al. (Reference Kowatari, Lee, Yamamura, Nagamori, Levy, Yamane and Yamamoto2009) conducted functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scans on both expert and novice designers during a design task to find that expert designers showed a bias toward right-hemisphere activation while the novices experienced a more bilateral response. Many experts showed a negative change in blood oxygenated level dependent signals in the left hemisphere. Both fMRI and electroencephalogram (EEG) measurement methods were used by Fink et al. (Reference Fink, Grabner, Benedek, Reishofer, Hauswirth, Fally, Neuper, Ebner and Neubauer2009) to record brain activity during creative-thinking tasks. Subjects working on tasks such as generating alternative uses, describing object characteristics, inventing names, and completing words experienced whole brain activation rather than a bias to either side. Abraham et al. (Reference Abraham, Pieritz, Thybusch, Rutter, Kroeger, Schweckendiek, Stark, Windmann and Hermann2012) examined brain activity associated with conceptual expansion and found that the regions involved were those responsible for the retention, retrieval, and integration of conceptual knowledge, which are strongly lateralized to the left hemisphere. Howard-Jones et al. (Reference Howard-Jones, Blakemore, Samuel, Summers and Claxton2005) identified the brain area involved in approaching a creative story generation task as the right prefrontal cortex. In general, the literature suggests that the notion of left/right brain thinking is not accurate. While some creative processes showed hemisphere bias in brain activity, others caused whole brain activation. This activity is influenced by the experience subjects have with the creative activity being studied. Also, while certain creative processes may elicit the activation of a particular area of the brain, other areas of the brain are necessary to utilize these processes.

3.7. Create tool: humor

Humor is one of the many ways that people can interact with one another, and it is important in maintaining healthy interpersonal relationships (De Koning & Weiss Reference De Koning and Weiss2002; Graham Reference Graham1995). Humor also helps in enhancing a person’s self-confidence, and self-confidence can benefit creativity (Kelley & Kelley Reference Kelley and Kelley2013). Self-confidence is associated with self-efficacy, and the willingness to take risks in design. Highly creative design solutions often involve an element of risk. Innovation Engineering uses humor and satire to demonstrate points. Beyond applying to innovative idea generation, it was noted that humor at work will increase the productivity and mood of employees because it leads to a more enjoyable experience.

To identify the relationship between humor and creativity, Humke & Schaefer (Reference Humke and Schaefer1996) had subjects complete humor and creative-thinking measures. They found a Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.77 between scores on the Multidimensional Sense of Humor Scale and the Franck Drawing Completion Test. Rouff (Reference Rouff1975) found a positive correlation between a subject’s abilities to identify humorous incongruities and RAT scores. Treadwell (Reference Treadwell1970) found significant positive correlations between ‘humor creation scores’, measured by a Cartoon Test, and three creativity scores: RAT scores, Gestalt Transformations test scores, and Novelty of Productions scores. Ziv (Reference Ziv1976) investigated the relationship between laughter and creative thinking. A humorous recording, selected due to its ability to produce high amplitude and long duration laughter in subjects listening to it, was played for subjects in an experimental condition prior to a Torrance Test of Creative Thinking. The subjects who listened to the recording performed significantly better on the test than the control condition that did not. In Ocker’s (Reference Ocker2005) observation of virtual groups’ performances, he noticed that witty but task-related humor was involved in the two most creative groups’ internal interactions, which suggests positive effects of humor on creativity.

4. Communicate tools

Scientists and engineers are often stereotyped as poor communicators; according to recent Graduate Record Examinations scores, engineering graduate degree applicants had some of the lowest average verbal reasoning and analytical writing scores of all intended majors (Educational Testing Service 2013). Furthermore, technical information is typically detailed and complex, so even skilled communicators may have trouble presenting it. Innovation Engineering emphasizes that it is necessary to effectively communicate innovative ideas to the intended audience. This section presents literature related to the tools and advice of the IE Communicate category, as summarized in Table 2.

4.1. Communicate tool: benefit promise

Benefit promise is the IE term for the specific benefit that an innovation will provide to the audience. The program stresses that it is important to tell people why they should care as opposed to simply listing features of a product or service. A study by Cooper & Kleinschmidt (Reference Cooper and Kleinschmidt1993) showed the relative importance of communicating product benefits to product success. After examining 109 different products from chemical companies, they identified the traits that lead to successful products. Featuring a specific benefit of importance to the customer ranked as the fourth most important trait of success, behind traits that involved the product’s relative quality and price as compared to the competition.

Table 2. Literature related to the IE Communicate category

4.2. Communicate tool: simplicity

In communication, simplicity refers to targeting the amount and type of information being exchanged. More information communicated means more to remember. Innovation Engineering recommends that all communications be as simple as possible to allow the most important aspects of the message to be received and remembered.

It is documented in Miller’s review (Reference Miller1994) that people can only discriminate about seven levels of single-dimensional stimuli (for example, seven numbers), indicating the limited information processing capabilities of humans. While the general recommendation of simple communication is featured in general-audience scientific books (Heath & Heath Reference Heath and Heath2007; Maeda Reference Maeda2006) there was limited academic research identified by the authors. One reason for this could be that simplicity in communication is assumed to be common sense.

Some sources were located that refute the benefit of simple communications. Shedler & Manis (Reference Shedler and Manis1986) tested the effect additional but unrelated details have on the credibility of arguments. Subjects in two conditions heard equal numbers of opposing arguments about the fitness of a mother and were asked whether they thought the mother should retain custody of her child. One condition heard additional details for the positive arguments while the other condition heard additional details for the negative arguments. Even though the details were unrelated to overall paternal fitness, the additional information significantly affected the subjects’ judgments in both conditions. Another study by Macklin et al. (Reference Macklin, Bruvold and Shea1985) examined how the readability of a print advertisement affected a reader’s ability to retain important information. Different versions of a printed advertisement were designed and statistically verified to contain the same information and vividness but have different levels of readability. These were shown to subjects whose recall was subsequently tested. The experiment revealed that the readability did not affect the ability to recall ad claims, attitude toward the brand or ad, or purchase intent.

4.3. Communicate tool: tell the truth

Innovation Engineering advises that honesty in communications imparts trust between a company and its customers. The Edelman Trust Barometer (Edelman 2013) collected information from 26 000 public subjects in 26 countries to identify attributes that encourage trust in companies. Honest communication from companies was identified as one of the most important attributes. Trifts & Haubl (Reference Trifts and Haubl2003), Corbitt et al. (Reference Corbitt, Thanasankit and Yi2003) and Jarvenpaa et al. (Reference Jarvenpaa, Tractinsky and Vitale2000) all show that the perceived trustworthiness of an Internet vendor is positively correlated with the probability that customers choose to purchase from those vendors. A mail survey conducted by Lau & Lee (Reference Lau and Lee1999) identified that brand performance is a strong predictor of trust in a brand, and that trust in a brand contributes to brand loyalty. Subjects in a study by Settle & Golden (Reference Settle and Golden1974) were shown advertisements where a new product was compared to the current best-selling alternative. One study condition was shown a version that claimed the new product was superior in five of five areas while another condition was shown a version that only claimed superiority in three of five areas. The subjects shown the latter reported a higher believability for the advertisement. Anderson (Reference Anderson1973) investigated the relationship between expectations and product performance; when subjects were given realistic expectations of a pen’s performance, they rated its performance higher than when given overly positive expectations.

One study offers insight into how much honest information should be supplied. Liberali et al. (Reference Liberali, Urban and Hauser2013) found that proactively supplying information that made brands vulnerable to criticism did not increase subject trust in the brand. Honesty should be paired with simplicity of communications in a strategic manner.

4.4. Communicate tool: repetition

Innovation Engineering advises that repeating a product message increases memorability. D’Souza & Rao (Reference D’Souza and Rao1995) tested the effect of repeating a specific advertisement relative to that of competitors. They found that higher repetition rates resulted in significant increases in top-of-mind awareness, brand preference, and brand choice. Ray & Sawyer (Reference Ray and Sawyer1971) exposed female shoppers to slides of advertisements with varying repetition rates, and then the shoppers took a product survey. Repetition rate had the most significant effect on advertisement recall, with an almost 40% increase when comparing a single exposure to four exposures. Purchase intention for the products shown also increased significantly. The significant effect of repetition on information recall is also supported by the literature in psychology (Cacioppo & Petty Reference Cacioppo and Petty1979; Mayer Reference Mayer1983). Mayer (Reference Mayer1983) found that repetition not only increased the amount of recalled information that was related to the provided recordings, but also had effects on what was remembered. The recall of conceptual principles benefited from repetition more than the recall of details such as concrete analogies and facts.

Cacioppo & Petty (Reference Cacioppo and Petty1979) found that message repetition increased the amount of information that was recalled and that as the number of repetitions increased, subject agreement with the repeated statement first increased and then decreased. This suggests that there may be an oversaturation point in message repetition that could have a negative effect on innovation communication.

4.5. Communicate tool: provide proof

Providing proof presents potential customers with concrete evidence to support a marketing claim. Innovation Engineering examples of proof include test results, company pedigree, testimonials, and guarantees. Innovation Engineering advises that more proof is better as long as the proof is meaningful.

Liberali et al. (Reference Liberali, Urban and Hauser2013) tested if access to direct product experience, print and online resources, word of mouth, or recommendations from trusted advisors would alter customer brand preferences. The study found that providing direct product experience and testimonials significantly improved brand consideration and trust in a brand. East et al. (Reference East, Hammond and Lomax2008) used mail-in surveys to measure the change in purchase intent after exposure to positive and negative word of mouth. The results showed that positive word of mouth improved the subjects’ probability of purchase by an average of around 50% while the negative word of mouth decreased the average purchase probability by 25%. Multiple references from Section 4.3 (Corbitt et al. Reference Corbitt, Thanasankit and Yi2003; Jarvenpaa et al. Reference Jarvenpaa, Tractinsky and Vitale2000; Trifts & Haubl Reference Trifts and Haubl2003) explain that customer trust improves brand consideration; this trust can be affected by the proof that is available.

Bearden & Shimp (Reference Bearden and Shimp1982) considered product warranty as an extrinsic cue that would influence customers’ perceptions of innovative products. By constructing models of customers’ risk perceptions and overall affect toward two new product concepts, they proved that a warranty mediated customers’ affective responses to innovative products via perceptions of risk. Thorelli et al. (Reference Thorelli, Lim and Ye1989) found a significant effect of warranties on three measures of product evaluations: perceived quality, overall attitude, and purchase intention of the product. Boulding & Kirmani (Reference Boulding and Kirmani1993) found that a better warranty helped a high-credibility firm to achieve better customer perceptions of product performance and overall quality, as well as to improve purchase intent. But this was not the case for a low-credibility firm, which suggests an interaction with trust. Fu & Chen (Reference Fu and Chen2011) present mixed findings on offering guarantees. In a study examining online auctions, they identified that while offering a refund guarantee did increase the price premium of the items up for bid, it did not affect the time until the first bid, number of bidders, or number of bids. This means that the customers were willing to pay more knowing they were guaranteed a refund, but the guarantee did not attract more interest or additional customers. In the psychology literature, Maddux & Rogers (Reference Maddux and Rogers1980) found that people who provided supporting evidence while arguing a position were significantly more likely to persuade a subject, independent of the arguer’s expertise and physical attractiveness.

4.6. Communicate tool: include a number

Innovation Engineering suggests that including numbers makes for concrete and understandable communications; for example, ‘this toothbrush design removes three times as much plaque as the leading brand’. The number three in this marketing message gives the audience a value to place on the product.

Holbrook (Reference Holbrook1978) had subjects read and rate advertisements that were either objective (i.e., ‘27 miles per gallon’) or subjective (‘truly excellent gas mileage’). Objective advertisements were statistically associated with higher brand attitude and claim belief than subjective ads. Rossiter & Percy (Reference Rossiter and Percy1978) had customers rate a product after seeing its advertisement. Subjects who saw ads that included concrete information, such as ‘Bavaria’s number one selling beer for the last ten years’, responded more favorably to the product and the brand than those who saw less concrete statements like ‘Bavaria’s finest beer’. Davis (Reference Davis1993), in his literature review, strongly suggested the use of numerical data and specific statements in environmental claims.

It is also important to include numbers and be specific when communicating internal product development goals. Six Sigma is a strategy used in industry to ‘improve profitability, to drive out waste in business processes and to improve the efficiency of all operations that meet or exceed customers’ needs and expectations’ (Antony & Banuelas Coronado Reference Antony and Banuelas Coronado2002, p. 24). Design for Six Sigma requires the identification of numerical customer requirements and engineering specifications (Mader Reference Mader2002). These specific goals can improve the performance of the project as proposed by Linderman et al. (Reference Linderman, Schroeder, Zaheer and Choo2003) and can also facilitate communication within a design team. Similarly, House of Quality, which is a ‘basic design tool of the management approach’ (Hauser & Clausing Reference Hauser and Clausing1988, p. 63) contains steps for identifying specific customer requirements and engineering specifications. These steps help benchmark development activities and set target design values (Hauser & Clausing Reference Hauser and Clausing1988), and are particularly useful for interdisciplinary product design teams.

4.7. Communicate tool: tell a story

A good story captivates audiences both intellectually and emotionally, and IE advises their use in communicating marketing messages. Green & Brock (Reference Green and Brock2000) gave subjects a short story to read and told them that it was either fictitious or real. A post-survey identified that subjects who reported higher transportation by the story (deeper mental processing/emotional involvement) rated the story’s situation more realistic and the characters more favorably, whether or not they were told the story was real. The positive effect of arranging facts into a story was examined by Pennington & Hastie (Reference Pennington and Hastie1988). Subjects acted as jury members for a mock trial, and after being presented evidence, were asked to determine if the defendant was innocent or guilty. The study found that when evidence supporting one verdict was presented in a story-like order, the subjects were significantly more likely to pass that verdict and reported significantly higher confidence in their decisions. This shows that stories not only influence judgments and perceptions, but also improve the persuasiveness of an argument.

5. Commercialize tools

Revenues and profits can measure product innovation success. But variables that influence these metrics, such as government policy, organizational change, and distribution, lie beyond a designer’s control. The customer base also determines profitability, and shifts in public opinion can have a major impact. Despite external challenges such as these, a development team must decide which innovative ideas to pursue, and which to table. Innovation Engineering offers sound advice for mitigating the risks that accompany the innovation processes, but the associated commercialization tools lack sophistication and accuracy. State-of-the-art approaches are presented below and summarized in Table 3.

5.1. Commercialize tool: do one thing great

Innovation Engineering advises companies to identify a core benefit that they can offer customers, and use this benefit to drive the mission and focus of the company. This can mean, for example, eliminating product offerings that are auxiliary to this core benefit, or changing the manner in which product benefits are presented. Supporting this, Boatwright & Nunes (Reference Boatwright and Nunes2001) found that removing low-sales items from grocery store shelves increased average category sales. Although the reduction in options caused some customers to leave the retailer, the majority of customers whose preferred brand was cut adopted a different brand. Iyengar & Lepper (Reference Iyengar and Lepper2000) examined the effect of limiting the number of choices available. Subjects in a grocery market were offered free samples from a large selection of jams (24 options) or a reduced selection of 6 options. The large selection set attracted more subjects to investigate the offer, but produced fewer final sales than the limited set. Redelmeier & Shafir (Reference Redelmeier and Shafir1995) investigated how decisions were altered by the addition of new options. When presented a hypothetical situation in which subjects had plans to complete one activity but were then offered a single more attractive alternative, only 21% chose the original activity. Interestingly, when subjects were offered two better alternatives, 40% chose to follow through with the original activity. When given multiple alternatives, a sizeable number of subjects avoided making a switching decision.

Table 3. Literature related to the IE Commercialize category

In a report of an interview with Dell’s founder, Magretta (Reference Magretta1998) emphasized the importance of focus and specialization in Dell’s success. Dell did not decide to make every piece that was needed for a computer, instead purchasing well-made components from other companies. They focused on ‘delivering solutions and systems to customers’ (Magretta Reference Magretta1998, p. 74), creating unique value for its customers and developing the company.

One study located does potentially refute the idea of limiting offerings. Oppewal & Koelemeijer (Reference Oppewal and Koelemeijer2005) mailed surveys to customers that asked for opinions about a variety of flower assortments. When subjects were given more assortment options, the average evaluation across assortments significantly improved, in a linear manner. However, this study did not investigate how many subjects purchased one of the assortments.

5.2. Commercialize tool: predicting sales and selecting ideas

After refining innovative ideas, it is necessary to select ideas to focus on developing them, while putting other less-promising ideas aside. This selection process can be partially guided by advice, such as Do One Thing Great: if the idea strays too far from the core company mission, it may not be right for development. But, ultimately, the success of an idea must be linked to metrics such as predicted revenues. As Herstatt, Verworn & Nagahira (Reference Herstatt, Verworn and Nagahira2004) observed, companies use economic criteria to assess their innovative ideas. The selection can be done by experts with simple selection criteria (Ferioli et al. Reference Ferioli, Dekoninck, Culley, Roussel and Renaud2010), both objective and subjective, but involving explicit tools can better support the selection. Innovation Engineering recommends the Fourt–Woodlock equation (Fourt & Woodlock Reference Fourt and Woodlock1960) as a tool to aid in this evaluation. The equation, as it is presented by the IE program, is

$$\begin{eqnarray}AS=FDM\times TR\times FPR+FDM\times TR\times RR\times RPR\times NRP,\end{eqnarray}$$

where AS is the annual sales forecast, FDM is the number of final decision makers (potential purchasers), TR is the trial rate, FPR is the first purchase revenue, RR is the repeat rate, RPR is the repeat purchase revenue, and NRP is the number of repeat purchases. It should be noted that these variables are not the same in all representations of the equation, but the meaning of the variables used does not change markedly from one version to another.

To create this equation, Fourt and Woodlock used trial rate and repeat ratios based on observed test markets and real purchase data, and made a number of assumptions about the consistency of the environment in which the products would be sold. The assumptions were that the distribution, promotional expenditures, prices, competitive activity, product, and packaging would not change substantially during the time of the estimate. In situations where these assumptions held and the rates could be determined by an initial test sample, this method predicted volume changes within 5% of the actual values. If the prediction shows that a product will bring substantial profit, a company can justify mass production. Conversely, if the forecast implies a poor return, the company can edit the offering in an attempt to improve the prediction, or scrap the offering altogether. As presented by IE, the equation did not include rates determined by an initial test market or stress on any of the stated assumptions. However, in situations observed by Fourt and Woodlock where one or more of the assumptions were violated, the estimate became much less accurate. Innovation Engineering also suggested estimating the trial and repeat rates, which will likely lead to further loss of accuracy.

Test markets have been used for many years, and forecasting models have been generated to predict new product performance; see Parfitt & Collins (Reference Parfitt and Collins1968), Eskin (Reference Eskin1973), Nakanishi (Reference Nakanishi1973), Jain, Mahajan & Muller (Reference Jain, Mahajan and Muller1995) and Decker & Gnibba-Yukawa (Reference Decker and Gnibba-Yukawa2010). These models, however, are more complex than the Fourt–Woodlock equation, and thus more difficult for a general audience to adopt. The Bass diffusion model (Bass Reference Bass1969) for adoption rate prediction has been studied substantially and modified to accurately predict specific adoption rates for specific markets (Bass, Krishnan & Jain Reference Bass, Krishnan and Jain1994; Satoh Reference Satoh2001; Li & Yang Reference Li and Yang2006; Diao, Zhang & Jia Reference Diao, Zhang and Jia2009; Tseng & Hu Reference Tseng and Hu2009). However, this model assumes that the innovation will end with complete adoption across a population, like personal computers or television.

The authors offer alternative approaches for selecting innovative ideas. Multi-Attribute Utility Theory (MAUT), based on Utility Theory in economics, enables idea selection by comparing overall utility of each idea in decision makers’ perspectives (Yates Reference Yates2003). The ideas are first evaluated in terms of multiple criteria respectively with corresponding utility functions; then the overall utility of an idea is usually obtained in an addictive manner for further comparisons. Applications of the MAUT include comparing an innovative parabolic solar cooker against existing cooking devices in India (Pohekar & Ramachandran Reference Pohekar and Ramachandran2006) and optimally positioning a product innovation in an existing market (Gavish, Horsky & Srikanth Reference Gavish, Horsky and Srikanth1983). Choice share (amongst potential customers) of innovations can also be estimated using customer-survey-based methods such as discrete choice analysis (Train Reference Train1986, Reference Train2009; Wassenaar & Chen Reference Wassenaar and Chen2003; Michalek et al. Reference Michalek, Feinberg and Papalambros2005; Kumar et al. Reference Kumar, Hoyle, Chen, Wang, Gomez-Levi and Koppelman2009; Zhang, Gensler & Garcia Reference Zhang, Gensler and Garcia2011).

Simple and effective, Pugh matrixes are widely used by engineers for idea selection (Honkala, Hämäläinen & Salonen Reference Honkala, Hämäläinen and Salonen2007; Ullman Reference Ullman2009). Design alternatives are evaluated in terms of a set of criteria against a base design concept. A scoring system (1, 0, $-$ 1) indicates that a design alternative performs better, equal to, or worse than the base concept for each criterion. A total score for each alternative is tallied, the best aspects of different ideas are combined, and the process continues iteratively until one idea is clearly the best alternative. Frey et al. (Reference Frey, Herder, Wijnia, Subrahmanian, Katsikopoulos and Clausing2009) evaluated this method by quantitatively modeling its implementation process. They confirmed the effectiveness of this method during the early design stage, and found that it facilitated the decision convergence with objectivity.

Idea selection can also be guided by formal engineering optimization approaches combined with market share predictions. Interdisciplinary optimization mathematically finds the ‘best’ alternative as evaluated by an objective function subject to conditions including customer preferences and design constraints (Papalambros & Wilde Reference Papalambros and Wilde2000), and has identified optimal product designs with predicted market performance (Michalek et al. Reference Michalek, Feinberg and Papalambros2005; MacDonald et al. Reference MacDonald, Lubensky, Sohns and Papalambros2009; Kelly et al. Reference Kelly, Maheut, Petiot and Papalambros2011).

Decision-Based Design (DBD) (Hazelrigg Reference Hazelrigg1998; Li & Azarm Reference Li and Azarm2000; Gupta & Samuel Reference Gupta and Samuel2001; Wassenaar & Chen Reference Wassenaar and Chen2003; Wassenaar et al. Reference Wassenaar, Chen, Cheng and Sudjianto2005) helps select a product design alternative through systematic steps. Hazelrigg (Reference Hazelrigg1998) offers a DBD framework that considers factors like design variables controlled by designers, product attributes valuable to customers, demand, price, and time. It allows for the incorporation of methods like discrete choice analysis and profit optimization, and can be adapted to a variety of design problems (Li & Azarm Reference Li and Azarm2000; Gupta & Samuel Reference Gupta and Samuel2001; Wassenaar & Chen Reference Wassenaar and Chen2003; Wassenaar et al. Reference Wassenaar, Chen, Cheng and Sudjianto2005). Wassenaar & Chen (Reference Wassenaar and Chen2003) use DBD to identify the design alternative with the largest net revenue, demonstrated with the design of an electric universal motor.

Day (Reference Day2007) suggested using both the risk matrix and the R–W–W (‘Real, Win, Worth it’) screen to determine whether an innovation should be implemented or not. Positioning an innovation on the risk matrix, which was defined by two familiarity dimensions (the company’s familiarity with the new product and its familiarity with the intended market), allowed an estimation of the innovation’s probability of failure. The R–W–W screen provided a three-level question set that the company should answer for its innovations, which helped the company assess the innovations for screening.

6. Limitations

The literature review focuses on tools and advice covered in the IE program. There are other tools and advice that can help with innovation but are not mentioned in this paper, including ones that can be used in place of IE’s tools and advice. When evaluating the effectiveness of each tool and advice, this paper focuses on comparing the related supporting and refuting literature. This comparison has a notable limitation: there is an intrinsic unbalance between the quantities of supporting and refuting literature. Evidence refuting or disproving effectiveness is more difficult to find because publishing a paper on an unsuccessful experiment is more difficult than publishing a paper on a successful one. A potential alternative to refuting literature is literature that demonstrates the solid performance of a competing tool.

Some tools suggested by IE have known or potential limitations. For example, the TRIZ matrix was developed based on patents from more than 50 years ago, and those patents are primarily mechanically oriented. In addition to the uselessness of the matrix implementation discussed in Section 3.2, researchers have found that the TRIZ matrix has limited ability to interpret patents from more recent years and modern industries (Mann Reference Mann2002; Sheu, Chen & Yu Reference Sheu, Chen and Yu2012). Eliminating product offerings, as a way to ‘do one thing great’, will have varied effectiveness dependent on product category, the sophistication of the design solution, and cultural differences, for example, variety-seeking behavior.

Importantly, the Create category of the IE program focuses only on certain approaches. Here we discuss others that are not mentioned in the IE training, and for which we were not able to locate test-versus-control experiments that demonstrate the validity of the approach. This does not imply that they do not work, only that they are beyond the scope of this paper’s discussion and the authors’ abilities to demonstrate their effectiveness. For example, the Concepts–Knowledge (C–K) Theory has been used in both the academics and the industry to help with innovation (Agogué & Kazakçi Reference Agogué, Kazakçi, Chakrabarti and Blessing2013). The C–K Theory is a design theory that considers design as related to two spaces: the ‘Concept’ space and the ‘Knowledge’ space; this theory also explains that creativity is due to certain expansions of the two spaces (Hatchuel & Weil Reference Hatchuel and Weil2003). This rationality of creativity can guide design processes to achieve creative outcomes systematically (Hatchuel, Le Masson & Weil Reference Hatchuel, Le Masson and Weil2004). In addition, there are product-design-related methods that were not specifically created for facilitating creativity, but could be beneficial to it. Functional analysis decomposes a product’s overall function into a set of detailed and manageable subfunctions so as to help understand design problems and generate concepts (Ullman Reference Ullman2009). The subfunctions, which can be defined using the functional basis (Stone & Wood Reference Stone and Wood2000; Hirtz et al. Reference Hirtz, Stone, McAdams, Szykman and Wood2002), serve as the bases for concept generation. This approach helps mitigate designers’ tendencies to focus on specific solutions in the early design stage (Bryant et al. Reference Bryant, McAdams, Stone, Kurtoglu and Campbell2006). Functional analysis also encourages designers to seek different solutions and ideations (Berawi Reference Berawi2011). Coatanéa et al. (Reference Coatanéa, Nonsiri, Christophe and Mokammel2014) incorporated casual graph and dimensional analysis into the early design analysis stage, which can help extract contradictions in the design problems systematically. Identifying these contradictions points the way to unique design solutions and thus innovation.

7. Discussion and conclusion

The literature review supports the use of the majority of IE tools and advice, as summarized in Tables 13. Some references challenge IE suggestions, such as Arlitt et al. (Reference Arlitt, Nix and Stone2012) who showed that a random selection of the inventive principles could be as effective as using the TRIZ matrix to arrive at particular inventive principles. Linsey et al. (Reference Linsey, Tseng, Fu, Cagan, Wood and Schunn2010) demonstrated that the use of stimuli can decrease creative output and even cause violations to design criteria via fixation. Mullen et al. (Reference Mullen, Johnson and Salas1991), Thornburg (Reference Thornburg1991) and Larey & Paulus (Reference Larey and Paulus1999) observed that creativity performances of interactive groups were not as good as those of nominal groups, which implies that group-based processes can inhibit creativity. Diehl & Stroebe (Reference Diehl and Stroebe1987) and Taggar (Reference Taggar2002) identified reasons for this possible inhibition. Hemispheric brain activation research (Howard-Jones et al. Reference Howard-Jones, Blakemore, Samuel, Summers and Claxton2005; Fink et al. Reference Fink, Grabner, Benedek, Reishofer, Hauswirth, Fally, Neuper, Ebner and Neubauer2009; Kowatari et al. Reference Kowatari, Lee, Yamamura, Nagamori, Levy, Yamane and Yamamoto2009; Abraham et al. Reference Abraham, Pieritz, Thybusch, Rutter, Kroeger, Schweckendiek, Stark, Windmann and Hermann2012) gives evidence that the entire brain is necessary for the generation and implementation of creative ideas, as opposed to a left/right brain duality. Macklin et al. (Reference Macklin, Bruvold and Shea1985) and Shedler & Manis (Reference Shedler and Manis1986) showed that it can be beneficial to increase complexity by including extra and possibly even irrelevant details, thus refuting the idea that simplicity always improves communication. Fu & Chen (Reference Fu and Chen2011) found that offering a refund guarantee for online auctions had limited effects on bids. Oppewal & Koelemeijer (Reference Oppewal and Koelemeijer2005) proved that increasing the number of offerings can lead to more positive reactions from customers. Alternative approaches to the Fourt–Woodlock equation are suggested in Section 5.2. The refuting evidence identified here is not all-inclusive. There is the possibility of limitations or shortcomings that remain unidentified due to the bias toward publishing successful studies.

The IE Create category contains tools and advice that aim to assist in the generation and refinement of innovative ideas. These tools/advice can be associated with specific stages of the creative process. Wallas (Reference Wallas1926) divides the creative process into four stages: Preparation, Incubation, Illumination, and Verification. In the Preparation stage, people actively seek information and investigate the problem at hand from all aspects; in the Incubation stage, there are no conscious efforts spent on the problem, but mental processes related to the problem continue unconsciously; the Illumination stage refers to the moment that a creative idea comes to mind; finally, this creative idea is refined and verified in the Verification stage (Wallas Reference Wallas1926). ‘Mind maps’, ‘TRIZ’, ‘stimuli’, ‘associations’, and ‘groups’ are all related to the Preparation stage as they assist people in actively working on the creativity task. ‘Stimuli’ and ‘association’ may be also related to the Incubation stage, because the stimuli that people viewed and the associations they made in the earlier period may play a role in the unconscious mental processes. ‘Groups’, however, may negatively affect the Incubation and the Illumination stages. The Incubation stage takes place when people are having a break and are not ‘thinking’ about the creativity task; but when people are in groups working on the task, this is very difficult to do. Outside group discussions may further distract a person’s attention and cause him or her to miss any ideas that suddenly arise in the Illumination stage. ‘Humor’ relates to the Incubation stage as humor can create a divergence from the task and diffuse concentration. ‘Dual brain’ can be related to all four stages of the creative process; it is possible that different stages use different parts of the brain and initiate different activities in the brain. These tools/advice also differ in their aims and focus. All the tools/advice suggested by IE, except for TRIZ, are generic and can be applied to a wide variety of problems, while TRIZ mainly addresses engineering problems. Some of the tools/advice are local and focus on the problem at hand, such as mind maps; and some others have a wide spectrum, such as humor and dual brain. It is important for the readers to consider these factors when choosing the tools/advice for use.

In the studies summarized in this paper, creativity is judged in numerous ways, for example, by the volume of ideas generated, quality of ideas as rated by judges, and results of tests such as the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking. The translation of effective improvement in creativity from the lab environment to the workplace is a complex undertaking. Table 1 notes the studies that occurred in workplace scenarios with a ‘w’, and these are perhaps the most effective studies in supporting the recommendations of IE, which is geared toward workplace settings.

The review identifies areas that require further research. Many references included in Section 3 incorporate idea-generation tasks, but only a few involve product design tasks. Additionally, design spans a wide variety of industries. Service-oriented problem solving, management issues, and governmental policy resolution all require creativity, and could be good testing grounds for promising creativity tools. A few references also mentioned the importance of motivation in creativity, although this subject was not explicitly included in the search. When subjects in the studies were asked to be creative or knew that their creativity would be judged, it prompted them to respond more creatively. This was explicitly documented by Miller et al. (Reference Miller, Russ, Gibson and Hall1970) and Howard-Jones et al. (Reference Howard-Jones, Blakemore, Samuel, Summers and Claxton2005). The IE program is likely most successful when companies want to improve their innovation skills. The relationship between motivation and innovation could be studied in more depth, especially in workplace settings. There were almost no studies identified that tested interaction effects between the different approaches discussed here, with the exception of the relationship identified between trust and communication of marketing message. For example, a study that investigates both the use of humor and forced associations on creativity would yield interesting results.

The literature review shows that some commonly used innovation-related tools and advice that are ‘assumed’ to work by many methods and professionals would benefit from academic support in addition to anecdotal evidence. Such tools and advice include ‘mind maps’, ‘benefit promise’, ‘simplicity’, and ‘tell a story’. These tools/approaches seem reasonable and their usefulness obvious, but they would benefit from research on their limitations and best-practice implementations. The literature review also shows that some advice, like ‘work in groups’ and ‘dual brain’, has comparable amounts of supporting and refuting evidence. Though the concepts are simple, implementation is complex and worthy of in-depth research in order to establish clearer guidelines.

This paper serves as an approach that can be used to evaluate other similar training programs when experimental tests are impossible at the moment due to limitations of budget, time, or practicality. Evaluating the IE program by assessing its components individually examines the foundation of the program. It enables potential participants to the program to justify the use of the different tools for themselves. The evidence (both positive and negative) found for each individual tool helps the participants to select subsets of tools for more in-depth uses with more confidence. The identified refuting literature also lays out caveats and limitations for using those tools. However, this evaluation approach does not consider the training procedures, activities, or exercises in the IE program. These factors could have some effects on the participants’ learning outcomes. The evaluation approach here also does not consider the synthetic effects of the tools. Extrapolating this idea, the best test of IE’s effectiveness would be a longitudinal study that incorporates all tools and advice in a test-versus-control environment, with experts and novices, in industrial and academic settings, and then relates experimental outputs to financial gains. It is possible that metrics such as trial markets or Kickstarter (Kickstarter n.d.) donations could be used to test potential for financial gain. If such a study could be conducted and positive results found, it would be a great benefit to IE’s credibility, as it is a newly founded program. Until such tests are addressed, as well as the limitations and gaps in knowledge mentioned above, it is inaccurate to claim that IE is a science, as its website currently does. The literature review presented here suggests that IE offers some well-supported tools and advice for systematic product innovation, but also that IE must stay innovative itself and incorporate the most current research in order to deliver benefit to its customers.

Financial Support

This work was funded by the U.S. DoC/NIST Hollings Manufacturing Extension Partnership in Iowa, managed by the Center for Industrial Research and Service (www.ciras.iastate.edu).

References

Abraham, A., Pieritz, K., Thybusch, K., Rutter, B., Kroeger, S., Schweckendiek, J., Stark, R., Windmann, S. & Hermann, C. 2012 Creativity and the brain: uncovering the neural signature of conceptual expansion. Neuropsychologia 50 (8).Google Scholar
Agogué, M. & Kazakçi, A. 2013 10 years of C–K theory: a survey on the academic and industrial impacts of a design theory. In An Anthology of Theories and Models of Design: Philosophy, Approaches and Empirical Explorations (ed. Chakrabarti, A. & Blessing, L. T. M.), pp. 219235. Springer.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. E. 1973 Consumer dissatisfaction: effect of disconfirmed expectancy on perceived product performance. Journal of Marketing Research 10 (1), 3844.Google Scholar
Antony, J. & Banuelas Coronado, R. 2002 Design for Six Sigma. Manufacturing Engineer 81 (1), 2426.Google Scholar
Arlitt, R., Nix, A. & Stone, R.2012 Evaluating TRIZ as a provider of provocative stimuli. Proceedings of the ASME 2012 International Mechanical Engineering Congress and Exposition, Houston, Texas, USA.Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. D. & Ezell, S. J. 2012 Innovation Economics: The Race for Global Advantage. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Bass, F. M. 1969 A new product growth for model consumer durables. Management Science 15 (5), 215227.Google Scholar
Bass, F. M., Krishnan, T. V. & Jain, D. C. 1994 Why the bass model fits without decision variables. Marketing Science 13 (3), 203223.Google Scholar
Bearden, W. O. & Shimp, T. A. 1982 The use of extrinsic cues to facilitate product adoption. Journal of Marketing Research 19 (2), 229239.Google Scholar
Berawi, M. A.2011 A guide for idea generation and creativity using function analysis. Proceeding of the 51th SAVE Annual Conference, Oregon, USA.Google Scholar
Birdi, K., Leach, D. & Magadley, W. 2012 Evaluating the impact of TRIZ creativity training: an organizational field study. R & D Management 42 (4), 315326.Google Scholar
Boatwright, P. & Nunes, J. C. 2001 Reducing assortment: an attribute-based approach. Journal of Marketing 65 (3), 5063.Google Scholar
Boulding, W. & Kirmani, A. 1993 A consumer-side experimental examination of signaling theory: do consumers perceive warranties as signals of quality? Journal of Consumer Research 20 (1), 111123.Google Scholar
Brouwer, E. 1997 Into Innovation: Determinants and Indicators. University of Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Bryant, C. R., McAdams, D. A., Stone, R. B., Kurtoglu, T. & Campbell, M. I.2006 A validation study of an automated concept generator design tool. Proceedings of ASME 2006 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences & Computers and Information in Engineering Conference, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T. & Petty, R. E. 1979 Effects of message repetition and position on cognitive response, recall and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37 (1), 97109.Google Scholar
Cheng, Y. Y., Wang, W. C., Liu, K. S. & Chen, Y. L. 2010 Effects of association instruction on fourth graders’ poetic creativity in Taiwan. Creativity Research Journal 22 (2), 228235.Google Scholar
Chulvi, V., González-Cruz, M. C., Mulet, E. & Aguilar-Zambrano, J. 2013 Influence of the type of idea-generation method on the creativity of solutions. Research in Engineering Design 24 (1), 3341.Google Scholar
Coatanéa, E., Nonsiri, S., Christophe, F. & Mokammel, F.2014 Graph based representation and analyses for conceptual stages. Proceedings of the ASME 2014 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences & Computers and Information in Engineering Conference, Buffalo, New York, USA.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. J. 2012 Innovation and Economic Growth. Goldman Sachs.Google Scholar
Cooper, R. G., Edgett, S. J. & Kleinschmidt, E. J. 2002 Optimizing the stage-gate process: what best-practice companies do - I. Research-Technology Management 45 (5), 2127.Google Scholar
Cooper, R. G. & Kleinschmidt, E. J. 1993 Major new product: what distinguishes the winners in the chemical industry. Journal of Product Innovation Management 10 (2), 90111.Google Scholar
Corbitt, B. J., Thanasankit, T. & Yi, H. 2003 Trust and e-commerce: a study of consumer perceptions. Electronic Commerce Research and Applications 2, 203215.Google Scholar
Coskun, H. 2009 The effects of associative exercises on the idea generation during brainstorming. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi 24 (64), 3446.Google Scholar
Coskun, H. 2011 The effects of group size, memory instruction, and session length on the creative performance in electronic brainstorming groups. Kuram Ve Uygulamada Egitim Bilimleri 11 (1), 9195.Google Scholar
D’Souza, G. & Rao, R. C. 1995 Can repeating an advertisement more frequently than the competition affect brand preference in a mature market? Journal of Marketing 59 (2), 3242.Google Scholar
Daly, S. R., Christian, J. L., Yilmaz, S., Seifert, C. M. & Gonzalez, R. 2012 Assessing design heuristics for idea generation in an introductory engineering course. International Journal of Engineering Education 28 (2), 463473.Google Scholar
Davis, J. J. 1993 Strategies for environmental advertising. Journal of Consumer Marketing 10 (2), 1936.Google Scholar
Day, G. S. 2007 Is it real? Can we win? Is it worth doing? Managing risk and reward in an innovation portfolio. Harvard Business Review 85 (12), 110120.Google Scholar
De Koning, E. & Weiss, R. L. 2002 The relational humor inventory: functions of humor in close relationships. American Journal of Family Therapy 30 (1), 118.Google Scholar
Decker, R. & Gnibba-Yukawa, K. 2010 Sales forecasting in high-technology markets: a utility-based approach. Journal of Product Innovation Management 27 (1), 115129.Google Scholar
Deming, W. E. 1986 Out of the Crisis. MIT Center for Advanced Educational Services.Google Scholar
Diao, L., Zhang, B. & Jia, L. 2009 Diffusion of information technology: an extension of the bass model. In IEEC 2009: International Symposium on Information Engineering and Electronic Commerce, pp. 712716.Google Scholar
Diehl, M. & Stroebe, W. 1987 Productivity loss in brainstorming groups - toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53 (3), 497509.Google Scholar
East, R., Hammond, K. & Lomax, W. 2008 Measuring the impact of positive and negative word of mouth on brand purchase probability. International Journal of Research in Marketing 25 (3), 215224.Google Scholar
Edelman 2013 Edelman Trust Barometer.Google Scholar
Educational Testing Service 2013 General test percentage distribution of scores within intended broad graduate major field based on seniors and nonenrolled college graduates. Available from: http://www.ets.org/s/gre/pdf/gre_guide_table4.pdf [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Eskin, G. J. 1973 Dynamic forecasts of new product demand using a depth of repeat model. Journal of Marketing Research 10 (2), 115129.Google Scholar
Eureka! Ranch (n.d.) Available from http://www.eurekaranch.com/ [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Ferioli, M., Dekoninck, E., Culley, S., Roussel, B. & Renaud, J. 2010 Understanding the rapid evaluation of innovative ideas in the early stages of design. International Journal of Product Development 12 (1), 6783.Google Scholar
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., Reishofer, G., Hauswirth, V., Fally, M., Neuper, C., Ebner, F. & Neubauer, A. C. 2009 The creative brain: investigation of brain activity during creative problem solving by means of EEG and fMRI. Human Brain Mapping 30 (3), 734748.Google Scholar
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Gebauer, D., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K. & Ebner, F. 2010 Enhancing creativity by means of cognitive stimulation: evidence from an fMRI study. Neuroimage 52 (4), 16871695.Google Scholar
Fourt, L. A. & Woodlock, J. W. 1960 Early prediction of market success for new grocery products. Journal of Marketing 25 (2), 3138.Google Scholar
Freedman, J. L. 1965 Increasing creativity by free-association training. Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (1), 8991.Google Scholar
Frey, D. D., Herder, P. M., Wijnia, Y., Subrahmanian, E., Katsikopoulos, K. & Clausing, D. P. 2009 The Pugh controlled convergence method: model-based evaluation and implications for design theory. Research in Engineering Design 20 (1), 4158.Google Scholar
Fu, J.-R. & Chen, J. H. F. 2011 The impact of seller expertise and a refund guarantee on auction outcome: evidence from an online field experiment of camera lens market. International Journal of Electronic Business Management 9 (3), 187195.Google Scholar
Gavish, B., Horsky, D. & Srikanth, K. 1983 An approach to the optimal positioning of a new product. Management Science 29 (11), 12771297.Google Scholar
González-Cruz, M. C., Aguilar-Zambrano, J. A., Aguilar-Zambrano, J. J. & Colombel, M. G. 2008 TRIZ, the systematic creativity strategy used in multidisciplinary product design teams. DYNA 83 (6), 337350.Google Scholar
Graham, E. 1995 The involvement of sense of humor in the development of social relationships. Communication Reports 8 (2), 158169.Google Scholar
Green, M. C. & Brock, T. C. 2000 The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 (5), 701721.Google Scholar
Gupta, S. K. & Samuel, A. K.2001 Integrating the market research with the product development process: a step towards design for profit. ASME Design for Manufacturing Conference, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar
Hall, D. 1998 Making the Courage Connection. Touchstone.Google Scholar
Hall, D.(Foreward) 2005a Jump Start Your Business Brain: Scientific Ideas and Advice That Will Immediately Double Your Business Success Rate. Foreword by T. Peters Clerisy Press.Google Scholar
Hall, D. 2005b Jump Start Your Marketing Brain. Clerisy Press.Google Scholar
Hall, D. 2007 Jump Start Your Brain, 2nd edn. Clerisy Press.Google Scholar
Hall, D. 2009 North Pole Tenderfoot. Clerisy Press.Google Scholar
Hall, D.2013 Report to the National Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Hall, D. & Graduates of the Innovation Engineering Leadership Institute (n.d.) Author Doug Hall. Available from: https://innovationengineeringlabs.com/blog/about.php#aboutdoug [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Harkins, J. D. & Macrosson, W. D. K. 1990 Creativity training: an assessment of a novel approach. Journal of Business and Psychology 5 (1), 143148.Google Scholar
Hatchuel, A., Le Masson, P. & Weil, B.2004 C–K theory in practice: lessons from industrial applications. Proceedings of the 8th International Design Conference – Design 2004, Dubrovnik.Google Scholar
Hatchuel, A. & Weil, B.2003 A new approach of innovative design: an introduction to C–K theory. International Conference on Engineering Design ICED 03, Stockholm.Google Scholar
Hauser, J. R. & Clausing, D. 1988 The house of quality. Harvard Business Review 66 (3), 6373.Google Scholar
Hazelrigg, G. A. 1998 A framework for decision-based engineering design. Journal of Mechanical Design 120 (4), 653658.Google Scholar
Heath, C. & Heath, D. 2007 Made to Stick. Random House Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Hernandez, N. V., Schmidt, L. C. & Okudan, G. E. 2013 Systematic ideation effectiveness study on TRIZ. Journal of Mechanical Design 135 (10), 101009.Google Scholar
Herstatt, C., Verworn, B. & Nagahira, A. 2004 Reducing project related uncertainty in the ‘fuzzy front end’ of innovation: a comparison of German and Japanese product innovation projects. International Journal of Product Development 1 (1), 4365.Google Scholar
Hirtz, J., Stone, R. B., McAdams, D. A., Szykman, S. & Wood, K. L. 2002 A functional basis for engineering design: reconciling and evolving previous efforts. Research in Engineering Design 13 (2), 6582.Google Scholar
Holbrook, M. B. 1978 Beyond attitude structure: toward the informational determinants of attitude. Journal of Marketing Research 15 (4), 545556.Google Scholar
Honkala, S., Hämäläinen, M. & Salonen, M.2007 Comparison of four existing concept selection methods. ICED’07: International Conference on Engineering Design, Paris, France.Google Scholar
Howard, T. J., Culley, S. & Dekoninck, E. A. 2011 Reuse of ideas and concepts for creative stimuli in engineering design. Journal of Engineering Design 22 (8).Google Scholar
Howard-Jones, P. A., Blakemore, S. J., Samuel, E. A., Summers, I. R. & Claxton, G. 2005 Semantic divergence and creative story generation: an fMRI investigation. Cognitive Brain Research 25 (1), 240250.Google Scholar
Humke, C. & Schaefer, C. E. 1996 Sense of humor and creativity. Perceptual and Motor Skills 82 (2), 544546.Google Scholar
Innovation Engineering (n.d.) Available from: http://www.innovationengineering.org/ [24 June 2015].Google Scholar
Iyengar, S. S. & Lepper, M. R. 2000 When choice is demotivating: can one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 (6), 9951006.Google Scholar
Jain, D., Mahajan, V. & Muller, E. 1995 An approach for determining optimal product sampling for the diffusion of a new product. Journal of Product Innovation Management 12 (2), 124135.Google Scholar
Jarvenpaa, S. L., Tractinsky, N. & Vitale, M. 2000 Consumer trust in an Internet store. Information Technology and Management 1 (1–2), 4571.Google Scholar
Kelley, T. & Kelley, D. 2013 Creative Confidence: Unleashing the Creative Potential within Us All, 1st edn. Crown Business.Google Scholar
Kelly, J. C., Maheut, P., Petiot, J. & Papalambros, P. 2011 Incorporating user shape preference in engineering design optimisation. Journal of Engineering Design 22 (9), 627650.Google Scholar
Kickstarter (n.d.) Kickstarter Basics: Kickstarter 101. Available from: http://www.kickstarter.com/help/faq/kickstarter%20basics?ref=nav [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Kleinknecht, A.(Ed.) 1996 Determinants of Innovation: The Message from New Indicators. Macmillan Press Ltd.Google Scholar
Klomp, L. & Leeuwen, G. H.v. 1999 The importance of innovation for company performance. Netherlands Official Statistics 14 (26–35).Google Scholar
Kokotovich, V. 2008 Problem analysis and thinking tools: an empirical study of non-hierarchical mind mapping. Design Studies 29 (1).Google Scholar
Kowatari, Y., Lee, S. H., Yamamura, H., Nagamori, Y., Levy, P., Yamane, S. & Yamamoto, M. 2009 Neural networks involved in artistic creativity. Human Brain Mapping 30 (5).Google Scholar
Krishnan, A.2009 What are academic disciplines? Some observations on the disciplinarity vs interdisciplinarity debate. University of Southampton, ESRC National Centre for Research Methods, NCRM Working Paper Series.Google Scholar
Kumar, D., Hoyle, C., Chen, W., Wang, N., Gomez-Levi, G. & Koppelman, F. S. 2009 A hierarchical choice modelling approach for incorporating customer preferences in vehicle package design. International Journal of Product Development 8 (3), 228251.Google Scholar
Larey, T. S. & Paulus, P. B. 1999 Group preference and convergent tendencies in small groups: a content analysis of group brainstorming performance. Creativity Research Journal 12 (3), 175184.Google Scholar
Lau, G. T. & Lee, S. H. 1999 Consumers’ trust in a brand and the link to brand loyalty. Journal of Market Focused Management 4, 341370.Google Scholar
Leonard, D. & Rayport, J. F. 1997 Spark innovation through empathic design. Harvard Business Review 75 (6), 102113.Google Scholar
León-Rovira, N., Heredia-Escorza, Y. & Río, L. M. L.-D. 2008 Systematic creativity, challenge-based instruction and active learning: a study of its impact on freshman engineering students. International Journal of Engineering Education 24 (6).Google Scholar
Li, H. & Azarm, S. 2000 Product design selection under uncertainty and with competitive advantage. Journal of Mechanical Design 122 (4), 411418.Google Scholar
Li, M. & Yang, J. 2006 Application of bass model to seamless technology diffusion and its new finding. In Proceedings of the Eighth West Lake International Conference on Small and Medium Business, pp. 179184.Google Scholar
Liberali, G., Urban, G. L. & Hauser, J. R. 2013 Competitive information, trust, brand consideration and sales: two field experiments. International Journal of Research in Marketing 30 (2), 101113.Google Scholar
Linderman, K., Schroeder, R. G., Zaheer, S. & Choo, A. S. 2003 Six Sigma: a goal-theoretic perspective. Journal of Operations Management 21 (2), 193203.Google Scholar
Linsey, J. S., Tseng, I., Fu, K., Cagan, J., Wood, K. L. & Schunn, C. 2010 A study of design fixation, its mitigation and perception in engineering design faculty. Journal of Mechanical Design 132 (4), 041003.Google Scholar
MacDonald, E., Lubensky, A., Sohns, B. & Papalambros, P. Y. 2009 Product semantics and wine portfolio optimisation. International Journal of Product Development 7 (1/2), 7398.Google Scholar
Macklin, M. C., Bruvold, N. T. & Shea, C. L. 1985 Is it always as simple as ‘keep it simple’? Journal of Advertising 14 (4), 2835.Google Scholar
Maddux, J. E. & Rogers, R. W. 1980 Effects of source expertness, physical attractiveness, and supporting arguments on persuasion: a case of brains over beauty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39 (2), 235244.Google Scholar
Mader, D. P. 2002 Design for Six Sigma. Quality Progress 35 (7), 8286.Google Scholar
Maeda, J. 2006 The Laws of Simplicity. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Magretta, J. 1998 The power of virtual integration: an interview with Dell computer’s Michael Dell. Harvard Business Review 76 (2), 7284.Google Scholar
Mann, D. 2002 Assessing the accuracy of the contradiction matrix for recent mechanical inventions. The TRIZ Journal.Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E. 1983 Can you repeat that? qualitative effects of repetition and advance organizers on learning from science prose. Journal of Educational Psychology 75 (1), 4049.Google Scholar
McGlynn, R. P., McGurk, D., Effland, V. S., Johll, N. L. & Harding, D. J. 2004 Brainstorming and task performance in groups constrained by evidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 93 (1).Google Scholar
Mednick, S. A. 1962 The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review 69 (3).Google Scholar
Mehrjerdi, Y. Z. 2011 Six-Sigma: methodology, tools and its future. Assembly Automation 31 (1), 7988.Google Scholar
Merriam-Webster.com 2014a Association. Available from: http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/association [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Merriam-Webster.com 2014b Innovation. Available from: http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/innovation [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Merriam-Webster.com 2014c Stimulus. Available from: http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/stimulus [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Merriam-Webster.com 2015a Invention. Available from: http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/invention [24 September 2015].Google Scholar
Merriam-Webster.com 2015b Science. Available from: http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/science [24 June 2015].Google Scholar
Michalek, J. J., Feinberg, F. M. & Papalambros, P. Y. 2005 Linking marketing and engineering product design decisions via analytical target cascading. Journal of Product Innovation Management 22 (1), 4262.Google Scholar
Mihov, K. M., Denzler, M. & Förster, J. 2010 Hemispheric specialization and creative thinking: a meta-analytic review of lateralization of creativity. Brain and Cognition 72 (3), 442448.Google Scholar
Miller, B. J., Russ, D., Gibson, C. & Hall, A. E. 1970 Effects of free association training, retraining, and information on creativity. Journal of Experimental Psychology 84 (2), 226229.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. 1994 The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 101 (2), 343352.Google Scholar
Mullen, B., Johnson, C. & Salas, E. 1991 Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: a meta-analytic integration. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 12 (1), 323.Google Scholar
Nakanishi, M. 1973 Advertising and promotion effects on consumer response to new products. Journal of Marketing Research 10 (3), 242249.Google Scholar
Ocker, R. J. 2005 Influences on creativity in asynchronous virtual teams: a qualitative analysis of experimental teams. IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication 48 (1), 2239.Google Scholar
Ogot, M. & Okudan, G. E. 2006 Integrating systematic creativity into first-year engineering design curriculum. International Journal of Engineering Education 22 (1).Google Scholar
Oppewal, H. & Koelemeijer, K. 2005 More choice is better: effects of assortment size and composition on assortment evaluation. International Journal of Research in Marketing 22 (1), 4560.Google Scholar
Papalambros, P. Y. & Wilde, D. J. 2000 Principles of Optimal Design: Modeling and Computation. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Parfitt, J. H. & Collins, B. J. K. 1968 Use of consumer panels for brand-share prediction. Journal of Marketing Research 5 (2), 131145.Google Scholar
Paulus, P. B. & Yang, H. C. 2000 Idea generation in groups: a basis for creativity in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 82 (1), 7687.Google Scholar
Pennington, N. & Hastie, R. 1988 Explanation-based decision-making: effects of memory structure on judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 14 (3), 521533.Google Scholar
Pirola-Merlo, A. & Mann, L. 2004 The relationship between individual creativity and team creativity: aggregating across people and time. Journal of Organizational Behavior 25 (2), 235257.Google Scholar
Pohekar, S. D. & Ramachandran, M. 2006 Multi-criteria evaluation of cooking devices with special reference to utility of parabolic solar cooker (PSC) in India. Energy 31 (8–9), 12151227.Google Scholar
Ray, M. L. & Sawyer, A. G. 1971 Repetition in media models: laboratory technique. Journal of Marketing Research 8 (1), 2029.Google Scholar
Redelmeier, D. A. & Shafir, E. 1995 Medical decision-making in situations that offer multiple alternatives. The Journal of the American Medical Association 273 (4), 302305.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, N. 2004 Innovation and Economic Growth. Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development.Google Scholar
Rossiter, J. R. & Percy, L. 1978 Visual imaging ability as a mediator of advertising response. Advances in Consumer Research 5, 621629.Google Scholar
Rouff, L. L. 1975 Creativity and sense of humor. Psychological Reports 37 (3), 1022.Google Scholar
Roy, R. 1993 Case studies of creativity in innovative product development. Design Studies 14 (4), 423443.Google Scholar
Satoh, D. 2001 A discrete bass model and its parameter estimation. Journal of the Operations Research Society of Japan 44 (1), 118.Google Scholar
Settle, R. B. & Golden, L. L. 1974 Attribution theory and advertiser credibility. Journal of Marketing Research 11 (2), 181185.Google Scholar
Shedler, J. & Manis, M. 1986 Can the availability heuristic explain vividness effects? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51 (1), 2636.Google Scholar
Sheu, D. D., Chen, C. H. & Yu, P. Y. 2012 Invention principles and contradiction matrix for semiconductor manufacturing industry: chemical mechanical polishing. Journal of Intelligent Manufacturing 23 (5), 16371648.Google Scholar
Stone, R. B. & Wood, K. L. 2000 Development of a functional basis for design. Journal of Mechanical Design 122, 359370.Google Scholar
Taggar, S. 2002 Individual creativity and group ability to utilize individual creative resources: a multilevel model. Academy of Management Journal 45 (2), 315330.Google Scholar
The University of Maine 2015 Foster Center for Student Innovation. Available from: http://foster.target.maine.edu/ [8 May 2015].Google Scholar
ThinkBuzan (n.d.) Ethos and Philosophy. Available from: http://www.thinkbuzan.com/us/company/about [12 December 2014].Google Scholar
Thorelli, H. B., Lim, J.-S. & Ye, J. 1989 Relative importance of country of origin, warranty, and retail store image on product evaluations. International Marketing Review 6 (1), 3546.Google Scholar
Thornburg, T. H. 1991 Group-size and member diversity influence on creative performance. Journal of Creative Behavior 25 (4), 324333.Google Scholar
Train, K. 1986 Qualitative Choice Analysis: Theory, Econometrics, and An Application to Automobile Demand. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Train, K. 2009 Discrete Choice Methods with Simulation, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Treadwell, Y. 1970 Humor and creativity. Psychological Reports 26, 5558.Google Scholar
Trifts, V. & Haubl, G. 2003 Information availability and consumer preference: can online retailers benefit from providing access to competitor price information? Journal of Consumer Psychology 13 (1–2), 149159.Google Scholar
TRIZ40 (n.d.) Available from: http://www.triz40.com/ [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Tseng, F.-M. & Hu, Y.-C. 2009 Quadratic-interval Bass model for new product sales diffusion. Expert Systems with Applications 36 (4), 84968502.Google Scholar
Ullman, D. 2009 The Mechanical Design Process, 4th edn. McGraw–Hill Series in Mechanical Engineering, McGraw–Hill Higher Education.Google Scholar
Unalan, H. T.2007 The effectiveness of the mind-maps in the courses of art education in the primary schools. IMSCI ’07: International Multi-Conference on Society, Cybernetics and Informatics.Google Scholar
Understanding Science (n.d.) What is science?Available from: http://undsci.berkeley.edu/article/whatisscience_01 [24 June 2015].Google Scholar
Valacich, J. S., Jung, J. H. & Looney, C. A. 2006 The effects of individual cognitive ability and idea stimulation on idea-generation performance. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice 10 (1), 115.Google Scholar
Wallas, G. 1926 The Art of Thought. Harcourt, Brace and Company, Inc.Google Scholar
Wang, M. Y., Chang, D. S. & Kao, C. H. 2010 Identifying technology trends for R&D planning using TRIZ and text mining. R & D Management 40 (5), 491509.Google Scholar
Wassenaar, H. J. & Chen, W. 2003 An approach to decision-based design with discrete choice analysis for demand modeling. Journal of Mechanical Design 125 (3), 490497.Google Scholar
Wassenaar, H. J., Chen, W., Cheng, J. & Sudjianto, A. 2005 Enhancing discrete choice demand modeling for decision-based design. Journal of Mechanical Design 127 (4), 514523.Google Scholar
Wessner, C. W.(Ed.) 2013 21st Century Manufacturing: The Role of the Manufacturing Extension Partnership Program, the National Academies Press.Google Scholar
WhiteHouse (n.d.) Innovation. Available from: http://www.whitehouse.gov/issues/economy/innovation [4 December 2014].Google Scholar
Wong, P. K., Ho, Y. P. & Autio, E. 2005 Entrepreneurship, innovation and economic growth: evidence from GEM data. Small Business Economics 24, 335350.Google Scholar
Yates, J. F. 2003 Decision Management: How to Assure Better Decisions in Your Company, 1st edn. Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Zhang, T., Gensler, S. & Garcia, R. 2011 A study of the diffusion of alternative fuel vehicles: an agent-based modeling approach. Journal of Product Innovation Management 28 (2), 152168.Google Scholar
Ziv, A. 1976 Facilitating effects of humor on creativity. Journal of Educational Psychology 68 (3), 318322.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. A summary of example sets of keywords used in the online literature search.

Figure 1

Figure 2. An example mind map generated with the concepts from the IE Create category.

Figure 2

Table 1. Literature related to the IE Create category. (Letters accompanying the references in the table refer to the setting in which the experiment was conducted. ‘c’ represents a classroom setting; ‘w’ a workplace; ‘l’ a lab or other controlled settings; and ‘o’ other—such as mail surveys. References not accompanied by letters do not include scientific experimental conditions.)

Figure 3

Table 2. Literature related to the IE Communicate category

Figure 4

Table 3. Literature related to the IE Commercialize category