Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-55tpx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-14T21:13:19.496Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does Day Length Affect Cognitive Performance in Memory Clinic Patients?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2014

Catherine Lacny
Affiliation:
College of Medicine, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada
Andrew Kirk*
Affiliation:
Division of Neurology, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada
Debra G. Morgan
Affiliation:
Canadian Centre for Health and Safety in Agriculture, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada
Chandima Karunanayake
Affiliation:
Canadian Centre for Health and Safety in Agriculture, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada
*
Division of Neurology, University of Saskatchewan, 103 Hospital Drive, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, S7N 0W8, Canada
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.
Objective:

To determine whether day length affects cognitive performance in rural and remote memory clinic patients.

Methods:

A rural and remote memory clinic in Saskatoon, Saskatchewan provided an opportunity to examine how cognitive performance on the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) is influenced by day length. Saskatchewan is an ideal location to test this association as day length varies greatly both seasonally and geographically. Following an initial assessment by the Rural and Remote Memory Clinic (RRMC) team in Saskatoon, patient follow-up appointments were performed either in-person or via telehealth videoconference. At each follow-up appointment the clinic neurologist administered the MMSE. The relationship between day length and MMSE scores at the sixweek follow-up appointment was analyzed in 154 patients. The mean daily temperature was controlled for in the analysis. Bivariate correlate and linear regression analyses were conducted.

Results:

There was no significant association between MMSE scores and either minutes of day length or mean daily temperature.

Conclusion:

Day length does not appear to have significant effect on cognitive performance of rural and remote memory clinic patients.

Résumé:

Résumé:Objectif:

Le but de l’étude était de déterminer si la longueur du jour influence la performance cognitive chez des patients vivant en milieu rural ou éloigné.

Méthode:

Nous avons profité du contexte d’une clinique de la mémoire située à Saskatoon en Saskatchewan pour examiner si la longueur du jour influence la performance cognitive au Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). La Saskatchewan est un lieu idéal pour évaluer cette association étant donné que la longueur du jour varie énormément selon les saisons et la géographie. Les patients ont subi une évaluation initiale à Saskatoon par l’équipe d’une Clinique de la mémoire en milieu rural et éloigné. Le suivi était fait en personne ou par vidéoconférence en télésanté. Le neurologue de la clinique les évaluait au moyen du MMSE à chaque rendez-vous de suivi. Nous avons analysé la relation entre la longueur du jour et les scores au MMSE à l’examen de suivi 6 semaines après l’évaluation initiale chez 154 patients. Les données ont été soumises à des analyses de corrélation et de régression linéaire bivariées, tenant compte de la température moyenne quotidienne.

Résultats:

Nous n’avons pas observé d’association significative entre les scores au MMSE et la longueur du jour en minutes ou la température quotidienne moyenne.

Conclusion:

La longueur du jour ne semble par avoir un effet significatif sur la performance cognitive des patients d’une clinique de la mémoire située en milieu rural ou éloigné.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Canadian Journal of Neurological 2011

References

1 Dumont, M, Beaulieu, C. Light exposure in the natural environment: relevance to mood and sleep disorders. Sleep Med. 2007;8: 557–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Kent, ST, McMclures, LA, Crosson, WL, Arnett, DK, Wadley, VG, Sathiakumar, N. Effect of sunlight exposure on cognitive function among depressed and non-depressed participants: a REGARDS cross-sectional study. Environ Health. 2009;8(34).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Sullivan, B, Payne, TW. A comparison of seasonal and nonseasonal depression. Am J Psychiat. 2007;164:1663–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Bachman, D, Rabins, P. “Sundowning” and other temporally associated agitation states in dementia patients. Annu Rev Med. 2006;57:499511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Volicer, L, Harper, DG, Manning, BC, Goldstein, R, Satlin, A. Sundowning and circadian rhythms in Alzheimer’s Disease. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:704–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Forbes, D, Culum, I, Lischka, AR, et al. Light therapy for managing cognitive, sleep, functional, behavioural, or psychiatric disturbances in dementia. Cochrane DB Syst Rev. 2009;4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7 Sloane, PD, Williams, CS, Mitchell, CM, et al. High-intensity environmental light in dementia: effect on sleep and activity. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55:1524–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Morgan, DG, Crossley, M, Kirk, A, et al. Improving access to dementia care: development and evaluation of a rural and remote memory clinic. Aging Ment Health. 2009;13(1):1730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Folstein, MF, Folstein, SE, McHugh, PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;Nov12(3):189–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10 McEachern, W, Kirk, A, Morgan, DG, Crossley, M, Henry, C. Reliability of the MMSE administered in-person and by telehealth. Can J Neuro Sci. 2008;35:643–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Morgan, DG, Crossley, M, Kirk, A, et al. Evaluation of telehealth for preclinic assessment and follow-up in an interprofessional rural and remote memory clinic. J App Gerontol Epub 2010 Apr7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar